Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Neuropsychology Review 2/2010

01.06.2010

Disconnexion Syndromes in Animals and Man: Part I

verfasst von: Norman Geschwind

Erschienen in: Neuropsychology Review | Ausgabe 2/2010

Einloggen, um Zugang zu erhalten

Excerpt

The early successes of the views of Broca and Wernicke led the classical neurologists to a mode of analysis of the disturbances of the higher neurological functions subsequently to be labelled with the derisive term “diagram-making.” Starting from the picture of the brain as a collection of sets of more or less specialized groups of cells connected by relatively discrete fibre pathways, these classical neurologists deduced a series of symptom complexes. On the basis of this model clinical syndromes could be divided into those resulting from lesions of grey matter and those which resulted from lesions of the white matter interconnecting specialized regions. Thus, cortical syndromes were distinguished from “conduction” syndromes. Basically it was this mode of analysis which dominated the literature until the First World War. …
Fußnoten
1
It will perhaps strike the reader as curious that Goldstein should have embraced so classical an approach. I have discussed more fully elsewhere Goldstein’s position in the history of aphasia (Geschwind 1964a) but can only comment here that he was in fact much more of a localizationist than is generally appreciated. His theoretical writings with their criticisms of classical ideas contain so many qualifications that they are often compatible with even the most extreme localizationist views. The reader who goes carefully through the “Special Part” of Goldstein’s 1927 monograph will repeatedly find in it an active defence of many classical ideas; indeed, many of Goldstein’s disagreements with other authors are primarily on details of localization. His later book on language (Goldstein 1948) continues to show his acceptance of many classical ideas, especially in his discussion of particular syndromes.
 
2
In the monkey there appears to be a bundle which is homologous to this structure in the rabbit and which perforates the splenium of the corpus callosum. There is, however, some question as to whether the bundle even in the rabbit does indeed rise in the visual cortex; for the monkey the site of origin of the corresponding tract is unknown, and it is certainly possible that it arises from retrosplenial cortex or from areas 18 or 19 on the medial surface. It is conceivable that this bundle may run directly from the visual cortex to the hippocampus in the rabbit while in higher forms these connexions must be made by way of association cortex. The analysis by Pribram and MacLean (1953) of the connexions of the mediobasal cortex of the monkey presents ample evidence for indirect pathways which could lead along the medial surface of the hemisphere from visual cortex to visual association cortex, hippocampal gyrus and hippocampus.
 
3
Petr et al. use the term “fusiform gyrus” for areas TF and even TH in the macaque. Bonin and Bailey (1947) note the great similarities of TF and TH. Papez (1929) uses the term “fusiform-hippocampal gyrus” and “pyriform area” for these two regions. It is likely that the fusiform gyrus in the human sense is not present in the monkey and that these two areas are probably most reasonably considered as hippocampal gyrus (now called parahippocampal gyrus by some authors). Whitlock and Nauta (1956) in reporting the results of Petr et al. substitute the term “hippocampal gyrus” for “fusiform gyrus” and we will follow their usage.
 
4
I use this term advisedly rather than “loss of visual discriminations.” The subsequent theoretical discussion will make the reason for this choice of words clear.
 
5
These elementary results have an important bearing on the question of cross-modal associations. Recent studies, e.g. those of Ettlinger and different co-workers have clearly demonstrated difficulties of tactile-visual or visual-auditory transfers in monkeys (Burton and Ettlinger 1960; Ettlinger 1961). The conclusion should, however, not be drawn that monkeys form no cross-modal associations. It is abundantly clear, in fact, that the majority of classical learning experiments with most organisms do in fact demonstrate formation of cross-modal associations, as long as the modality to which the association is made is a “limbic” modality, i.e. a reinforcer, such as food, water, etc. I will return to the question of the difficulty of transfer between “non-limbic” modalities at a later point.
 
6
The authors state that they ablated “areas 18 and 19,” but it is possible from their diagram that part of the association cortex posterior to the lunate sulcus was preserved. Bonin and Bailey (1947) place OB behind the lunate sulcus and Crosby et al. (1962) show area 18 as behind the lunate sulcus. It is not unlikely that some of area 18 was spared in the Ades and Raab experiments.
 
7
Since the writing of this section it has been called to the author’s attention that other experimenters have had results different from those of Downer. It will be important to ascertain the reasons for these discrepancies.
 
8
Meyer and Yates (1955) showed that patients with left temporal lobe lesions are likely to have verbal recall difficulties. Milner (1962) has confirmed their findings, showing that left anterior temporal lobectomy has a more profound effect on verbal memory than similar right temporal lobectomies. It is quite likely that the reason for this is that left temporal lobectomy cuts of connexions between the posterior speech area and the limbic system and thus leads to verbal learning deficit.
 
9
This mechanism for incomprehension of spelled words appears to Dr. Howes and myself to be more simply and more clearly based physiologically than the classical explanation, which simply invokes a new disturbance, “word-sound deafness,” to account for incomprehension of spelled words. By any standard the term “word-sound deafness” is a poor one. “Letter-name deafness” would have been closer to being a correct description. “Inability to understand words spelled orally” is the best descriptive term.
 
10
I am indebted to Sir Charles Symonds for having called Vialet’s monograph to my attention. It was in fact his paper (Symonds 1953) which alerted me to this interesting syndrome. I am also grateful to him for having read and criticized an earlier paper of mine on this topic.
 
11
It may be objected that the alexia in a half-field from a right parietal lesion is the result of “neglect” of that field. While I do not wish to discuss this problem extensively here, I would like to point out that what I am attempting to show is that one mechanism of “neglect” of a normal left visual field is disconnexion of the normal right occipital cortex from the speech area.
 
12
In an illiterate society a lack of visual-auditory associations would not seriously inconvenience anyone except in unusual situations; literacy makes this ability highly important. Other cross-modal association deficits may exist but might never be detected because they cause so little disturbance. It is conceivable that direct visual-tactile associations may be as badly developed in many humans as they appear to be in monkeys (Ettlinger 1960) but only specific testing will bring this out. It is important, of course, to study children as early as possible in the course of development.
 
13
These authors also found another group of parietal areas which responded to click with only slightly longer latency than the primary auditory region. They presented evidence that the response in these areas depended on collaterals from the medial geniculate body. These areas would not in my terms be “association” areas. I will not discuss their possible function here.
 
14
It should be added that the second temporal gyrus of man appears to be a phylo-genetically very late region of whose connexions we know very little. It may be a region of great importance and it is conceivable that the view of Wernicke’s area presented above is too narrow. I would, however, disagree with those authors who include in Wernicke’s area all the posterior regions involved in speech in both the temporal and parietal lobes.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Ades, H. W. (1959). In Handbook of physiology (Section 1, Vol. I, p. 585). Washington: American Physiological Society. Ades, H. W. (1959). In Handbook of physiology (Section 1, Vol. I, p. 585). Washington: American Physiological Society.
Zurück zum Zitat Ades, H. W., & Raab, D. H. (1949). J. Neurophysiol., 12, 101.PubMed Ades, H. W., & Raab, D. H. (1949). J. Neurophysiol., 12, 101.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Akelaitis, A. J. (1941a). Amer. J. Psychiat., 97, 1147. Akelaitis, A. J. (1941a). Amer. J. Psychiat., 97, 1147.
Zurück zum Zitat Akelaitis, A. J. (1941b). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 45, 788. Akelaitis, A. J. (1941b). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 45, 788.
Zurück zum Zitat Akelaitis, A. J. (1941c). Amer. J. Psychiat., 98, 409. Akelaitis, A. J. (1941c). Amer. J. Psychiat., 98, 409.
Zurück zum Zitat Akelaitis, A. J. (1942a). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 48, 108. Akelaitis, A. J. (1942a). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 48, 108.
Zurück zum Zitat Akelaitis, A. J. (1942b). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 48, 914. Akelaitis, A. J. (1942b). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 48, 914.
Zurück zum Zitat Akelaitis, A. J. (1945). Amer. J. Psychiat., 101, 594. Akelaitis, A. J. (1945). Amer. J. Psychiat., 101, 594.
Zurück zum Zitat Akelaitis, A. J., Risteen, W. A., Herren, R. Y., & Van Wagenen, W. P. (1942). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 7, 971. Akelaitis, A. J., Risteen, W. A., Herren, R. Y., & Van Wagenen, W. P. (1942). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 7, 971.
Zurück zum Zitat Bailey, P., & Bonin, G. v. (1951). The isocortex of man. Urbana. Bailey, P., & Bonin, G. v. (1951). The isocortex of man. Urbana.
Zurück zum Zitat Bailey, P., Bonin, Gv, Garol, H. W., & McCulloch, W. S. (1943a). J. Neurophysiol., 6, 121. Bailey, P., Bonin, Gv, Garol, H. W., & McCulloch, W. S. (1943a). J. Neurophysiol., 6, 121.
Zurück zum Zitat Bailey, P., Bonin, Gv, Garol, H. W., & McCulloch, W. S. (1943b). J. Neurophysiol., 6, 129. Bailey, P., Bonin, Gv, Garol, H. W., & McCulloch, W. S. (1943b). J. Neurophysiol., 6, 129.
Zurück zum Zitat Bailey, P., Bonin, G. v., & McCulloch, W. S. (1950). The isocortex of the chimpanzee. Urbana. Bailey, P., Bonin, G. v., & McCulloch, W. S. (1950). The isocortex of the chimpanzee. Urbana.
Zurück zum Zitat Bastian, H. C. (1898). A treatise on aphasia and other speech defects. London. Bastian, H. C. (1898). A treatise on aphasia and other speech defects. London.
Zurück zum Zitat Bates, J. A. V., & Ettlinger, G. (1960). Arch. Neurol., 3, 177.PubMed Bates, J. A. V., & Ettlinger, G. (1960). Arch. Neurol., 3, 177.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Biemond, A. (1956). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 75, 231. Biemond, A. (1956). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 75, 231.
Zurück zum Zitat Birch, H. G. (1962). In J. Money (Ed.), Reading disability (p. 161). Baltimore. Birch, H. G. (1962). In J. Money (Ed.), Reading disability (p. 161). Baltimore.
Zurück zum Zitat Blum, J. S. (1951). Comp. Psychol. Monog., 20, 219. Blum, J. S. (1951). Comp. Psychol. Monog., 20, 219.
Zurück zum Zitat Bonin, G. v. (1962). In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance (p. 1). Baltimore. Bonin, G. v. (1962). In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance (p. 1). Baltimore.
Zurück zum Zitat Bonin, G. v., & Bailey, P. (1947). The neocortex of Macaca Mulatta. Urbana. Bonin, G. v., & Bailey, P. (1947). The neocortex of Macaca Mulatta. Urbana.
Zurück zum Zitat Bonin, G. v., & Bailey, P. (1961). In H. Hofer, A. H. Schultz, & D. Starck (Eds.), Primatologia (Vol. II/2, Lieferung 10). Bonin, G. v., & Bailey, P. (1961). In H. Hofer, A. H. Schultz, & D. Starck (Eds.), Primatologia (Vol. II/2, Lieferung 10).
Zurück zum Zitat Chow, K. L. (1961). In Brain mechanisms and learning (p. 507). Oxford: Council for International Organizations of Medical Sciences. Chow, K. L. (1961). In Brain mechanisms and learning (p. 507). Oxford: Council for International Organizations of Medical Sciences.
Zurück zum Zitat Connolly, C. J. (1950). External morphology of the primate brain. Springfield. Connolly, C. J. (1950). External morphology of the primate brain. Springfield.
Zurück zum Zitat Critchley, M. (1953). The paietal lobes. London. Critchley, M. (1953). The paietal lobes. London.
Zurück zum Zitat Crosby, E. C., Humphrey, T., & Lauer, E. W. (1962). Correlative anatomy of the nervous system. New York. Crosby, E. C., Humphrey, T., & Lauer, E. W. (1962). Correlative anatomy of the nervous system. New York.
Zurück zum Zitat Curtis, H. J. (1940). J. Neurophysiol., 3, 407. Curtis, H. J. (1940). J. Neurophysiol., 3, 407.
Zurück zum Zitat Dejerine, J. (1891). Mim. Soc. Biol., 3, 191. Dejerine, J. (1891). Mim. Soc. Biol., 3, 191.
Zurück zum Zitat Dejerine, J. (1892). Mim. Soc. Biol., 4, 61. Dejerine, J. (1892). Mim. Soc. Biol., 4, 61.
Zurück zum Zitat Denny-Brown, D. (1962). In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Inerhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance (p. 244). Baltimore. Denny-Brown, D. (1962). In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Inerhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance (p. 244). Baltimore.
Zurück zum Zitat Downer, J. L. de C. (1959). Brain, 82, 251. Downer, J. L. de C. (1959). Brain, 82, 251.
Zurück zum Zitat Downer, J. L. de C. (1962). In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance (p. 87). Baltimore. Downer, J. L. de C. (1962). In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance (p. 87). Baltimore.
Zurück zum Zitat Egger, M. D., & Flynn, J. P. (1963). J. Neurophysiol., 26, 705.PubMed Egger, M. D., & Flynn, J. P. (1963). J. Neurophysiol., 26, 705.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Ettlinger, G. (1962). In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance (p. 75). Baltimore. Ettlinger, G. (1962). In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance (p. 75). Baltimore.
Zurück zum Zitat Ettlinger, G., & Kalsbeck, J. E. (1961). J. Physiol., 156, 34 pages. Ettlinger, G., & Kalsbeck, J. E. (1961). J. Physiol., 156, 34 pages.
Zurück zum Zitat Foix, C. (1922). Rev. Neurol., 38, 322. Foix, C. (1922). Rev. Neurol., 38, 322.
Zurück zum Zitat Foix, C., & Hillemand, P. (1925). Bull Mém. Soc. Méd. Hôp. Paris, 49, 393. Foix, C., & Hillemand, P. (1925). Bull Mém. Soc. Méd. Hôp. Paris, 49, 393.
Zurück zum Zitat Geschwind, N. (1962). In J. Money (Ed.), Reading disability (p. 115). Baltimore. Geschwind, N. (1962). In J. Money (Ed.), Reading disability (p. 115). Baltimore.
Zurück zum Zitat Geschwind, N. (1964a). Cortex, 1, 214. Geschwind, N. (1964a). Cortex, 1, 214.
Zurück zum Zitat Geschwind, N. (1964b). In C. I. J. M. Stuart (Ed.), Monograph series on language and linguistics (No. 17, p. 155). Georgetown University. Geschwind, N. (1964b). In C. I. J. M. Stuart (Ed.), Monograph series on language and linguistics (No. 17, p. 155). Georgetown University.
Zurück zum Zitat Geschwind, N., & Fusillo, M. (1964). Trans. Amer. Neurol. Assoc., 89, 172. Geschwind, N., & Fusillo, M. (1964). Trans. Amer. Neurol. Assoc., 89, 172.
Zurück zum Zitat Geschwind, N., & Kaplan, E. (1962a). New Engl J. Med., 266, 1013. Geschwind, N., & Kaplan, E. (1962a). New Engl J. Med., 266, 1013.
Zurück zum Zitat Glickstein, M., Arora, H. A., & Sperry, R. W. (1963). J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 56, 11.CrossRef Glickstein, M., Arora, H. A., & Sperry, R. W. (1963). J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 56, 11.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gloor, P. (1960). In Handbook of physiology (Section 1, Vol. 2, p. 1395). Washington: American Physiological Society. Gloor, P. (1960). In Handbook of physiology (Section 1, Vol. 2, p. 1395). Washington: American Physiological Society.
Zurück zum Zitat Goldstein, K. (1908). J. Psychol. Neurol., Lpz., 11, pp. 169 and 270. Goldstein, K. (1908). J. Psychol. Neurol., Lpz., 11, pp. 169 and 270.
Zurück zum Zitat Goldstein, K. (1927). In A. Bethe, G. v. Bergmann, G. Embden, & A. Ellinger (Eds.), Handbuch der Normalen und Pathologischen Physiologie (Vol. 10, p. 600). Berlin. Goldstein, K. (1927). In A. Bethe, G. v. Bergmann, G. Embden, & A. Ellinger (Eds.), Handbuch der Normalen und Pathologischen Physiologie (Vol. 10, p. 600). Berlin.
Zurück zum Zitat Goldstein, K. (1948). Language and language disturbances. New York. Goldstein, K. (1948). Language and language disturbances. New York.
Zurück zum Zitat Hartmann, F. (1907). Mschr. Psychiat. Neurol., 21, pp. 97 and 248. Hartmann, F. (1907). Mschr. Psychiat. Neurol., 21, pp. 97 and 248.
Zurück zum Zitat Head, H. (1926). Aphasia and kindred disorders of speech. Cambridge. Head, H. (1926). Aphasia and kindred disorders of speech. Cambridge.
Zurück zum Zitat Hécaen, H., de Ajuriaguerra, J., & David, M. (1952). Mschr. Psychiat. Neurol., 123, 239.CrossRef Hécaen, H., de Ajuriaguerra, J., & David, M. (1952). Mschr. Psychiat. Neurol., 123, 239.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hilgard, E. R., & Marquis, D. G. (1961). Conditioning and learning (2nd Ed.). G. A. Kimble (Ed.). New York. Hilgard, E. R., & Marquis, D. G. (1961). Conditioning and learning (2nd Ed.). G. A. Kimble (Ed.). New York.
Zurück zum Zitat Hoff, H. (1961). In Reports at the VTJ International Congress of Neurology. Rome; also in Excerpta Medica, International Congress Series, No. 38, p. 1. Hoff, H. (1961). In Reports at the VTJ International Congress of Neurology. Rome; also in Excerpta Medica, International Congress Series, No. 38, p. 1.
Zurück zum Zitat Howes, D. H. (1962). In J. Money (Ed.), Reading disability (p. 131). Baltimore. Howes, D. H. (1962). In J. Money (Ed.), Reading disability (p. 131). Baltimore.
Zurück zum Zitat Jansen, J., & Brodal, A. (1954). Aspects of cerebellar anatomy. Oslo. Jansen, J., & Brodal, A. (1954). Aspects of cerebellar anatomy. Oslo.
Zurück zum Zitat Jasper, H., Ajmone-Marsan, C., & Stoll, J. (1952). Arch. Neurol Psychiat., Chicago, 67, 155. Jasper, H., Ajmone-Marsan, C., & Stoll, J. (1952). Arch. Neurol Psychiat., Chicago, 67, 155.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaada, B. R. (1960). In Handbook of physiology (Section 1, Vol. 2, p. 1345). Washington: American Physiological Society. Kaada, B. R. (1960). In Handbook of physiology (Section 1, Vol. 2, p. 1345). Washington: American Physiological Society.
Zurück zum Zitat Klüver, H., & Bucy, P. (1938). J. Psychol., 5, 33. Klüver, H., & Bucy, P. (1938). J. Psychol., 5, 33.
Zurück zum Zitat Konorski, J. (1961). In Brain mechanisms and learning (p. 115). Oxford: Council for International Organizations of Medical Sciences. Konorski, J. (1961). In Brain mechanisms and learning (p. 115). Oxford: Council for International Organizations of Medical Sciences.
Zurück zum Zitat Krieg, W. J. S. (1963). Connections of the cerebral cortex. Evanston. Krieg, W. J. S. (1963). Connections of the cerebral cortex. Evanston.
Zurück zum Zitat Lange, J. (1936). In O. Bumke & O. Foerster (Eds.), Handbuch der Neurologic (Vol. 6, p. 807). Berlin. Lange, J. (1936). In O. Bumke & O. Foerster (Eds.), Handbuch der Neurologic (Vol. 6, p. 807). Berlin.
Zurück zum Zitat Liepmann, H. (1898). Ein Fall von reiner Sprachtaubheit. Breslau. Liepmann, H. (1898). Ein Fall von reiner Sprachtaubheit. Breslau.
Zurück zum Zitat Liepmann, H. (1900). Das Krankheitsbild der Apraxie (‘motoischen Asymbolie*). Berlin. Liepmann, H. (1900). Das Krankheitsbild der Apraxie (‘motoischen Asymbolie*). Berlin.
Zurück zum Zitat Liepmann, H. (1906). Der weitere Krankheitsverlauf bei dem einseitig Apraktischen und der Gehirnbefund auf Grund von Seienschnitten. Berlin. Liepmann, H. (1906). Der weitere Krankheitsverlauf bei dem einseitig Apraktischen und der Gehirnbefund auf Grund von Seienschnitten. Berlin.
Zurück zum Zitat Liepmann, H., & Maas, O. (1907). J. Psychol. Neurol., 10, 214. Liepmann, H., & Maas, O. (1907). J. Psychol. Neurol., 10, 214.
Zurück zum Zitat Liepmann, H., & Storch, E. (1902). Mschr. Psychiat. Neurol., 11, 115.CrossRef Liepmann, H., & Storch, E. (1902). Mschr. Psychiat. Neurol., 11, 115.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat McCulloch, W. S., & Garol, H. W. (1941). J. Neurophysiol., 4, 555. McCulloch, W. S., & Garol, H. W. (1941). J. Neurophysiol., 4, 555.
Zurück zum Zitat MacLean, P. D. (1960). In Handbook of physiology (Section 1, Vol III, p. 1723). Washington: American Physiological Society. MacLean, P. D. (1960). In Handbook of physiology (Section 1, Vol III, p. 1723). Washington: American Physiological Society.
Zurück zum Zitat Maspes, P. E. (1948). Rev. Neurol., 80, 100. Maspes, P. E. (1948). Rev. Neurol., 80, 100.
Zurück zum Zitat Meyer, A. (1905). Psychol Bull, 2, 261. Reprinted in “The collected papers of Adolf Meyer” (Vol. I, p. 334). Baltimore, 1950. Meyer, A. (1905). Psychol Bull, 2, 261. Reprinted in “The collected papers of Adolf Meyer” (Vol. I, p. 334). Baltimore, 1950.
Zurück zum Zitat Meyer, D. R. (1958). In Biological and biochemical bases of behavior. Madison. Meyer, D. R. (1958). In Biological and biochemical bases of behavior. Madison.
Zurück zum Zitat Meyer, V., & Yates, H. J. (1955). J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat., 18, 44.CrossRef Meyer, V., & Yates, H. J. (1955). J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat., 18, 44.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Milner, B. (1962). In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Interhemispheic relations and cerebral dominance (p. 177). Baltimore. Milner, B. (1962). In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Interhemispheic relations and cerebral dominance (p. 177). Baltimore.
Zurück zum Zitat Myers, R. E. (1962b). In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed), Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance (p. 51). Baltimore. Myers, R. E. (1962b). In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed), Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance (p. 51). Baltimore.
Zurück zum Zitat Myers, R. E., & Sperry, R. W. (1953). Anat. Rec., 115, 351. Myers, R. E., & Sperry, R. W. (1953). Anat. Rec., 115, 351.
Zurück zum Zitat Papez, J. W. (1929). Comparative neurology. New York. Papez, J. W. (1929). Comparative neurology. New York.
Zurück zum Zitat Poblete, R., Ruben, R. J., & Walker, A. E. (1959). J. Neurophysiol., 22, 538.PubMed Poblete, R., Ruben, R. J., & Walker, A. E. (1959). J. Neurophysiol., 22, 538.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Pribram, K. H. (1961). In A. Simon, C. C. Herbert & R. Straus (Eds.), The physiology of emotions (p. 173). Springfield. Pribram, K. H. (1961). In A. Simon, C. C. Herbert & R. Straus (Eds.), The physiology of emotions (p. 173). Springfield.
Zurück zum Zitat Pribram, K. H. (1962). In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance (p. 107). Baltimore. Pribram, K. H. (1962). In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance (p. 107). Baltimore.
Zurück zum Zitat Pribram, H. B., & Barry, J. (1956). J. Neurophysiol., 19, 99.PubMed Pribram, H. B., & Barry, J. (1956). J. Neurophysiol., 19, 99.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Pribram, K. H., & MacLean, P. D. (1953). J. Neurophysiol., 16, 324.PubMed Pribram, K. H., & MacLean, P. D. (1953). J. Neurophysiol., 16, 324.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Pribram, K. H., Lennox, M. A., & Dunsmore, R. H. (1950). J. Neurophysiol., 13, 127. Pribram, K. H., Lennox, M. A., & Dunsmore, R. H. (1950). J. Neurophysiol., 13, 127.
Zurück zum Zitat Pribram, K. H., Rosner, B. S., & Rosenblith, W. A. (1954). J. Neurophysiol., 17, 336.PubMed Pribram, K. H., Rosner, B. S., & Rosenblith, W. A. (1954). J. Neurophysiol., 17, 336.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Raymond, F., & Egger, M. (1906). Rev. Neurol., 14, 371. Raymond, F., & Egger, M. (1906). Rev. Neurol., 14, 371.
Zurück zum Zitat Rubins, J. L., & Friedman, E. D. (1948). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 60, 554. Rubins, J. L., & Friedman, E. D. (1948). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 60, 554.
Zurück zum Zitat Schilder, P., & Stengel, E. (1928). Z. ges. Neurol. Psychiat., 113, 143.CrossRef Schilder, P., & Stengel, E. (1928). Z. ges. Neurol. Psychiat., 113, 143.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schilder, P., & Stengel, E. (1931). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 25, 598. Schilder, P., & Stengel, E. (1931). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 25, 598.
Zurück zum Zitat Segundo, J. P., Naquet, R., & Arana, R. (1955). Arch. Neurol Psychiat., Chicago, 73, 515. Segundo, J. P., Naquet, R., & Arana, R. (1955). Arch. Neurol Psychiat., Chicago, 73, 515.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith, G. E. (1907). J. Anat., Lond., 41, 237. Smith, G. E. (1907). J. Anat., Lond., 41, 237.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith, K. U., & Akelaitis, A. J. (1942). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 47, 519. Smith, K. U., & Akelaitis, A. J. (1942). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 47, 519.
Zurück zum Zitat Stepien, L. S., Cordeau, J. P., & Rasmussen, T. (1960). Brain, 83, 470.CrossRef Stepien, L. S., Cordeau, J. P., & Rasmussen, T. (1960). Brain, 83, 470.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sugar, O., French, J. D., & Chusid, J. G. (1948). J. Neurophysiol., 11, 175.PubMed Sugar, O., French, J. D., & Chusid, J. G. (1948). J. Neurophysiol., 11, 175.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Sugar, O., Amador, L. V., & Griponissiotis, B. (1950). J. Neurophysiol., 13, 229.PubMed Sugar, O., Amador, L. V., & Griponissiotis, B. (1950). J. Neurophysiol., 13, 229.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Sweet, W. H. (1959). In Handbook of physiology (Section 1, Vol. I, p. 459). Washington: American Physiological Society. Sweet, W. H. (1959). In Handbook of physiology (Section 1, Vol. I, p. 459). Washington: American Physiological Society.
Zurück zum Zitat Trescher, J. H., & Ford, F. R. (1937). Arch. Neurol Psychiat., Chicago, 37, 959. Trescher, J. H., & Ford, F. R. (1937). Arch. Neurol Psychiat., Chicago, 37, 959.
Zurück zum Zitat Vialet, N. (1893). Les Centres Cérébraux de la Vision. Paris. Vialet, N. (1893). Les Centres Cérébraux de la Vision. Paris.
Zurück zum Zitat Weinstein, E. A., Kahn, R. L., & Slote, W. H. (1955). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 74, 235. Weinstein, E. A., Kahn, R. L., & Slote, W. H. (1955). Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., Chicago, 74, 235.
Zurück zum Zitat Wernicke, C. (1874). Der aphasische Symptomencomplex. Breslau. Wernicke, C. (1874). Der aphasische Symptomencomplex. Breslau.
Metadaten
Titel
Disconnexion Syndromes in Animals and Man: Part I
verfasst von
Norman Geschwind
Publikationsdatum
01.06.2010
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Neuropsychology Review / Ausgabe 2/2010
Print ISSN: 1040-7308
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-6660
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-010-9131-0

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2010

Neuropsychology Review 2/2010 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Schwindelursache: Massagepistole lässt Otholiten tanzen

14.05.2024 Benigner Lagerungsschwindel Nachrichten

Wenn jüngere Menschen über ständig rezidivierenden Lagerungsschwindel klagen, könnte eine Massagepistole der Auslöser sein. In JAMA Otolaryngology warnt ein Team vor der Anwendung hochpotenter Geräte im Bereich des Nackens.

Schützt Olivenöl vor dem Tod durch Demenz?

10.05.2024 Morbus Alzheimer Nachrichten

Konsumieren Menschen täglich 7 Gramm Olivenöl, ist ihr Risiko, an einer Demenz zu sterben, um mehr als ein Viertel reduziert – und dies weitgehend unabhängig von ihrer sonstigen Ernährung. Dafür sprechen Auswertungen zweier großer US-Studien.

Bluttest erkennt Parkinson schon zehn Jahre vor der Diagnose

10.05.2024 Parkinson-Krankheit Nachrichten

Ein Bluttest kann abnorm aggregiertes Alpha-Synuclein bei einigen Menschen schon zehn Jahre vor Beginn der motorischen Parkinsonsymptome nachweisen. Mit einem solchen Test lassen sich möglicherweise Prodromalstadien erfassen und die Betroffenen früher behandeln.

Darf man die Behandlung eines Neonazis ablehnen?

08.05.2024 Gesellschaft Nachrichten

In einer Leseranfrage in der Zeitschrift Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology möchte ein anonymer Dermatologe bzw. eine anonyme Dermatologin wissen, ob er oder sie einen Patienten behandeln muss, der eine rassistische Tätowierung trägt.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.