Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics 1/2012

Open Access 01.12.2012 | Study protocol

Improving outcomes of preschool language delay in the community: protocol for the Language for Learning randomised controlled trial

verfasst von: Melissa Wake, Penny Levickis, Sherryn Tobin, Naomi Zens, James Law, Lisa Gold, Obioha C Ukoumunne, Sharon Goldfeld, Ha ND Le, Jemma Skeat, Sheena Reilly

Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics | Ausgabe 1/2012

Abstract

Background

Early language delay is a high-prevalence condition of concern to parents and professionals. It may result in lifelong deficits not only in language function, but also in social, emotional/behavioural, academic and economic well-being. Such delays can lead to considerable costs to the individual, the family and to society more widely. The Language for Learning trial tests a population-based intervention in 4 year olds with measured language delay, to determine (1) if it improves language and associated outcomes at ages 5 and 6 years and (2) its cost-effectiveness for families and the health care system.

Methods/Design

A large-scale randomised trial of a year-long intervention targeting preschoolers with language delay, nested within a well-documented, prospective, population-based cohort of 1464 children in Melbourne, Australia. All children received a 1.25-1.5 hour formal language assessment at their 4th birthday. The 200 children with expressive and/or receptive language scores more than 1.25 standard deviations below the mean were randomised into intervention or ‘usual care’ control arms. The 20-session intervention program comprises 18 one-hour home-based therapeutic sessions in three 6-week blocks, an outcome assessment, and a final feed-back/forward planning session. The therapy utilises a ‘step up-step down’ therapeutic approach depending on the child’s language profile, severity and progress, with standardised, manualised activities covering the four language development domains of: vocabulary and grammar; narrative skills; comprehension monitoring; and phonological awareness/pre-literacy skills. Blinded follow-up assessments at ages 5 and 6 years measure the primary outcome of receptive and expressive language, and secondary outcomes of vocabulary, narrative, and phonological skills.

Discussion

A key strength of this robust study is the implementation of a therapeutic framework that provides a standardised yet tailored approach for each child, with a focus on specific language domains known to be associated with later language and literacy. The trial responds to identified evidence gaps, has outcomes of direct relevance to families and the community, includes a well-developed economic analysis, and has the potential to improve long-term consequences of early language delay within a public health framework.

Trial registration

Current Controlled Trials ISRCTN03981121.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1471-2431-12-96) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

All authors declare that they and their spouses, partners or children have no financial and non-financial relationships or interests that may be relevant to the submitted work. The authors declare they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MW conceived the Language for Learning trial with JL, LG, UO, SG, SR and JS; she takes overall responsibility for all aspects of the trial and this manuscript. ST was the Project Manager, assisted by PL and NZ. NZ and JL designed the intervention, with advice from JS and SR, who also advised on measures and their interpretation. OU advised on statistical issues, LG and HL on the economic evaluation, and SG on the translational aspects of the trial. All authors contributed, read and approved the final manuscript.
Abkürzungen
AEDI
Australian early development index
A&SQ
Ages & stages questionnaire
CCC
2: Children communication checklist, 2nd edition
CNREP
Children's test of non-word repetition
CELF-P2
Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals- preschool edition 2
CI
Confidence interval
CTOPP
Comprehensive test of phonological processing
dB HL
Decibels hearing loss
ELVS
Early language in victoria study
GSV
Growth scale value
HUI
Health utilities index
IQ
Intelligence quotient
KBIT-2
Kaufman brief intelligence test second edition
LGAs
Local government areas
M&CH
Maternal & child health
NHMRC
National health and medical research council
NCEs
Normal curve equivalents
PPVT-4
Peabody picture vocabulary test
PedsQL
Pediatric quality of life inventory
QALYs
Quality-adjusted life years
RCT
Randomised controlled trial
RACV
Royal automobile club of victoria
SLI
Specific language impairment
SD
Standard deviations
SDQ
Strengths and difficulties questionnaire
SPAT-R
Sutherland phonological awareness test – revised: modified
WRAT
Wide range achievement test.

Background

Importance of language delay

Children who have delayed language development as they move into school are at risk of a raft of difficulties. Impacts of poor oral language skills go well beyond early literacy development and ‘school readiness’ to increasingly apparent associations with emotional, behavioural and social difficulties [14]. Perhaps most worrying is the emerging evidence of very long-term sequelae that are not restricted solely to the school years or to children with serious clinical presentations [5]. Thus, epidemiologic data from the 1970 British Cohort Study show that difficulties at school entry have effects into adulthood on literacy, mental health and unemployment [6].

Epidemiology

Language delay is one of the most common pre-school developmental difficulties. Prevalence estimates vary according to definition and cut point. The most quoted estimate (7% of 5 year olds) includes only those with specific language impairment, a specific category of children demonstrating poor language skills but with normal non-verbal cognition [7]. The prevalence is higher when criteria include all children with language delay; for instance, in our Early Language in Victoria Study (ELVS), nearly 20% of 4 year olds scored below −1.25 standard deviations (SD) and 25% below −1 SD on one or both of the standardised expressive and receptive (comprehension) axes [8]. Rates are even higher in socially disadvantaged populations, with language delay affecting up to 50% of preschool children reared in poverty [9]. While SLI criteria are often used in clinical research, there is evidence that language and cognition share their genetic foundation, [10] and that children with and without specific delay have broadly the same overt language features and need for intervention services [11].

Does intervention improve language outcomes?

There are growing grounds for optimism that interventions can improve language delay. Between publication in 2003 and the 2012 update, the number of trials included in the Cochrane review of interventions for children with speech and language delays/disorders rose from 33 to 64, with the number of trials included in meta-analysis rising from 25 to 54 with a total of 3872 participants [12]. Positive outcomes were identified in a number of areas, notably expressive vocabulary (effect size 0.7, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.05 to 1.25, p = 0.04), syntax (effect size 0.6, 95% CI 0.15 to 1.95, p = 0.01) and overall phonological development (effect size 0.4, 95% CI 0.13 to 0.72, p = 0.005). Less attention has been paid to broader social outcomes, such as the impact of interventions on activity limitation and participation restriction, that are of critical importance both for children’s development and success [13] and for families [14].
The majority of these trials could be construed as “targeted” interventions delivered by speech and language therapists. Parent training is a feature of a number of the studies of younger children but it is rarely possible to distil the discrete effect of parental input. Some of the review’s studies include intervention delivered by less specialised staff, early educationalists, paraprofessionals etc. under the guidance of a speech and language therapist, but to date a clear picture has not emerged as to whether such interventions, while presumably cheaper, are as effective as those delivered by specialists.
While encouraging, this evidence remains limited. Most studies are small (under 20 in each arm) with limited follow-up, many of the studies are not protocol driven and detail of the interventions was often lacking. Unsurprisingly there are few replications and heterogeneity is high. Most of the studies were ‘efficacy’ trials, carried out in controlled environments with therapies often administered by the person developing the intervention. Very few could be construed as ‘effectiveness’ trials with the potential to be rolled out across a service. The underlying populations were often not well-characterised, and little is ever reported about the children’s developmental history. Finally, very few of these studies included any form of economic analysis, making it impossible to establish the costs and benefits of the interventions.

Taking language intervention to the population level

Given both the prevalence and long-term consequences of early language delay, it is clearly an important public health issue and one for which the development of appropriate, effective population-level interventions has the potential to make a major contribution to society [15, 16].
However, the optimal timing to intervene for language delay in whole populations is not known. Very early intervention for children with late-onset language may be inefficient, because most such children resolve spontaneously by ages 3 [17] and 4 years [18]. It is now apparent that false negatives are very common up to at least 3 years. Thus, in the Early Language in Victoria Study, around half of those children formally assessed as having language delay at age 4 were not late talkers at age 2 [8].
Conversely, there are also questions about the efficacy of interventions after school commencement. The only rigorous, large-scale trial to date involved 161 6–11 year olds with language delay; short-term benefits for intervention children receiving intensive speech/language therapy over 6 months were not sustained to 12 months [19]. While this might relate to the nature of this specific intervention, alternatively it might mean that language delay is already relatively ‘fixed’ in older children.

Designing an effective population approach - the next steps

Given the evidence above that treatment can be effective, the next step is to determine whether systematic delivery of effective treatment does deliver substantial population benefit. If 2–3 years is too early but the school years too late, 4–5 years may represent an ideal window. Yet by 4 years language delay is clearly not homogeneous, so therapy must be flexible enough to meet individual needs [20].
The trial reported here is designed to translate the available evidence into a program that addresses many of the issues discussed above. Such a program would ideally be standardised and replicable, yet flexible enough for children with diverse cognitive and language profiles (unlike the standardised, uniform programs currently the focus of population research at younger ages, eg [21, 22]). It would be of greater intensity and duration than achieved in most clinical services, [23] since the limited literature suggests a dose–response relationship [24] with both duration [25] and intensity [26]. This must, however, be weighed against cost and logistic constraints, as well as parent priorities. In our trials, parents have been willing to attend blocks of up to 6 sessions for child issues detected by screening that they consider important but not necessarily urgent, such as overweight [27, 28] and slowness to talk [22].
Boyle’s large-scale RCT in 6–11 year old children, although ultimately ineffective, did demonstrate that a flexible, intensive (1.5-2 hours per week over 15 weeks), replicable, manual-guided therapy program is feasible for use with large numbers of language-delayed children [19]. The intervention was designed along dimensions previously identified for manual-guided treatment, [29] drew together procedures for language intervention considered by researcher and professionals likely to be effective, [30] and was well-received by children, schools and parents.
In light of the above, we are therefore conducting a novel population-based trial of intervention for language delay at age 4. Designed to address the identified evidence gaps, it will have adequate statistical power on the available evidence. It is manual-driven to be standardised yet flexible, is designed so that it could be rolled out in the community, will have outcomes of direct relevance to the families and the community, and includes a well-developed economic analysis. Because the participating children have been followed since infancy (see below), a rich early dataset is available with which to explore differential impacts of the intervention.

Aims and hypotheses

The Language for Learning trial poses two specific researchable questions:
1.
Does a population-based intervention targeting 4 year olds with language delay (expressive and/or receptive standard scores more than 1.25 standard deviations below the mean) improve language and associated outcomes?
 
2.
Is the intervention cost-effective for families and the health care system?
 
We hypothesise that:
1.
Compared to the control group, benefits to the intervention group at 5 and 6 years will include better mean scores on standardised tests of:
a.
Expressive/receptive language (primary functional outcomes) and vocabulary, phonological analysis and narrative skills
 
b.
Other secondary outcomes:
 
i.
Social skills and relationships
 
ii.
Emotional and behavioural well-being
 
iii.
Early literacy
 
iv.
Health-related quality of life
 
v.
‘School readiness’, measured by the Australian Early Development Index (AEDI)
 
 
2)
The intervention will be acceptable and cost-effective (against common decision thresholds).
 

Methods/design

Study design

Language for Learning is a large-scale randomised trial (ISRCTN03981121) of a targeted year-long intervention for expressive and/or receptive language delay at age 4 years, nested within a cross-sectional population-based ascertainment of language delay and described here in accordance with CONSORT guidelines. Figure 1 shows progress at time of writing. Because it has re-recruited participants from two earlier low-intensity language and literacy promotion trials with null findings, Let’s Learn Language[17] and Let’s Read[31], the trial is taking place predominantly in the same 8 Melbourne local government areas (LGAs) in which these participants continue to reside.

Prior research with this sample

Children in these two completed trials turned 4 in 2010. Combining these samples provided efficiencies in time and cost, as well as providing a wealth of early-life data on the participants and their prior service utilisation (see Measures, below). Features common to both trials include: (1) their population focus, targeting all children born in defined periods in 8 of Melbourne’s 31 LGAs (each mean birth rate, 1400 per annum); (2) recruitment in infancy by Maternal & Child Health (M&CH) nurses, who provide well-child care to all Victorian children to age 5 (reach: 97% after birth and≈75% at 12 months); (3) repeated annual measures of children’s language, behaviour and potential confounders, with many measures common to both trials; and (4) very high retention rates.
Briefly, the Let’s Read trial (ISRCTN04602902) aimed to determine whether a shared book-reading intervention delivered universally through primary care over the first 3 years of life improved language and pre-literacy outcomes by age 4 years. In 2006, M&CH centres in 5 relatively disadvantaged LGAs recruited around 650 infants at age 4–8 weeks. After subsequent randomisation intervention nurses then delivered four brief (10 minute) literacy promotion interventions at the routine 4–8, 12 and 18 month and 3½ year old visits that are part of the well-child care schedule available to every child born in Victoria. Despite excellent uptake and 89% retention at 4 years, intervention and control children had similar language and preliteracy outcomes at age 4 years [32]. Further, although recruited from relatively disadvantaged areas, the participants themselves were not particularly disadvantaged.
The Let’s Learn Language trial (ISRCTN20953675) aimed to determine whether a 6-week group parent language promotion program for slow-to-talk toddlers improves language at 2 and 3 years. 1217 children were recruited at 12 month M&CH visits in 2007; the 301 scoring ≤ 20th percentile on a 100-word expressive vocabulary list at 18 months entered the trial. Again, despite extremely good uptake and 89% retention at 3 years, intervention and control children had similar expressive and receptive language, vocabulary and behaviour at age 3, and language scores were very close to those of the general population [17].

Summary of procedures

Figure 2 graphically summarises the trial and its procedures for both the intervention and control groups in the form of a Perera diagram [33]. Two weeks before each child’s 4th birthday, parents of each Let’s Read and Let’s Learn Language participant were re-contacted and invited to participate in the new trial. Parents were sent brief written questionnaires and children received formal language assessments (Let’s Read children, March-July; Let’s Learn Language children, May-December 2010). Eligible children who entered the trial were then randomised (see below), with intervention children then offered a 20-session intervention program that ran between the 4th and 5th birthdays. All children are being re-assessed at 5 and 6 years by researchers blind to randomisation status at a single face-to-face visit in the child’s home or a convenient local venue (e.g., their maternal and child health centre).

Inclusion criteria

Children were eligible for the trial if they participated in either the Let’s Read or Let’s Learn Language trials, and had expressive and/or receptive language scores more than 1.25 SD below the normative mean on the CELF-P2 at age 4 years, with no child younger than 4.0 years, and no child older than 4.8 years at assessment.

Exclusion criteria

Were known intellectual disability, major medical conditions, hearing loss >40 dB HL in the better ear and autism spectrum disorders. Children for whom English is a second language were not excluded, but parents had to be able to complete questionnaires without interpreters at a Grade 6 level of written English.

Randomisation

OU (biostatistician) coordinated the randomisation process. Allocation to the trial arms, via computer generated random numbers, was concealed using sealed opaque envelopes. Envelopes were ordered and opened only upon confirmation of consent and recruitment for each participant. Randomisation was stratified by prior trial participation (Let’s Read versus Let’s Learn Language) and modality of language problem at recruitment (receptive only, expressive only, or both receptive and expressive). Randomisation was blocked within each stratum using randomly-permuted block sizes in a non-systematic sequence. The randomisation sequence was held by a researcher otherwise unconnected to the trial who revealed each child’s allocation to trial staff upon confirmation of recruitment.
Parents were mailed letters telling them of their child’s allocation status. Control group letters outlined the child’s language status and available speech pathology services, using letters developed for the Early Language in Victoria Study in 2007 and approved by the RCH Ethics Committee. Intervention group letters included information about the intervention program. Trial staff then phoned the parent to answer questions and arrange the first sessions.

Intervention overview

Each child commenced the 20-session, year-long program within approximately 2 months of the 4-year-old baseline. It comprises 18 weekly intervention-focused sessions in three 6-week blocks starting every 3 months; the 5-year-old blinded assessment; and an exit feedback/planning session in the following month (see Figure 2). Sessions are delivered in the child’s home by a ‘language assistant’, a university graduate experienced with parents and children and knowledgeable about child health and development; while we did not specify the professional background, the assistants ultimately had psychology and sociology backgrounds.
Before the intervention commenced, the language assistants were trained in the program, its activities and manual, and in maximising the ‘therapeutic alliance’. The pre-intervention training consisted of a ½-day workshop as well as a 2-hour long 1:1 session with the speech pathologist. Two further ½-day workshops were held before the commencement of the second intervention block (focusing on intervention techniques) and after Block 3 (focusing on the 5 year follow up assessments and the procedures and content for session 20).
Although the content is specific to this project, the manual design was adapted from Boyle, [19] and explains program principles and domains, documents the standardised activities and their hierarchies, and provides brief tasks to monitor the child’s progress towards their individual goals. The session structure was based on the ‘emergent literacy intervention program’, [34] where each session follows a specific pattern and includes a focus on alphabet knowledge and narrative.
At each session, the child and parent receive an hour of one-on-one contact with the language assistant, including: (1) brief review; (2) activities introduced by the language assistant directed at the child; (3) activities for parent and child together, with support from the language assistant; and (4) activities for home practice. Although each activity has standardised supporting materials and manual instructions, the program is personalized by selecting harder or easier (‘step up’ and ‘step down’) activities according to profile, severity and progress. In addition to the standard components of narrative skills and phonological awareness/pre-literacy skills (see below: Intervention content), specific activities are selected for each child for each block from the range offered in the manual (see below: Intervention content -Vocabulary and grammar) depending on the child’s language skills. Before each 6-week block, the trial’s coordinating speech pathologist (NZ) and each language assistant review progress and jointly select the mix and level of activities.
Four basic principles are followed. Therapy directly and overtly targets parent participation; it provides varied activities and offers multiple opportunities for practising each skill; activities are child- and parent-friendly and fun; and it encourages good parent–child interaction strategies, e.g., reinforcement and praise, following the child’s conversational lead, and ‘scaffolding’ child attempts.

Intervention content

Sessions include activities that encompass three domains, chosen for their importance to language, social and educational outcomes and demonstrated feasibility for standardised large-scale intervention delivered without specialised speech pathologist skills [19].
Vocabulary and grammar deficits impact on language, literacy [35] and discourse/narrative skills. Both are crucial for social and educational attainment and considered ameliorable [24]. Depending on the child’s language skills, activities focus on vocabulary expansion (e.g., learning new words), identifying word features (e.g., semantic groups such as ‘animals’), sentence structures and grammatical markers (e.g., targeting correct sentences or ‘ing’ endings in verbs), or comprehension skills (e.g., following instructions and asking clarifying questions if needed). With support from the language assistant, these vocabulary and grammar activities are directed at the parent and child together.
Narrative skills, often a focus for clinical intervention, [36] underpin communicative competence and correlate strongly with reading comprehension; deficits impact on social interaction and understanding of classroom processes [37]. In this intervention program, they are targeted through shared book reading activities, which explicitly teach ‘story grammar’ elements such as ‘who, what, where’ [37].
Phonological awareness/preliteracy skills are strongly linked to oral language [38] and literacy, [39] are usually established well before a child starts school, [40] and can be effectively taught to children with language disorders [41]. Print conventions (e.g., left to right reading), awareness of rhyme, and letter-sound connections are targeted through shared book reading. Other activities specifically target skills like phoneme identity and phoneme matching and are directly taught to the children by the language assistant.

Measures

4 year old ascertainment assessment (4 years, n≈1,500): Because of uncertainties noted in all systematic reviews about the predictive properties of screening tools, [24, 42, 43] the main criterion for study entry was a formal assessment of language skills using the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Preschool (CELF-P2) [44]. The CELF-P2 is norm-referenced for children from 3:0–6:11 years and yields two core subscales of receptive and expressive language. We administered the Word Structure, Expressive Vocabulary and Recalling Sentences Expressive subtests and the Sentence Structure, Concepts and Following Directions and Basic Concepts Receptive subtests. We did not analyse the Language Content or Language Structure Indices.
Trial measures collected at 4 years (baseline), 5 years (intermediate outcomes) and 6 years (definitive outcomes) are detailed in Table 1. A range of measures (not described here) were also collected at multiple waves between 1 and 3 years of age in the two preceding trials, and will support additional exploratory and mediator analyses.
Table 1
Primary and secondary outcome measures for the Language for Learning Trial
Construct
Timing (years)
Measure
Additional information
 
4
5
6
  
Primary Outcome
     
Expressive and Receptive language
Clinical Ev aluation of Language Fundamentals - Preschool Second Edition (CELF P2) [44]
Baseline only: Basic Concepts
     
5 Years only: Word Classes (receptive + expressive subtests)
     
6 Years only: Word Classes (receptive only).
Secondary Outcomes
     
Receptive vocabulary
 
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-4) [45]
Used with Intervention group only at 4–5 Years (Baseline)
Phonological skills
 
Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP) [46]
Subtests used: Elision; Blending Words; Sound Matching.
     
Contribute to Phonological Awareness Composite Score.
 
 
Sutherland Phonological Awareness Test – Revised: Modified (SPAT-R) [47]
Three individual scores obtained for the 3 subtests used: Rhyme Detection Subtest; Onset Phoneme Identification; Letter Knowledge (study specific)
 
 
Children's Test of Non-Word Repetition (CNREP) [48]
Data on subgroup only at baseline, as measure was discontinued due to time restrictions
Literacy skills
  
Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT) [49]
3 subtests used: Word Reading; Sentence Comprehension;
     
Spelling; Word Reading and Spelling create ‘Reading Composite’
Pragmatic skills (social language use)
Children’s Communication Checklist, 2nd Edition (CCC:2) [50]
28 items on the 4 subscales used: inappropriate initiation; stereotyped language; use of context; nonverbal communication.
Narrative
 
The Renfrew Language Scales: Bus Story Test [51]
Used with Intervention group only at baseline
Non-verbal intelligence
  
Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, 2nd Edition (KBIT-2) [52]
Only the matrices subtest was used as it gives a measure of nonverbal (fluid) intelligence
Early childhood development
 
Ages & Stages Questionnaire (A&SQ) [53]
5 developmental areas: Communication; Gross Motor; Fine Motor; Problem Solving; Personal-Social; totals for each developmental area are compared with empirical cut-points for each area
   
Australian Early Development Index (AEDI) [54]
Teacher reported questionnaire measuring 5 domains: social competence; emotional maturity; language and cognitive skills (school-based); communication skills and general knowledge
Quality of life
Health Utilities Index (HUI)- Mark 2 and 3 [55]
Parent-reported measure scored using a single- and multi-attribute utility function based on preference scores (sensation, mobility, self-care, fertility, vision, hearing, speech, ambulation, dexterity, emotion, cognition, pain). Scores will be used to calculate quality-adjusted life years (QALYs)
 
Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory (PedsQL); parent-proxy [56]
Parent-completed 23 item scale comprising 4 dimensions, with 3 summary Scores: Total; Physical Health; Psychosocial Health.
Behaviour
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ ) [57]
25 item measure that yields one score of total behavioural problems and scores for emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity, peer problems, and prosocial subscales
Service Utilisation
Study generated
Parent-reported questionnaire to track health service utilisation by population with a specific health condition

Economic evaluation

Although progress has been made in modelling the costs and long terms benefits of intervention for language-impaired children [58] and in interpreting unit costs, [59] economic analyses remain few and far between [60]. We will employ cost-consequences analysis conducted from both the broad societal perspective and the narrower perspective of the health care sector, [61] as interventions cost-effective from a health care perspective can add substantially to family costs [62]. The economic evaluation will compare any incremental costs of the intervention (costs accrued in the intervention arm compared to costs accrued in the control arm) to the full list of incremental primary and secondary outcomes, all expressed in their natural units of measurement. Uncertainty in the cost and outcome data and sensitivity of economic evaluation results to the methods of evaluation chosen will be tested through extensive sensitivity analyses.
The estimation of costs will collect resource use data from three main sources: research team records; intervention provider records; and parental report (via written questionnaires at child ages 4, 5 and 6). Key costs for the economic evaluation are program costs (including language assistant and other researcher time in relation to the intervention, intervention material costs and travel expenses) and family costs (family time spent on the intervention, costs to health service use and other government services outside of the intervention, and travel costs). Parents will be asked to recall health service resource use over the previous 12 months for their child, including doctor visits, other government services, private speech pathology, parental time and travel costs. Measured resource use will be valued using existing estimates of the cost of each unit of resource use from sources such as the Medicare Benefit Schedule fee rates for family practitioner and specialist doctor attendances, Australian Bureau of Statistics estimates of average Australian earnings, Royal Automobile Club of Victoria (RACV) estimates of travel costs, etc.

Sample size

We anticipated that 1500 of the 1850 children in the two trials would be assessed at 4 years, allowing for 20% loss to follow-up (similar to ELVS). Assuming 240 subjects (16% of the 1500) would have language scores more than 1.25 SD below the CELF-P2 normative means, a further 10% would decline participation and 1% would be excluded, we estimated 210 subjects would enter the trial (105 in each arm). 10% attrition (similar to our recent behaviour [63] and obesity [28, 64] trials) would provide 94 children in each trial arm at outcome, giving 80% power to detect a difference of 0.41 SD at the 5% level of significance. Even if attrition were 25%, we could still detect a 0.45 SD difference with 78 in each arm.

Data analysis

For Hypotheses 1 and 2, outcomes and costs will be compared between the trial arms using the intention-to-treat principle with participants analysed according to the trial arms they were randomised to. We will compare mean outcomes at 5 and 6 year old follow-up using linear regression in unadjusted analyses and analyses adjusted for the following prognostic factors: child gender, whether recruited from Let’s Read or Let’s Learn Language, expressive and receptive language scores at baseline, and baseline measure of the outcome being considered when available.
The trial is powered primarily to address the main comparison between trial arms, but we will also use tests of interaction to conduct exploratory analyses addressing differential effects of the intervention across the following subgroups:
1.
Language delay sub-group (expressive, receptive, mixed expressive/receptive);
 
2.
Non-specific (non-verbal IQ < 85) vs specific (non-verbal IQ ≥ 85) language delay;
 
3.
Social disadvantage, to determine whether this population intervention may increase, not decrease, inequalities.
 
Recognising that definitive answers to these complex issues may need even larger samples, we plan to make these data available for data pooling and meta-analysis via the Centre for Research Excellence in Children’s Language (NHMRC Grant 1023493) for which Reilly, Wake, Law, Gold, and Goldfeld are Chief Investigators.

Discussion

This rigorous trial addresses the urgent need to improve the long-term consequences of early language delay, within a public health framework appropriate to its high prevalence and societal burden. Using existing cohorts offers time- and cost-efficiencies and a unique opportunity to understand different responses to therapy. The therapy interventions are not controversial, being already widely used clinically by speech pathologists. The flexible but standardised approach has already been shown by Boyle to be feasible and acceptable to parents and older children. Our innovation is in the systematic identification of language delay and rigorous attention to program delivery and dose in preschool children. The trial responds to identified evidence gaps, has outcomes of direct relevance to families and the community, and includes a well-developed economic analysis.
If effective, we expect the following outcomes:
  • The best evidence yet that language delay can be readily identified, cost-efficiently addressed and significantly improved before formal schooling starts.
  • A well-tested intervention that could potentially be delivered to children by a range of health and educational professionals, going some way to addressing the speech therapist shortages in a number of English-speaking countries and addressing a real and timely health services policy imperative.

Acknowledgements

The trial is funded by the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC Project Grant 607407). The NHMRC played no role in the trial’s design; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication. We thank all the children, parents, maternal and child health nurses and research assistants (including Jon Quach, Elizabeth Varelias, Liz Varley, Rebecca Nadalin, Hannah Bryson, Laura Punaro, Lisa Quinn, Leah Zelencich, Peta Newell and Jane Sheehan) who took part in the trial. We are indebted to Professor Luigi Girolametto for his central role in the preceding Let’s Learn Language trial and the early advice he provided on the structure of the Language for Learning intervention. We especially acknowledge the outstanding contributions of Sherryn Tobin, Natasha Napiza and Ruth Nicholls as Project Managers of the earlier Let’s Learn Language and Let’s Read trials, without whom our population sample would not have existed. MW was part-funded by NHMRC Population Health Career Development Grants 284556 and 546405; SG, JS and OU by NHMRC Capacity Building Grant 436914; SR by NHMRC Practitioner Fellowship 491210; and LG by NHMRC Capacity Building Grant 425855 and NHMRC Early Career Fellowship 1035100. Murdoch Childrens Research Institute is supported by the Victorian Government’s Operational Infrastructure Support Program.
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Competing interests

All authors declare that they and their spouses, partners or children have no financial and non-financial relationships or interests that may be relevant to the submitted work. The authors declare they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MW conceived the Language for Learning trial with JL, LG, UO, SG, SR and JS; she takes overall responsibility for all aspects of the trial and this manuscript. ST was the Project Manager, assisted by PL and NZ. NZ and JL designed the intervention, with advice from JS and SR, who also advised on measures and their interpretation. OU advised on statistical issues, LG and HL on the economic evaluation, and SG on the translational aspects of the trial. All authors contributed, read and approved the final manuscript.
Anhänge

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Rice ML, Sell MA, Hadley PA: Social interactions of speech, and language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1991, 34 (6): 1299-1307.CrossRefPubMed Rice ML, Sell MA, Hadley PA: Social interactions of speech, and language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1991, 34 (6): 1299-1307.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Caulfield MB, Fischel JE, DeBaryshe BD, Whitehurst GJ: Behavioral correlates of developmental expressive language disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1989, 17 (2): 187-201. 10.1007/BF00913793.CrossRefPubMed Caulfield MB, Fischel JE, DeBaryshe BD, Whitehurst GJ: Behavioral correlates of developmental expressive language disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1989, 17 (2): 187-201. 10.1007/BF00913793.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnston SS, Reichle J: Designing and implementing interventions to decrease challenging behavior. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools. 1993, 24 (4): 225-235.CrossRef Johnston SS, Reichle J: Designing and implementing interventions to decrease challenging behavior. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools. 1993, 24 (4): 225-235.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Prizant BM, Audet LR, Burke GM, Hummel LJ, Maher SR, Theadore G: Communication disorders and emotional/behavioral disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disord. 1990, 55 (2): 179-192.CrossRef Prizant BM, Audet LR, Burke GM, Hummel LJ, Maher SR, Theadore G: Communication disorders and emotional/behavioral disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disord. 1990, 55 (2): 179-192.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Clegg J, Hollis C, Mawhood L, Rutter M: Developmental language disorders–a follow-up in later adult life. Cognitive, language and psychosocial outcomes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005, 46 (2): 128-149. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00342.x.CrossRefPubMed Clegg J, Hollis C, Mawhood L, Rutter M: Developmental language disorders–a follow-up in later adult life. Cognitive, language and psychosocial outcomes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005, 46 (2): 128-149. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00342.x.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Law J, Rush R, Schoon I, Parsons S: Modeling developmental language difficulties from school entry into adulthood: literacy, mental health, and employment outcomes. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research. 2009, 52 (6): 1401-1416. 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0142).CrossRef Law J, Rush R, Schoon I, Parsons S: Modeling developmental language difficulties from school entry into adulthood: literacy, mental health, and employment outcomes. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research. 2009, 52 (6): 1401-1416. 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0142).CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Tomblin JB, Records NL, Buckwalter P, Zhang X, Smith E, O'Brien M: Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 1997, 40 (6): 1245-1260.CrossRef Tomblin JB, Records NL, Buckwalter P, Zhang X, Smith E, O'Brien M: Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 1997, 40 (6): 1245-1260.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Reilly S, Wake M, Ukoumunne OC, Bavin E, Prior M, Cini E, Conway L, Eadie P, Bretherton L: Predicting language outcomes at 4 years of age: findings from early language in victoria study. Pediatrics. 2010, 126 (6): e1530-e1537. 10.1542/peds.2010-0254.CrossRefPubMed Reilly S, Wake M, Ukoumunne OC, Bavin E, Prior M, Cini E, Conway L, Eadie P, Bretherton L: Predicting language outcomes at 4 years of age: findings from early language in victoria study. Pediatrics. 2010, 126 (6): e1530-e1537. 10.1542/peds.2010-0254.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Locke A, Ginsborg J, Peers I: Development and disadvantage: implications for the early years and beyond. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders. 2002, 37 (1): 3-15. 10.1080/13682820110089911.CrossRef Locke A, Ginsborg J, Peers I: Development and disadvantage: implications for the early years and beyond. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders. 2002, 37 (1): 3-15. 10.1080/13682820110089911.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Trouton A, Spinath FM, Plomin R: Twins Early Development Study (TEDS). A multivariate, longitudinal genetic investigation of language and cognition problems in childhood. Twin Research. 2002, 5 (5): 444-448.CrossRefPubMed Trouton A, Spinath FM, Plomin R: Twins Early Development Study (TEDS). A multivariate, longitudinal genetic investigation of language and cognition problems in childhood. Twin Research. 2002, 5 (5): 444-448.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Rice ML, Tomblin JB, Hoffman L, Richman WA, Marquis J: Grammatical tense deficits in children with SLI and nonspecific language impairment: relationships with nonverbal IQ over time. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 2004, 47 (4): 816-834. 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/061).CrossRef Rice ML, Tomblin JB, Hoffman L, Richman WA, Marquis J: Grammatical tense deficits in children with SLI and nonspecific language impairment: relationships with nonverbal IQ over time. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 2004, 47 (4): 816-834. 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/061).CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Law J, Garrett Z, Nye C, Dennis J: Speech and language therapy interventions for children with primary speech and language delay or disorder (Cochrane Review) (2012 update of the 2003 Review). 2012, Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, in press Law J, Garrett Z, Nye C, Dennis J: Speech and language therapy interventions for children with primary speech and language delay or disorder (Cochrane Review) (2012 update of the 2003 Review). 2012, Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, in press
13.
Zurück zum Zitat World Health Organization: International classification of functioning, disability and health (ICF). 2001, World Health Organisation, Geneva, Switzerland World Health Organization: International classification of functioning, disability and health (ICF). 2001, World Health Organisation, Geneva, Switzerland
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Thomas-Stonell N, Oddson B, Robertson B, Rosenbaum P: Outcomes in preschool children following language treatment: Parent and clinician perspectives. J Commun Disord. 2009, 42: 29-42. 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2008.08.002.CrossRefPubMed Thomas-Stonell N, Oddson B, Robertson B, Rosenbaum P: Outcomes in preschool children following language treatment: Parent and clinician perspectives. J Commun Disord. 2009, 42: 29-42. 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2008.08.002.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Keating DP, Hertzman C: In Developmental Health & the Wealth of Nations. Modernity's paradox. 1999, Keating H, New York, NY: Guilford Press, 1-17. Keating DP, Hertzman C: In Developmental Health & the Wealth of Nations. Modernity's paradox. 1999, Keating H, New York, NY: Guilford Press, 1-17.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Willms DJ: In Developmental Health & the Wealth of Nations. Quality and inequality in children's literacy. 1999, Keating H, New York, NY: Guilford Press, 72-93. Willms DJ: In Developmental Health & the Wealth of Nations. Quality and inequality in children's literacy. 1999, Keating H, New York, NY: Guilford Press, 72-93.
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Wake M, Tobin S, Girolametto L, Ukoumunne OC, Gold L, Levickis P, Sheehan J, Goldfeld S, Reilly S: Outcomes of population based language promotion for slow to talk toddlers at ages 2 and 3 years: Let’s Learn Language cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ. 2011, 343: d4741-10.1136/bmj.d4741.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Wake M, Tobin S, Girolametto L, Ukoumunne OC, Gold L, Levickis P, Sheehan J, Goldfeld S, Reilly S: Outcomes of population based language promotion for slow to talk toddlers at ages 2 and 3 years: Let’s Learn Language cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ. 2011, 343: d4741-10.1136/bmj.d4741.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Paul R, Spangle-Looney S, Dahm P: Communication and socialization skills at age 2 and 3 in “late talking” young children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 1991, 34: 858-865.CrossRef Paul R, Spangle-Looney S, Dahm P: Communication and socialization skills at age 2 and 3 in “late talking” young children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 1991, 34: 858-865.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Boyle J, McCartney E, Forbes J, O’Hare A: A randomized controlled trial and economic evaluation of direct vs indirect and individual vs group modes of speech and language therapy for children with primary language impairment. Health Technology Assessment. 2007, 11 (25): 1-139.CrossRef Boyle J, McCartney E, Forbes J, O’Hare A: A randomized controlled trial and economic evaluation of direct vs indirect and individual vs group modes of speech and language therapy for children with primary language impairment. Health Technology Assessment. 2007, 11 (25): 1-139.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Dockrell J, Lindsay G: Meeting the needs of children with specific speech and language difficulties. European Journal of Special Needs Education. 2000, 15: 24-41. 10.1080/088562500361682.CrossRef Dockrell J, Lindsay G: Meeting the needs of children with specific speech and language difficulties. European Journal of Special Needs Education. 2000, 15: 24-41. 10.1080/088562500361682.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Buschmann A, Jooss B, Rupp A, Feldhusen F, Pietz J, Philippi H: Parent based language intervention for 2-year-old children with specific expressive language delay: a randomised controlled trial. Arch Dis Child. 2009, 94 (2): 110-116.CrossRefPubMed Buschmann A, Jooss B, Rupp A, Feldhusen F, Pietz J, Philippi H: Parent based language intervention for 2-year-old children with specific expressive language delay: a randomised controlled trial. Arch Dis Child. 2009, 94 (2): 110-116.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Sheehan J, Girolametto L, Reilly S, Ukoumunne O, Price A, Gold L, Weitzman E, Wake M: Feasibility of a language promotion program for toddlers at risk. Early Childhood Services. 2009, 3 (1): 33-50. Sheehan J, Girolametto L, Reilly S, Ukoumunne O, Price A, Gold L, Weitzman E, Wake M: Feasibility of a language promotion program for toddlers at risk. Early Childhood Services. 2009, 3 (1): 33-50.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Glogowska M, Roulstone S, Enderby P, Peters TJ: Randomised controlled trial of community based speech and language therapy in preschool children. BMJ. 2000, 321: 923-926. 10.1136/bmj.321.7266.923.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Glogowska M, Roulstone S, Enderby P, Peters TJ: Randomised controlled trial of community based speech and language therapy in preschool children. BMJ. 2000, 321: 923-926. 10.1136/bmj.321.7266.923.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Law J, Garrett Z, Nye C: The effectiveness of speech and language therapy interventions for children with primary speech and language delay or disorder. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 2004, 47: 924-943. 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/069).CrossRef Law J, Garrett Z, Nye C: The effectiveness of speech and language therapy interventions for children with primary speech and language delay or disorder. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 2004, 47: 924-943. 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/069).CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Reid J, Donaldson ML, Howell J, Dean EC, Grieve R: In Child Language. Effectiveness of therapy for child phonological disorder. 1996, Adridge M. Clevedon, Avon, Multilingual Matters Reid J, Donaldson ML, Howell J, Dean EC, Grieve R: In Child Language. Effectiveness of therapy for child phonological disorder. 1996, Adridge M. Clevedon, Avon, Multilingual Matters
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Barratt J, Littlejohns P, Thompson J: Trial of intensive compared to weekly speech therapy in preschool children. Arch Dis Child. 1992, 671: 106-108.CrossRef Barratt J, Littlejohns P, Thompson J: Trial of intensive compared to weekly speech therapy in preschool children. Arch Dis Child. 1992, 671: 106-108.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Wake M, Gold L, McCallum Z, Gerner B, Waters E: Economic evaluation of a primary care trial to reduce weight gain in overweight/obese children: the LEAP trial. Ambul Pediatr. 2008, 8 (5): 336-341. 10.1016/j.ambp.2008.06.006.CrossRefPubMed Wake M, Gold L, McCallum Z, Gerner B, Waters E: Economic evaluation of a primary care trial to reduce weight gain in overweight/obese children: the LEAP trial. Ambul Pediatr. 2008, 8 (5): 336-341. 10.1016/j.ambp.2008.06.006.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Wake M, Baur L, Gerner B, Gibbons K, Gold L, Gunn J, Levickis P, McCallum Z, Naughton G, Sanci L, Ukoumunne OC: Outcomes and costs of primary care surveillance and intervention for overweight/obese children: the LEAP 2 trial. BMJ. 2009, 339: b3308-10.1136/bmj.b3308.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Wake M, Baur L, Gerner B, Gibbons K, Gold L, Gunn J, Levickis P, McCallum Z, Naughton G, Sanci L, Ukoumunne OC: Outcomes and costs of primary care surveillance and intervention for overweight/obese children: the LEAP 2 trial. BMJ. 2009, 339: b3308-10.1136/bmj.b3308.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Carroll KM: Manual-guided psychosocial treatment. A new virtual requirement for pharmacotherapy trials?. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997, 54: 923-928. 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830220041007.CrossRefPubMed Carroll KM: Manual-guided psychosocial treatment. A new virtual requirement for pharmacotherapy trials?. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997, 54: 923-928. 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830220041007.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat McCartney E, Boyle J, Bannatyne S, Jessiman E, Kelsey C, Smith J, O'Hare A: Becoming a manual occupation? Construction of a therapy manual for language impaired children in primary schools. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 2004, 39 (1): 135-148. 10.1080/01431160310001603592.CrossRefPubMed McCartney E, Boyle J, Bannatyne S, Jessiman E, Kelsey C, Smith J, O'Hare A: Becoming a manual occupation? Construction of a therapy manual for language impaired children in primary schools. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 2004, 39 (1): 135-148. 10.1080/01431160310001603592.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Goldfeld S, Napiza N, Quach J, Reilly S, Ukoumunne OC, Wake M: Outcomes of a universal shared reading intervention by 2 years of age: the let's read trial. Pediatrics. 2011, 127 (3): 445-453. 10.1542/peds.2009-3043.CrossRefPubMed Goldfeld S, Napiza N, Quach J, Reilly S, Ukoumunne OC, Wake M: Outcomes of a universal shared reading intervention by 2 years of age: the let's read trial. Pediatrics. 2011, 127 (3): 445-453. 10.1542/peds.2009-3043.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Goldfeld S, Quach J, Nicholls R, Reilly S, Ukoumunne O, Wake M: Four-year old outcomes of a universal infant-toddler shared reading intervention., The Let’s Read trial. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, in press Goldfeld S, Quach J, Nicholls R, Reilly S, Ukoumunne O, Wake M: Four-year old outcomes of a universal infant-toddler shared reading intervention., The Let’s Read trial. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, in press
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Perera R, Heneghan C, Yudkin P: Graphical method for depicting randomised trials of complex interventions. BMJ. 2007, 334 (7585): 127-129. 10.1136/bmj.39045.396817.68.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Perera R, Heneghan C, Yudkin P: Graphical method for depicting randomised trials of complex interventions. BMJ. 2007, 334 (7585): 127-129. 10.1136/bmj.39045.396817.68.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Pile E, Girolametto L, Johnson CJ, Cleave PL, Chen X: Shared book reading intervention for children with language impairment: using parents-as-aides in language intervention. Canadian Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology. 2010, 34 (3): 96-109. Pile E, Girolametto L, Johnson CJ, Cleave PL, Chen X: Shared book reading intervention for children with language impairment: using parents-as-aides in language intervention. Canadian Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology. 2010, 34 (3): 96-109.
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Nash H, Snowling M: Teaching new words to children with poor existing vocabulary knowledge: a controlled evaluation of the definition and context methods. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 2006, 41 (3): 335-354. 10.1080/13682820600602295.CrossRefPubMed Nash H, Snowling M: Teaching new words to children with poor existing vocabulary knowledge: a controlled evaluation of the definition and context methods. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 2006, 41 (3): 335-354. 10.1080/13682820600602295.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Hoggan KC, Strong CJ: The magic of "Once upon a time": narrative teaching strategies. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools. 1994, 25 (2): 76-89.CrossRef Hoggan KC, Strong CJ: The magic of "Once upon a time": narrative teaching strategies. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools. 1994, 25 (2): 76-89.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Davies P, Shanks B, Davies K: Improving narrative skills in young children with delayed language development. Educ Rev. 2004, 56: 271-286. 10.1080/0013191042000201181.CrossRef Davies P, Shanks B, Davies K: Improving narrative skills in young children with delayed language development. Educ Rev. 2004, 56: 271-286. 10.1080/0013191042000201181.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Storch SA, Whitehurst GJ: Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Dev Psychol. 2002, 38: 934-947.CrossRefPubMed Storch SA, Whitehurst GJ: Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Dev Psychol. 2002, 38: 934-947.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Ehri LC, Nunes SR, Willows DM, Schuster BV, Yaghoub-Zadeh Z, Shanahan T: Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: evidence from the national reading panel's meta-analysis. Read Res Q. 2001, 36 (3): 250-287. 10.1598/RRQ.36.3.2.CrossRef Ehri LC, Nunes SR, Willows DM, Schuster BV, Yaghoub-Zadeh Z, Shanahan T: Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: evidence from the national reading panel's meta-analysis. Read Res Q. 2001, 36 (3): 250-287. 10.1598/RRQ.36.3.2.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Whitehurst G, Lonigan C: In Handbook of Early Literacy Development. Emergent literacy: from prereaders to readers. 2001, Neuman S, Dickinson D, New York: Guilford, 11-29. Whitehurst G, Lonigan C: In Handbook of Early Literacy Development. Emergent literacy: from prereaders to readers. 2001, Neuman S, Dickinson D, New York: Guilford, 11-29.
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Bowyer-Crane C, Snowling MJ, Duff FJ, Fieldsend E, Carroll JM, Miles J, Götz K, Hulme C: Improving early language and literacy skills: differential effects of an oral language versus a phonology with reading intervention. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008, 49 (4): 422-432. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01849.x.CrossRefPubMed Bowyer-Crane C, Snowling MJ, Duff FJ, Fieldsend E, Carroll JM, Miles J, Götz K, Hulme C: Improving early language and literacy skills: differential effects of an oral language versus a phonology with reading intervention. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008, 49 (4): 422-432. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01849.x.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Law J, Boyle J, Harris F, Harkness A, Nye C: Screening for primary speech and language delay: a systematic review of the literature. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 1998, 33 (S1): 21-23. 10.3109/13682829809179388.CrossRefPubMed Law J, Boyle J, Harris F, Harkness A, Nye C: Screening for primary speech and language delay: a systematic review of the literature. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 1998, 33 (S1): 21-23. 10.3109/13682829809179388.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Nelson HD, Nygren P, Walker M, Panoscha R: Screening for speech and language delay in preschool children: systematic evidence review for the US Preventive Services Task Force. Pediatrics. 2006, 117 (2): e298-319. 10.1542/peds.2005-1467.CrossRefPubMed Nelson HD, Nygren P, Walker M, Panoscha R: Screening for speech and language delay in preschool children: systematic evidence review for the US Preventive Services Task Force. Pediatrics. 2006, 117 (2): e298-319. 10.1542/peds.2005-1467.CrossRefPubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Semel E, Wiig E, Secord W: The Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Preschool, Second Edition (CELF-P2): Australian Standardised Edition. 2006, Harcourt Assessment Inc., NSW, Australia Semel E, Wiig E, Secord W: The Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Preschool, Second Edition (CELF-P2): Australian Standardised Edition. 2006, Harcourt Assessment Inc., NSW, Australia
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Dunn LM, Dunn DM: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition (PPVT™-4): Fourth edition. 2007, Pearson Assessments, Minneapolis, MN Dunn LM, Dunn DM: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition (PPVT™-4): Fourth edition. 2007, Pearson Assessments, Minneapolis, MN
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA: Comprehensive test of phonological processing. 1999, Pro-ed Publishing, Austin, Texas Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA: Comprehensive test of phonological processing. 1999, Pro-ed Publishing, Austin, Texas
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Neilson R: Sutherland Phonological Awareness Test: Revised - Manual. 2007, Language Speech and Literary Services, Jamberoo, NSW Neilson R: Sutherland Phonological Awareness Test: Revised - Manual. 2007, Language Speech and Literary Services, Jamberoo, NSW
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Gathercole S, Willis C, Baddeley A, Emslie H: Children's test of non word repetition: a test of phonological working memory. Memory. 1994, 2 (2): 103-127. 10.1080/09658219408258940.CrossRefPubMed Gathercole S, Willis C, Baddeley A, Emslie H: Children's test of non word repetition: a test of phonological working memory. Memory. 1994, 2 (2): 103-127. 10.1080/09658219408258940.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Wilkinson SG, Robertson JG: Wide Range Achievement Test Professional Manual. 2006, Psychological Assessment Resources Inc, Florida, USA Wilkinson SG, Robertson JG: Wide Range Achievement Test Professional Manual. 2006, Psychological Assessment Resources Inc, Florida, USA
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Bishop DVM: The Children's Communication Checklist 2nd edition. 2003, The Psychological Corporation, London, UK Bishop DVM: The Children's Communication Checklist 2nd edition. 2003, The Psychological Corporation, London, UK
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Renfrew C: The Bus Story Test: A test of narrative speech. 1997, Speechmark Publishing Inc., Milton Keynes, UK Renfrew C: The Bus Story Test: A test of narrative speech. 1997, Speechmark Publishing Inc., Milton Keynes, UK
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaufman SA, Kaufman LN: Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test 2nd edition. 2004, Pearson, Inc., Bloomington, MN Kaufman SA, Kaufman LN: Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test 2nd edition. 2004, Pearson, Inc., Bloomington, MN
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Bricker D, Squires J, Mounts L, Potter L, Nickel R, Twombly E: Ages and Stages Questionnaire - A parent completed child monitoring system 2nd edition. 1999, Paul H Brookes Publishing Co, Baltimore, USA Bricker D, Squires J, Mounts L, Potter L, Nickel R, Twombly E: Ages and Stages Questionnaire - A parent completed child monitoring system 2nd edition. 1999, Paul H Brookes Publishing Co, Baltimore, USA
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Centre for Community Child Health and Telethon Institute for Child Health Research: A snapshot of Early Childhood Development in Australia: AEDI National Report 2009. 2009, Canberra, Australian Government Centre for Community Child Health and Telethon Institute for Child Health Research: A snapshot of Early Childhood Development in Australia: AEDI National Report 2009. 2009, Canberra, Australian Government
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Feeny D: The health utilities: a tool for assessing health benefits. PRO Newsletter. 2005, 34: 2-6. Feeny D: The health utilities: a tool for assessing health benefits. PRO Newsletter. 2005, 34: 2-6.
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Varni JW, Seid M, Kurtin PS: PedsQL 4.0: reliability and validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory version 4.0 generic core scales in healthy and patient populations. Medical Care. 2001, 39: 800-812. 10.1097/00005650-200108000-00006.CrossRefPubMed Varni JW, Seid M, Kurtin PS: PedsQL 4.0: reliability and validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory version 4.0 generic core scales in healthy and patient populations. Medical Care. 2001, 39: 800-812. 10.1097/00005650-200108000-00006.CrossRefPubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Goodman R: Psychometric properties of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001, 40: 1337-1345. 10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015.CrossRefPubMed Goodman R: Psychometric properties of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001, 40: 1337-1345. 10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015.CrossRefPubMed
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Marsh K, Bertranou E, Suominem H, Venatachalem M: An economic evaluation of speech and language therapy. 2010, Matrix Evidence, London Marsh K, Bertranou E, Suominem H, Venatachalem M: An economic evaluation of speech and language therapy. 2010, Matrix Evidence, London
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Beecham J, Law J, Lindsay G, Zeng B: Costing speech, language and communication interventions.  , International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, in press Beecham J, Law J, Lindsay G, Zeng B: Costing speech, language and communication interventions.  , International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, in press
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Law J, Zeng B, Lindsay G, Beecham J: Cost-effectiveness of interventions for children with speech, language and communication needs (SLCN): a review using the Drummond and Jefferson (1996) ‘Referee's Checklist’. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 2012, 47 (1): 1-10. 10.1111/j.1460-6984.2011.00084.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Law J, Zeng B, Lindsay G, Beecham J: Cost-effectiveness of interventions for children with speech, language and communication needs (SLCN): a review using the Drummond and Jefferson (1996) ‘Referee's Checklist’. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 2012, 47 (1): 1-10. 10.1111/j.1460-6984.2011.00084.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Drummond M, Sculpher M, Torrance G, O’Brien B, Stoddart G: Methods for the economic evaluation of health care programs: 3rd edition ed. 2005, Oxford Uni Press, Oxford Drummond M, Sculpher M, Torrance G, O’Brien B, Stoddart G: Methods for the economic evaluation of health care programs: 3rd edition ed. 2005, Oxford Uni Press, Oxford
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Gibbard D, Coglan L, MacDonald J: Cost-effectiveness analysis of current practice and parent intervention for children under 3 years presenting with expressive language delay. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 2004, 39 (2): 229-244. 10.1080/13682820310001618839.CrossRefPubMed Gibbard D, Coglan L, MacDonald J: Cost-effectiveness analysis of current practice and parent intervention for children under 3 years presenting with expressive language delay. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 2004, 39 (2): 229-244. 10.1080/13682820310001618839.CrossRefPubMed
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Hiscock H, Bayer JK, Price A, Ukoumunne OC, Wake M: A universal parenting program to prevent early child behaviour problems: cluster randomised trial. BMJ. 2008, 336: 318-321. 10.1136/bmj.39451.609676.AE.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hiscock H, Bayer JK, Price A, Ukoumunne OC, Wake M: A universal parenting program to prevent early child behaviour problems: cluster randomised trial. BMJ. 2008, 336: 318-321. 10.1136/bmj.39451.609676.AE.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
64.
Zurück zum Zitat McCallum Z, Wake M, Gerner B, Baur LA, Gibbons K, Gold L, Gunn J, Harris C, Naughton G, Riess C, Sanci L, Ukoumunne O, Waters E: Outcome data from the LEAP (Live, Eat and Play) trial: a randomized controlled trial of a primary care intervention for childhood overweight/mild obesity. Int J Obes. 2007, 31: 630-636. McCallum Z, Wake M, Gerner B, Baur LA, Gibbons K, Gold L, Gunn J, Harris C, Naughton G, Riess C, Sanci L, Ukoumunne O, Waters E: Outcome data from the LEAP (Live, Eat and Play) trial: a randomized controlled trial of a primary care intervention for childhood overweight/mild obesity. Int J Obes. 2007, 31: 630-636.
Metadaten
Titel
Improving outcomes of preschool language delay in the community: protocol for the Language for Learning randomised controlled trial
verfasst von
Melissa Wake
Penny Levickis
Sherryn Tobin
Naomi Zens
James Law
Lisa Gold
Obioha C Ukoumunne
Sharon Goldfeld
Ha ND Le
Jemma Skeat
Sheena Reilly
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2012
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Pediatrics / Ausgabe 1/2012
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2431
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2431-12-96

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2012

BMC Pediatrics 1/2012 Zur Ausgabe

„Übersichtlicher Wegweiser“: Lauterbachs umstrittener Klinik-Atlas ist online

17.05.2024 Klinik aktuell Nachrichten

Sie sei „ethisch geboten“, meint Gesundheitsminister Karl Lauterbach: mehr Transparenz über die Qualität von Klinikbehandlungen. Um sie abzubilden, lässt er gegen den Widerstand vieler Länder einen virtuellen Klinik-Atlas freischalten.

ADHS-Medikation erhöht das kardiovaskuläre Risiko

16.05.2024 Herzinsuffizienz Nachrichten

Erwachsene, die Medikamente gegen das Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-Hyperaktivitätssyndrom einnehmen, laufen offenbar erhöhte Gefahr, an Herzschwäche zu erkranken oder einen Schlaganfall zu erleiden. Es scheint eine Dosis-Wirkungs-Beziehung zu bestehen.

Erstmanifestation eines Diabetes-Typ-1 bei Kindern: Ein Notfall!

16.05.2024 DDG-Jahrestagung 2024 Kongressbericht

Manifestiert sich ein Typ-1-Diabetes bei Kindern, ist das ein Notfall – ebenso wie eine diabetische Ketoazidose. Die Grundsäulen der Therapie bestehen aus Rehydratation, Insulin und Kaliumgabe. Insulin ist das Medikament der Wahl zur Behandlung der Ketoazidose.

Frühe Hypertonie erhöht späteres kardiovaskuläres Risiko

Wie wichtig es ist, pädiatrische Patienten auf Bluthochdruck zu screenen, zeigt eine kanadische Studie: Hypertone Druckwerte in Kindheit und Jugend steigern das Risiko für spätere kardiovaskuläre Komplikationen.

Update Pädiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.