Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Prevention Science 3/2010

01.09.2010

Can Adolescents Learn Self-control? Delay of Gratification in the Development of Control over Risk Taking

verfasst von: Daniel Romer, Angela L. Duckworth, Sharon Sznitman, Sunhee Park

Erschienen in: Prevention Science | Ausgabe 3/2010

Einloggen, um Zugang zu erhalten

Abstract

Recent findings from developmental neuroscience suggest that the adolescent brain is too immature to exert control over impulsive drives, such as sensation seeking, that increase during adolescence. Using a discounting of delayed reward paradigm, this research examines the ability to delay gratification as a potential source of control over risk-taking tendencies that increase during adolescence. In addition, it explores the role of experience resulting from risk taking as well as future time perspective as contributors to the development of this ability. In a nationally representative sample (n = 900) of young people aged 14–22, a structural equation analysis shows that risk taking as assessed by use of three popular drugs (tobacco, marijuana, and alcohol) is inversely related to the ability to delay gratification. The relation is robust across gender, age, and different levels of sensation seeking. In addition, high sensation seekers exhibit dramatic age-related increase in delay of gratification, lending support to the hypothesis that engaging in risky behavior provides experience that leads to greater patience for long-term rewards. The findings support the conclusion that a complete understanding of the development of self-control must consider individual differences not easily explained by universal trends in brain maturation.
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 463–496.CrossRefPubMed Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 463–496.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Allison, P. D. (2001). Missing data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Allison, P. D. (2001). Missing data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Zurück zum Zitat Bembenutty, H. F., & Karabenick, S. A. (2004). Inherent association between academic delay of gratification, future time perspective, and self-regulated learning. Educational Psychology Review, 16, 35–57.CrossRef Bembenutty, H. F., & Karabenick, S. A. (2004). Inherent association between academic delay of gratification, future time perspective, and self-regulated learning. Educational Psychology Review, 16, 35–57.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bentler, P. M. (2004). EQS 6 structural equation manual. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software, Inc. Bentler, P. M. (2004). EQS 6 structural equation manual. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software, Inc.
Zurück zum Zitat Botvin, G. J., Griffin, K. W., & Nichols, T. D. (2006). Preventing youth violence and delinquency through a universal school-based prevention approach. Prevention Science, 7, 403–408.CrossRefPubMed Botvin, G. J., Griffin, K. W., & Nichols, T. D. (2006). Preventing youth violence and delinquency through a universal school-based prevention approach. Prevention Science, 7, 403–408.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Boyd, J. N., Keough, K. A., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1999). Who’s smoking, drinking, and using drugs? Time perspective as a predictor of substance use. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21, 149–164. Boyd, J. N., Keough, K. A., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1999). Who’s smoking, drinking, and using drugs? Time perspective as a predictor of substance use. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21, 149–164.
Zurück zum Zitat Casey, B. J., Getz, S., & Galvan, A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Developmental Review, 28, 62–77.PubMed Casey, B. J., Getz, S., & Galvan, A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Developmental Review, 28, 62–77.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Chambers, R. A., Potenza, M. N., & Taylor, J. R. (2003). Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: A critical period of addiction vulnerability. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1041–1052.CrossRefPubMed Chambers, R. A., Potenza, M. N., & Taylor, J. R. (2003). Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: A critical period of addiction vulnerability. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1041–1052.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Donohew, L., Hoyle, R. H., Lorch, E. P., Palmgreen, P., & Stephenson, M. T. (2002). Reliability and validity of a brief measure of sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 401–414.CrossRef Donohew, L., Hoyle, R. H., Lorch, E. P., Palmgreen, P., & Stephenson, M. T. (2002). Reliability and validity of a brief measure of sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 401–414.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Psychological Science, 16, 939–944.CrossRefPubMed Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Psychological Science, 16, 939–944.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Evans, G. W., & Rosenbaum, J. (2008). Self-regulation and the income-achievement gap. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23, 504–514.CrossRef Evans, G. W., & Rosenbaum, J. (2008). Self-regulation and the income-achievement gap. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23, 504–514.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fong, G. T., & Hall, P. A. (2003). Time perspective: A potentially important construct for decreasing health risk behaviors among adolescents. In D. Romer (Ed.), Reducing adolescent risk: Toward an integrated approach (pp. 106–112). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Fong, G. T., & Hall, P. A. (2003). Time perspective: A potentially important construct for decreasing health risk behaviors among adolescents. In D. Romer (Ed.), Reducing adolescent risk: Toward an integrated approach (pp. 106–112). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Zurück zum Zitat Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 769–792.CrossRefPubMed Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 769–792.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Green, L., Fry, A. F., & Myerson, J. (1994). Discounting of delayed rewards: A life-span comparison. Psychological Science, 5, 33–36.CrossRef Green, L., Fry, A. F., & Myerson, J. (1994). Discounting of delayed rewards: A life-span comparison. Psychological Science, 5, 33–36.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Green, L., Myerson, J., Lichtman, D., Rosen, S., & Fry, A. (2008). Temporal discounting in choice between delayed rewards: The role of age and income. Psychology and Aging, 11, 79–84.CrossRef Green, L., Myerson, J., Lichtman, D., Rosen, S., & Fry, A. (2008). Temporal discounting in choice between delayed rewards: The role of age and income. Psychology and Aging, 11, 79–84.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hinton, J. M., Jameson, T. L., & Whitney, P. (2003). Impulsive decision-making and working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 298–306.CrossRef Hinton, J. M., Jameson, T. L., & Whitney, P. (2003). Impulsive decision-making and working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 298–306.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications (pp. 76–99). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications (pp. 76–99). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Zurück zum Zitat Jentsch, D., & Taylor, J. R. (1999). Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: Implications for the control of behavior by rewardrelated stimuli. Psychopharmacology, 146, 373–390.CrossRefPubMed Jentsch, D., & Taylor, J. R. (1999). Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: Implications for the control of behavior by rewardrelated stimuli. Psychopharmacology, 146, 373–390.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Johnson, M. W., & Bickel, W. (2002). Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 77, 129–146.CrossRefPubMed Johnson, M. W., & Bickel, W. (2002). Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 77, 129–146.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2008). Monitoring the future national survey results on drug use, 1975–2007: Vol. 1. Secondary school students (NIH Publication N. 08-64-18A). Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse. Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2008). Monitoring the future national survey results on drug use, 1975–2007: Vol. 1. Secondary school students (NIH Publication N. 08-64-18A). Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse.
Zurück zum Zitat Joireman, J., Sprott, D. E., & Spangenberg, E. R. (2005). Fiscal responsibility and the consideration of future consequences. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 1159–1168.CrossRef Joireman, J., Sprott, D. E., & Spangenberg, E. R. (2005). Fiscal responsibility and the consideration of future consequences. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 1159–1168.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kaplan, D. (2000). Structural equation modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Kaplan, D. (2000). Structural equation modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Zurück zum Zitat Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlstrom, K., et al. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD—A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 177–186.CrossRefPubMed Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlstrom, K., et al. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD—A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 177–186.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Lewin, K. (1939). Field theory and experiment in social psychology: Concepts and methods. American Journal of Sociology, 44, 868–896.CrossRef Lewin, K. (1939). Field theory and experiment in social psychology: Concepts and methods. American Journal of Sociology, 44, 868–896.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat McCartt, A. T., Shabanova, V. I., & Leaf, W. A. (2003). Driving experience, crashes and traffic citations of teenage beginning drivers. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 35, 311–320.CrossRefPubMed McCartt, A. T., Shabanova, V. I., & Leaf, W. A. (2003). Driving experience, crashes and traffic citations of teenage beginning drivers. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 35, 311–320.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Mischel, W., Peake, P. K., & Shoda, Y. (1988). The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 687–696.CrossRefPubMed Mischel, W., Peake, P. K., & Shoda, Y. (1988). The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 687–696.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938.CrossRefPubMed Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100, 674–701.CrossRefPubMed Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100, 674–701.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Morrissey, M. A., Grabowski, D. C., Dee, T. S., & Campbell, C. (2006). The strength of graduated drivers license programs and fatalities among teens and passengers. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 38, 135–141.CrossRef Morrissey, M. A., Grabowski, D. C., Dee, T. S., & Campbell, C. (2006). The strength of graduated drivers license programs and fatalities among teens and passengers. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 38, 135–141.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nelson, C. A., Bloom, F. E., Cameron, J. L., Amaral, D., Dahl, R. E., & Pine, D. (2002). An integrative, multidisciplinary approach to the study of brain-behavior relations in the context of typical and atypical development. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 499–520.CrossRefPubMed Nelson, C. A., Bloom, F. E., Cameron, J. L., Amaral, D., Dahl, R. E., & Pine, D. (2002). An integrative, multidisciplinary approach to the study of brain-behavior relations in the context of typical and atypical development. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 499–520.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Nurmi, J. (1991). How do adolescents see the future? A review of the development of future orientation and planning. Developmental Review, 11, 1–59.CrossRef Nurmi, J. (1991). How do adolescents see the future? A review of the development of future orientation and planning. Developmental Review, 11, 1–59.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press. Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Pattij, T., & Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J. (2008). The neuropharmacology of impulsive behavior. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences, 29, 192–199.CrossRefPubMed Pattij, T., & Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J. (2008). The neuropharmacology of impulsive behavior. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences, 29, 192–199.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Rachlin, H. (2000). The science of self-control. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Rachlin, H. (2000). The science of self-control. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Raine, A., Reynolds, C., Venables, P. H., & Mednick, S. A. (2002). Stimulation seeking and intelligence: A prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 663–674.CrossRefPubMed Raine, A., Reynolds, C., Venables, P. H., & Mednick, S. A. (2002). Stimulation seeking and intelligence: A prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 663–674.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Raykov, T. (1997). Estimation of composite reliability for congeneric measures. Applied Psychological Measurement, 21, 173–184.CrossRef Raykov, T. (1997). Estimation of composite reliability for congeneric measures. Applied Psychological Measurement, 21, 173–184.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Reynolds, B. (2004). Do high rates of cigarette consumption increase delay discounting? A cross-sectional comparison of adolescent smokers and young-adult smokers and nonsmokers. Behavioural Processes, 67, 545–549.CrossRefPubMed Reynolds, B. (2004). Do high rates of cigarette consumption increase delay discounting? A cross-sectional comparison of adolescent smokers and young-adult smokers and nonsmokers. Behavioural Processes, 67, 545–549.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Reynolds, B. (2006). A review of delay-discounting research with humans: Relations to drug use and gambling. Behavioural Pharmacology, 17, 651–667.CrossRefPubMed Reynolds, B. (2006). A review of delay-discounting research with humans: Relations to drug use and gambling. Behavioural Pharmacology, 17, 651–667.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Reynolds, B., & Schiffbauer, R. (2005). Delay of gratification and delay discounting: A unifying feedback model of delay-related impulsive behavior. Psychological Record, 55, 439–460. Reynolds, B., & Schiffbauer, R. (2005). Delay of gratification and delay discounting: A unifying feedback model of delay-related impulsive behavior. Psychological Record, 55, 439–460.
Zurück zum Zitat Reynolds, B., Penfold, R. B., & Patak, M. (2008). Dimensions of impulsive behavior in adolescents: Laboratory behavioral assessments. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16, 124–131.CrossRefPubMed Reynolds, B., Penfold, R. B., & Patak, M. (2008). Dimensions of impulsive behavior in adolescents: Laboratory behavioral assessments. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16, 124–131.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Riggs, N. R., Greenberg, M. T., Kusche, C. A., & Pentz, M. A. (2006). The meditational role of neurogcognition in the behavioral outcomes of a social-emotional prevention program in elementary school students: Effects of the PATHS curriculum. Prevention Science, 7, 91–102.CrossRefPubMed Riggs, N. R., Greenberg, M. T., Kusche, C. A., & Pentz, M. A. (2006). The meditational role of neurogcognition in the behavioral outcomes of a social-emotional prevention program in elementary school students: Effects of the PATHS curriculum. Prevention Science, 7, 91–102.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Roberti, J. W. (2004). A review of behavioral and biological correlates of sensation seeking. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 256–279.CrossRef Roberti, J. W. (2004). A review of behavioral and biological correlates of sensation seeking. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 256–279.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Romer, D., & Hennessy, M. (2007). A biosocial-affect model of adolescent sensation seeking: The role of affect evaluation and peer-group influence in adolescent drug use. Prevention Science, 8, 89–101.CrossRefPubMed Romer, D., & Hennessy, M. (2007). A biosocial-affect model of adolescent sensation seeking: The role of affect evaluation and peer-group influence in adolescent drug use. Prevention Science, 8, 89–101.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccamanno, L., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 14931–14936.CrossRef Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccamanno, L., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 14931–14936.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (1994). Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis. In A. von Eye, & C. C. Clogg (Eds.), Latent variables analysis: Applications for developmental research (pp. 399–419). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (1994). Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis. In A. von Eye, & C. C. Clogg (Eds.), Latent variables analysis: Applications for developmental research (pp. 399–419). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Zurück zum Zitat Shamosh, N. A., DeYoung, C. G., Green, A. E., Reis, D. L., Johnson, M. R., Conway, A. R. A., et al. (2008). Individual differences in delay discounting: Relation to intelligence, working memory, and anterior prefrontal cortex. Psychological Science, 19, 904–911.CrossRefPubMed Shamosh, N. A., DeYoung, C. G., Green, A. E., Reis, D. L., Johnson, M. R., Conway, A. R. A., et al. (2008). Individual differences in delay discounting: Relation to intelligence, working memory, and anterior prefrontal cortex. Psychological Science, 19, 904–911.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Shedler, J., & Block, J. (1990). Adolescent drug use and psychological health: A longitudinal inquiry. American Psychologist, 45, 612–630.CrossRefPubMed Shedler, J., & Block, J. (1990). Adolescent drug use and psychological health: A longitudinal inquiry. American Psychologist, 45, 612–630.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Smith, G. T., Fischer, S., Cyders, M. A., Annus, A. M., Spillane, N. S., & McCarthy, D. M. (2007). On the validity and utility of discriminating among impulsivity-like traits. Assessment, 14, 155–170.CrossRefPubMed Smith, G. T., Fischer, S., Cyders, M. A., Annus, A. M., Spillane, N. S., & McCarthy, D. M. (2007). On the validity and utility of discriminating among impulsivity-like traits. Assessment, 14, 155–170.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Spanagel, R., & Weiss, F. (1999). The dopamine hypothesis of reward: Past and current status. Trends in Neuroscience, 22, 521–527.CrossRef Spanagel, R., & Weiss, F. (1999). The dopamine hypothesis of reward: Past and current status. Trends in Neuroscience, 22, 521–527.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Spear, L. P. (2000). Neurobehavioral changes in adolescence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 111–114.CrossRef Spear, L. P. (2000). Neurobehavioral changes in adolescence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 111–114.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Spear, L. P. (2007). The developing brain and adolescent-typical behavior patterns: An evolutionary approach. In D. Romer & E. Walker (Eds.), Adolescent psychopathology and the developing brain: Integrating brain and prevention science (pp. 9–30). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Spear, L. P. (2007). The developing brain and adolescent-typical behavior patterns: An evolutionary approach. In D. Romer & E. Walker (Eds.), Adolescent psychopathology and the developing brain: Integrating brain and prevention science (pp. 9–30). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk taking. Developmental Review, 28, 78–106.CrossRefPubMed Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk taking. Developmental Review, 28, 78–106.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O’Brien, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E., & Banich, M. (2009). Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development, 80, 28–44.CrossRefPubMed Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O’Brien, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E., & Banich, M. (2009). Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development, 80, 28–44.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, Office of Applied Studies (2008). Results from the 2007 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: National Findings (NSDUH Series H-34, DHHS Publication No. SMA 08-4343). Rockville, MD. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, Office of Applied Studies (2008). Results from the 2007 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: National Findings (NSDUH Series H-34, DHHS Publication No. SMA 08-4343). Rockville, MD.
Zurück zum Zitat Turner, M. G., & Piquero, A. R. (2002). The stability of self-control. Journal of Criminal Justice, 30, 457–471.CrossRef Turner, M. G., & Piquero, A. R. (2002). The stability of self-control. Journal of Criminal Justice, 30, 457–471.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat U.S. Bureau of the Census. (2001). Estimates of the population of the United States, by age, sex, and race: 1990 to 1999. Washington, DC: U.S. Bureau of the Census. U.S. Bureau of the Census. (2001). Estimates of the population of the United States, by age, sex, and race: 1990 to 1999. Washington, DC: U.S. Bureau of the Census.
Zurück zum Zitat Verdejo-Garcia, A., Lawrence, A. J., & Clark, L. (2008). Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: Review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers, and genetic association studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 777–810.CrossRefPubMed Verdejo-Garcia, A., Lawrence, A. J., & Clark, L. (2008). Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: Review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers, and genetic association studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 777–810.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Wallace, J. M., Bachman, J. G., O’Malley, P. M., Schulenberg, J. E., Cooper, S. M., & Johnston, L. D. (2003). Gender and ethnic differences in smoking, drinking, and illicit drug use among American 8th, 10th and 12th grand students, 1976–2000. Addiction, 98, 225–234.CrossRefPubMed Wallace, J. M., Bachman, J. G., O’Malley, P. M., Schulenberg, J. E., Cooper, S. M., & Johnston, L. D. (2003). Gender and ethnic differences in smoking, drinking, and illicit drug use among American 8th, 10th and 12th grand students, 1976–2000. Addiction, 98, 225–234.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 669–689.CrossRef Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 669–689.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (2006). Are juvenile offenders extreme future discounters? Psychological Science, 17(11), 989–994.CrossRefPubMed Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (2006). Are juvenile offenders extreme future discounters? Psychological Science, 17(11), 989–994.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Winfree, L. T., Taylor, T. J., He, N., & Esbensen, F.-A. (2006). Self-control and variability over time: Multivariate results using a 5-year multisite panel of youths. Crime & Delinquency, 52, 253–286.CrossRef Winfree, L. T., Taylor, T. J., He, N., & Esbensen, F.-A. (2006). Self-control and variability over time: Multivariate results using a 5-year multisite panel of youths. Crime & Delinquency, 52, 253–286.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zald, D. H., Cowan, R. L., Riccardi, P., Baldwin, R. M., Ansari, M. S., Li, R., et al. (2008). Midbrain dopamine receptor availability is inversely associated with novelty-seeking traits in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 14372–14378.CrossRefPubMed Zald, D. H., Cowan, R. L., Riccardi, P., Baldwin, R. M., Ansari, M. S., Li, R., et al. (2008). Midbrain dopamine receptor availability is inversely associated with novelty-seeking traits in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 14372–14378.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1271–1288.CrossRef Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1271–1288.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zuckerman, M. (1994). Biobehavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. New York: Cambridge University Press. Zuckerman, M. (1994). Biobehavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Metadaten
Titel
Can Adolescents Learn Self-control? Delay of Gratification in the Development of Control over Risk Taking
verfasst von
Daniel Romer
Angela L. Duckworth
Sharon Sznitman
Sunhee Park
Publikationsdatum
01.09.2010
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Prevention Science / Ausgabe 3/2010
Print ISSN: 1389-4986
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6695
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-010-0171-8

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 3/2010

Prevention Science 3/2010 Zur Ausgabe