The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Deficiency in antisaccade performance has been proposed as a schizophrenia endophenotype. METHOD: The authors assessed performance on an antisaccade task (and a prosaccade control condition) by 10 monozygotic twin pairs discordant for DSM-IV schizophrenia and 10 monozygotic healthy twin pairs matched for age, sex, and parental socioeconomic status. The authors computed antisaccade gain, latency, and error rate, as well as prosaccade gain and latency. RESULTS: The schizophrenic twins made more antisaccade reflexive errors than the nonschizophrenic co-twins and comparison twins, who did not significantly differ from each other. The nonschizophrenic members of discordant pairs performed worse than the comparison twins on antisaccade gain and latency but did not differ from their schizophrenic co-twins on these variables. There were no differences on prosaccade performance. Antisaccade errors were correlated with negative symptoms in the patients. CONCLUSIONS: Antisaccade spatial accuracy and latency deficits may serve as markers of genetic liability for schizophrenia.

The antisaccade task requires the initiation of a rapid eye movement away from a peripheral visual stimulus. Patients with schizophrenia show a higher frequency of reflexive errors than do healthy subjects, as well as longer latency and less spatial accuracy for correct antisaccades (13). Antisaccade performance has been linked to frontal-striatal functioning in healthy individuals and dysfunction in schizophrenia (2, 3). Schizophrenic patients’ first-degree relatives share some of these deficits (1, 4–6), and more than 50% of the variance in antisaccade errors is due to additive genes (7), suggesting that this task taps genetic vulnerability for schizophrenia.

Monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia are an important population in which to test the validity of a putative endophenotype. This population allows one to address whether a candidate endophenotype is associated with the genetic predisposition for the illness, its expression, or both.

To our knowledge, no study of antisaccade performance in monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia has been published. We, therefore, aimed to investigate whether antisaccade deficits are observed in this population. We included an oculomotor control condition, the prosaccade task, on which no between-group differences were expected.

Method

Ethical approval and written informed consent were obtained. The study group comprised 10 Caucasian monozygotic discordant twin pairs and 10 Caucasian monozygotic comparison twin pairs (six male and four female pairs in each group). The exclusion criteria were history of neurological illness, head injury with loss of consciousness for more than 1 minute, and substance misuse or dependence in the last year. One 29-year-old schizophrenic patient (but not her co-twin) was excluded because of strabismus. Zygosity was determined by using a twin likeness questionnaire and assessment of 12 independent and highly polymorphic microsatellite markers. In each of the discordant pairs, one twin (the proband) met the DSM-IV criteria for schizophrenia while the co-twin (the nonschizophrenic co-twin) was free of any psychotic disorder. The probands’ symptoms were rated by using the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS) (8) and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) (9) (intra- and interrater reliability: intraclass correlation coefficient=0.91–0.93). On average, 9.93 years (SD=8.11) had elapsed since the onset of the probands’ illness. The comparison twins were without lifetime DSM-IV axis I diagnoses and were free of family history, to their second-degree relatives, of psychosis (10).

The saccade tasks were identical to those in a previous protocol (11). Testing took place either in the laboratory or at the participants’ homes (five discordant and three comparison pairs). For all pairs, both members were seen in the same environment (laboratory or home). A trial consisted of the target in the center of a computer monitor for a random duration of 1,000–2,000 msec and, subsequently, in one of four peripheral locations (±6°, ±12°) for 1,000 msec. Each task comprised 60 trials. For prosaccades, the participants were instructed to follow the target as accurately as possible. During the antisaccade task, the participants were instructed that while the target was in the center of the screen they should look at the target and that when the target jumped to the side they should look at the location exactly opposite the target, as though at its mirror image. Eye movements were recorded by using infrared oculography sampled at 500 Hz. The data were scored by a rater (U.E.) blind to group status (inter- and intrarater reliability: r=0.91–0.99). The antisaccade data of one male discordant pair could not be used because of poor quality. Saccades were detected by using minimum amplitude (1°), velocity (30°/second), and latency (100 msec) criteria. We calculated latency (in milliseconds), gain (saccade amplitude divided by target amplitude, expressed as a percentage), and for antisaccades, error rate (reflexive saccades divided by total number of valid trials, expressed as a percentage).

Group differences were analyzed by using a regression model. Inferences were based on standard errors that were robust against correlations within twin clusters, variance heterogeneity, and departures from normality (12) (Stata Corp., College Station, Tex.). The model uses group as the independent variable and the saccade measures as the dependent variables. The alpha level for overall group comparisons was 5%. Significant differences were followed by three Bonferroni-adjusted post hoc comparisons (alpha level, 0.05/3=0.017). Gender and age were included as covariates. Effect sizes were defined as the estimated group difference divided by the square root of the average of the variances within the proband and the unaffected co-twin groups. Pearson correlations between the SAPS/SANS scores and the saccade variables were calculated.

Results

The probands’ treatment included clozapine (N=4), olanzapine (N=2), risperidone (N=1), and none (N=2). Their mean SAPS and SANS scores were 5.22 (SD=3.38) and 10.11 (SD=6.79), respectively. Of the nonschizophrenic co-twins, seven had past psychiatric histories: major depressive disorder (N=3), major depression and obsessive-compulsive disorder (N=1), depression and schizotypal personality disorder (N=1), and schizotypal personality disorder (N=2). None had active axis I diagnoses at assessment or was taking medication.

The twin groups did not differ in age (discordant: mean=31.20, SD=9.90; comparison: mean=33.90, SD=12.12) or parental socioeconomic status according to the U.K. National Statistics Socio-Economic Classification (http://www.statistics.gov.uk/methods_quality/ns_sec) (discordant: mean=2.50, SD=0.71; comparison: mean=2.38, SD=0.72) (p>0.36, two-tailed t test).

For the saccadic variables, the normality and homogeneity of variance were assessed by using box plots. While normality appeared a reasonable assumption, variability in antisaccade latency was somewhat higher among the probands, although there were no outliers (defined as having a score more than 1.5 interquartile ranges outside either quartile). The 25th, 50th, and 75th percentiles of antisaccade latency were 270.61, 279.09, and 410.77 msec for the probands; 279.30, 300.47, and 317.72 msec for the co-twins; and 249.76, 270.82, and 302.00 for the comparison subjects.

Significant group effects were obtained for antisaccade gain, latency, and error rate (Table 1). On the antisaccade error rate, the probands differed at a nearly significant level from the healthy comparison twins and significantly from the co-twins. The nonschizophrenic co-twins displayed significantly less (i.e., hypometric) gain and greater latency than the comparison twins. There were no significant group differences on prosaccade performance.

The SANS score was correlated with antisaccade errors (r=0.72, N=8, p=0.05), and the SAPS score was correlated with prosaccade gain (r=–0.74, N=9, p=0.03) (for other correlations, p>0.10).

Discussion

The nonschizophrenic co-twins of schizophrenic patients displayed significant impairments in antisaccade gain and latency. They made antisaccades of hypometric amplitudes (with a large effect size) and had longer latencies (with a medium effect size) relative to the comparison twins, but they did not differ from the probands on these variables. Similar deficits have been demonstrated in schizophrenic patients and their first-degree relatives (1, 4, 5), suggesting that these deficits may serve as schizophrenia endophenotypes in genetic studies. These differences are particularly striking given that all groups displayed comparable prosaccade gain and latency.

The probands’ antisaccade gain and latency deficits (with medium-to-large effect sizes) failed to reach statistical significance, possibly because of the probands’ smaller group and large variance. In the proband group, the error rates were correlated with negative symptom levels (2), and prosaccade gain was correlated with positive symptom levels (11).

The limitations of this study include the fact that we did not study monozygotic twins concordant for schizophrenia or singletons. This might be relevant, given the suggestion that environment may have a particularly strong role in etiology for monozygotic discordant pairs (13). Additionally, the study group was small, because monozygotic discordant twins are rare. Finally, the group differences may have been confounded by the nonschizophrenic co-twins’ past psychiatric diagnoses.

TABLE 1

Presented in part at the 12th Biennial Winter Workshop on Schizophrenia, Davos, Switzerland, Feb. 7–13, 2004. Received April 11, 2005; revision received June 7, 2005; accepted June 20, 2005. From the Division of Psychological Medicine, the Centre for Neuroimaging Sciences, and the Department of Biostatistics and Computing, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; and the Mental Health and Neural Systems Research Unit, Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster, U.K. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Ettinger, Centre for Neuroimaging Sciences, P.O. Box 89, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, U.K.; (e-mail).Supported by the British Academy (grant SG-36241 to Dr. Ettinger), the Wellcome Trust (fellowship 064971 to Dr. Picchioni), and a Leverhulme Trust Early Career Fellowship (Dr. Ettinger).The authors thank the twins who took part in this study for their contribution.

References

1. Karoumi B, Saoud M, d’Amato T, Rosenfeld F, Denise P, Gutknecht C, Gaveau V, Beaulieu FE, Dalery J, Rochet T: Poor performance in smooth pursuit and antisaccadic eye-movement tasks in healthy siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 2001; 101:209–219Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Ettinger U, Kumari V, Chitnis XA, Corr PJ, Crawford TJ, Fannon DG, O’Ceallaigh S, Sumich AL, Doku VC, Sharma T: Volumetric neural correlates of antisaccade eye movements in first-episode psychosis. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:1918–1921LinkGoogle Scholar

3. Munoz DP, Everling S: Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat Rev Neurosci 2004; 5:218–228Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Thaker GK, Ross DE, Cassady SL, Adami HM, Medoff DR, Sherr J: Saccadic eye movement abnormalities in relatives of patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2000; 45:235–244Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Ettinger U, Kumari V, Crawford TJ, Corr PJ, Das M, Zachariah E, Hughes C, Sumich AL, Rabe-Hesketh S, Sharma T: Smooth pursuit and antisaccade eye movements in siblings discordant for schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res 2004; 38:177–184Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Calkins ME, Curtis CE, Iacono WG, Grove WM: Antisaccade performance is impaired in medically and psychiatrically healthy biological relatives of schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Res 2004; 71:167–178Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Malone SM, Iacono WG: Error rate on the antisaccade task: heritability and developmental change in performance among preadolescent and late-adolescent female twin youth. Psychophysiology 2002; 39:664–673Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Andreasen NC: Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City, University of Iowa, 1984Google Scholar

9. Andreasen NC: Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City, University of Iowa, 1983Google Scholar

10. Gershon ES, Guroff JJ: Information from relatives: diagnosis of affective disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1984; 41:173–180Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Ettinger U, Kumari V, Crawford TJ, Flak V, Sharma T, Davis RE, Corr PJ: Saccadic eye movements, schizotypy, and the role of neuroticism. Biol Psychol 2005; 68:61–78Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Binder DA: On the variances of asymptotically normal estimators from complex surveys. Int Stat Rev 1983; 51:279–292CrossrefGoogle Scholar

13. McNeil TF, Cantor-Graae E, Torrey EF, Sjostrom K, Bowler A, Taylor E, Rawlings R, Higgins ES: Obstetric complications in histories of monozygotic twins discordant and concordant for schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1994; 89:196–204Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar