Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Public Health 1/2007

Open Access 01.12.2007 | Research article

Do inattention and hyperactivity symptoms equal scholastic impairment? evidence from three European cohorts

verfasst von: Alina Rodriguez, Marjo-Riitta Järvelin, Carsten Obel, Anja Taanila, Jouko Miettunen, Irma Moilanen, Tine Brink Henriksen, Katri Pietiläinen, Hanna Ebeling, Arto J Kotimaa, Karen Markussen Linnet, Jørn Olsen

Erschienen in: BMC Public Health | Ausgabe 1/2007

download
DOWNLOAD
print
DRUCKEN
insite
SUCHEN

Abstract

Background

Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) affects many children, adolescents, and adults and is associated with a number of impairments. Poor academic performance is related to ADHD in clinical samples. However, it is unclear to what extent core ADHD symptoms and scholastic impairment are related in non-referred school-aged children.

Methods

Data come from three population-based cohorts from Sweden, Denmark, and Finland, which are part of the Nordic Network on ADHD. The combined sample size was 13,087 children who were studied at ages 7–8 or 10–12 years. Teachers rated children on inattention and hyperactivity symptoms and reported children's scholastic performance on basic skills.

Results

There was a significant association in all cohorts between core ADHD symptoms and scholastic impairment in reading, writing, and mathematics. Particularly, inattention was related to a two to tenfold increase in scholastic impairment. Prevalence of hyperactivity symptoms was similar across the three cohorts, but inattention was lowest among children from the Finnish cohort, after stratification on living conditions.

Conclusion

These results extend previous reports of scholastic impairment among children with clinically diagnosed ADHD to non-referred population samples from three European countries. Surveillance policies should be implemented in school systems to catch children in need of behavioral or scholastic support early.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1471-2458-7-327) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

AR participated in the design of the study and drafted the manuscript. KP, JM, and AR performed the statistical analyses. CO provided the Danish, AT and IM the Finnish, and AR the Swedish cohort datasets. JO and TBH, MRJ, and AR supervised the cohort studies in Denmark, Finland, and Sweden respectively. All authors contributed to the manuscript, read and approved the final version.

Background

Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is the most common neurobehavioral disorder in children and adolescents. ADHD is associated with impairment in all aspects of a child's life, i.e. family, social, and academic [1, 2]. Children with ADHD often follow a sustained negative developmental trajectory [3] and impairments can continue into adulthood [4]. ADHD is a concern for public health and policy makers not only due to the wide ranging associated difficulties, but also because it is a substantial economic burden for society in terms of medical treatment [5] and indirect costs related to high risk behaviors [6, 7] and their consequences. Early identification of possible cases would be helpful in the planning of public services.
ADHD is characterized by inattention and hyperactivity symptoms inappropriate for age or developmental level and diagnosis requires symptoms to be associated with considerable impairment. Scholastic underachievement is associated with clinically diagnosed ADHD in children [8] and lower academic attainment and lower socioeconomic status in adults [4]. However, it is unknown whether the relation between scholastic impairment also holds true for core ADHD symptoms in the much larger group of cases that are not clinically diagnosed. If ADHD symptoms are trait-like and continuously distributed in the population as has been previously proposed [9], then we expect that a positive association will be evident between core symptoms and impairment, as is true for diagnosed cases. If the presence of core symptoms, irrespective of diagnosis, is systematically associated with scholastic impairment, then teachers may be key in early identification of children who need support.
Our aim was to investigate the association between core symptoms of ADHD and impairments in basic scholastic skills, i.e. reading, writing, and mathematics in large non-referred samples from three European countries. This approach advances our understanding in several ways. First, we examine the basic academic skills, as opposed to concentrating on reading, which has been previously associated with ADHD. Second, the large sample size enables us to adequately test whether the relations are consistent for girls as well as boys. Third, we have the opportunity to check whether findings replicate in the three participating countries.
We focused on teacher-rated restlessness, fidgetiness, and inattention because these symptoms are basic to the diagnostic criteria listed for ADHD in the DSM-IV [1] and Hyperkinetic Disorder in the International Classification of Diseases (ICD)-10 [10]. Further these symptoms appear in widely used parent, teacher, and self-administered screening instruments (e.g. Rutter Scale; Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire, SDQ; Connors; Child Behavior Checklist, CBCL). ADHD behaviors at school, as opposed to at home, are especially important because previous work examining the full range of symptoms has found that they predict later academic underachievement [11]. Teacher ratings have been found to make a stronger contribution to the prediction of ADHD subtype than parent ratings [12] and have high classification accuracy of ADHD diagnosis [13].
We studied a large sample of children from three population based birth cohorts from Sweden, Denmark, and Finland. These countries share similar cultural traditions, political structures, and tax-paid healthcare, school systems and social services for children. Despite general similarities, local variations exist and this provides an opportunity to examine whether the associations replicate across a large geographic region. Most previous studies of ADHD have been confined to specific geographic locations. We hypothesized that the core ADHD symptoms and scholastic impairment would be positively related in all cohorts. Further, we expected to find cross-national similarity in the prevalence rates of these symptoms.

Methods

Samples

Prospective data originate from three birth cohorts from Sweden, 1st Child in the Family, Denmark, The Aarhus Birth Cohort (ABC), and Finland, Northern Finland 1985–86 Birth Cohort (NFBC 1986), which longitudinally investigate medical and psychosocial endpoints. These cohorts are part of the Nordic Network on ADHD, which the Nordic Ministry of Health established and funded to promote a collaborative scientific effort to study various aspects of ADHD. The cohorts share a number of characteristics that make comparison reasonable, although the studies were not originally designed for cross-national comparison. The Ethics Review Board in each of the respective countries approved the studies.
All cohorts consecutively recruited women in early pregnancy via antenatal health services and achieved high recruitment rates (91% to 99%). Antenatal health care in the three countries is tax-paid. The percentage of pregnant women not receiving this type of antenatal care is less than 0.2% [14]. Routines for antenatal care are standardized within each country and all countries have low perinatal and infant mortality rates [15, 16]. Inclusion criteria were the ability to understand the local language and additionally, in Sweden, nulliparity and Scandinavian origin. Participants received antenatal health care in either Uppsala County (Sweden), Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology in Aarhus (Denmark), or in Oulu or Lapland provinces (Finland). Antenatal care was provided at all levels as appropriate.
At follow-up participants were traced through the national registries using personal identification numbers in each country, enabling us to trace even those who moved outside the original geographic areas, i.e. anywhere within the national borders. Follow-up data collection occurred when children were approximately 7–8 years in Sweden and Finland and 10–12 years old in Denmark. Permission to contact the child's teacher was obtained from parents in all cohorts. All cohorts collected data on child behavior symptoms and scholastic performance from teachers via postal questionnaire.

Variables

Teachers assessed child behavior using the official translations of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) [17] in Sweden and Denmark and the Rutter scale (RB2) [18] in Finland. Scales were completed in full and in accordance with procedures for each instrument in the local language. The SDQ builds on the Rutter scale [19] and are highly correlated [17]. Both have documented reliability and validity [18, 20] and cutoff scores on both instruments discriminate well between children with and without a clinical disorder [18, 20]. SDQ assesses hyperactivity-inattention with a five-item subscale while the Rutter scale uses three items. The three items on the Rutter are essentially equivalent to three of the SDQ hyperactivity-inattention items and are the focus of this paper: SDQ items (nr. 2) restless, (nr. 10) fidgety, and (nr. 15) easily distracted and Rutter B2 items (nr. 1) restless (nr. 3) squirmy, fidgety, and (nr. 16) not able to concentrate. Behavioral descriptions are scored similarly on both instruments: 0 (does not apply), 1 (somewhat true), or 2 (certainly true).
Teachers assessed deficits in reading, writing, and mathematical skills using 7-point and 5-point scales in Sweden and Denmark, respectively, and impaired/unimpaired format in Finland. We dichotomized the scales into below vs. average and above average performance for the purpose of the present study. Previous work shows that teachers using single-item ratings are accurate judges of impairment and their ratings concur with achievements test results [21, 22]. Table 1 summarizes key participant characteristics and measures for the cohorts.
Table 1
Participants and measures
 
Sweden
Denmark
Finland
Initial data collection (years)
1992 – 1994
1990–1992
1985 – 1986
Initiated during gestational week
10
14
12
Geographic area
Uppsala
Aarhus
Northern Finland
 
County
 
(Oulu Lapland Provinces)
Inclusion criteria
nulliparity + Scandinavian origin
all pregnant women
all pregnant women
Sample size
   
   Mothers (% of eligible)
476 (91%)
8010 (98%)
9362 (99%)
   live births
411
8244
9432
Maternal age (Y, sd)
27.0 (4)
28.6(5)
27.8 (6)
Family structure at birth
   
   cohabitated with expectant father
91%
95%
95%
Follow-up data collection
2001–2002
2001
1993–1994
Retained at follow-up
290 (74%)
5039 (61%)
9297 (99%)
Sample size:
   
   Participating eligible teachers1
208 (96%)
4354 (85%)
8525 (92%)
Child age (years)
7–8
10–12
7–8
Child gender (% boys)
49%
51%
51%
Maternal education (%)
   
   secondary
85.7
90.6
90.5
   college/university2
15.2
9.5
9.5
Family structure
   
   two biological parents
78.1%
78.2%
87.8%
   disrupted family3
21.9%
21.8%
12.2%
Inattention & hyperactivity symptoms assessment
SDQ4
SDQ4
CBQ5
Scholastic performance:
   
   writing, reading, mathematics
7-pt scale
5-pt scale
impaired/unimpaired
1 teachers' eligiblity was determined by parental consent
2 four or more years of college/university education or university degree
3 disrupted family encompassed single parent households and reconstructed family with step-parent
4 Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire subscale for hyperactivity-inattention items: (nr. 2) restless, (nr. 10) fidgety, and (nr. 15) easily distracted
5 Children's Behaviour Questionnaire Rutter B2 subscale for hyperactivity-inattention items: (nr. 1) restless (nr. 3) squirmy, fidgety, and (nr. 16) not able to concentrate

Analyses

Equivalent statistical analyses were conducted separately for each cohort using SAS version 8.2 (SAS, Cary, NC, USA). The sum of the three core ADHD symptoms (restlessness, fidgetiness, and inattention) ranged from 0 to 6. We reported the mean sum score by gender across cohorts and examined possible differences using 95% confidence intervals (CI).
The cohorts differed on living conditions, maternal education (equivalent in Danish and Finnish cohorts, but higher in the Swedish) and on family structure at follow-up (Swedish and Danish were equivalent, but percentage of intact families was higher in the Finnish cohort). Such indices of disadvantage have been previously shown to relate to ADHD diagnosis [23]. Therefore, we stratified by these two potentially confounding variables. We checked whether prevalence of severe ratings differed across cohorts and genders after stratification. Maternal education was dichotomized into no university education vs. at least some university education (coded 0 or 1, respectively). Family structure was defined as either intact, i.e. continuously living within the original biological family unit (coded 0), or within a disrupted family (coded 1), i.e. either single-parent or reconstructed family unit including a step-parent.
Multivariate logistic regression models were used to assess the association between core ADHD symptoms and scholastic impairment adjusted for maternal education, family structure, and child gender. We examined whether the sum of symptoms, based on a continuous scale, was associated with impairment among boys and girls in each of the cohorts. We also report the strength of the associations using standardized estimates (called β). We then focused on the most severe ratings, i.e. certainly true because of their clinical relevance in relation to impairment. Core symptoms scores were dichotomized as severe ratings (coded 1) versus lower ratings (somewhat true or not true, coded 0). We combined the two hyperactivity symptoms and required a severe score on both restlessness and fidgetiness to code 1 and 0 represented any other combination. All classifications were done prior to analyses. Separate models were run for each type of symptom (predictor) and each scholastic skill (outcome).

Results

The samples were based on the number of traceable live births in each cohort (rather than on the number of recruited pregnant women) for which parental consent to contact the teacher was obtained and consisted of a total of 13,087 children. Table 1 shows retention at follow-up was considerably lower in Sweden and Denmark (74% and 61%, respectively) than in Finland (92%). Maternal consent to contact teachers in Sweden was obtained for 79% and of these 96% of teachers participated. Attrition analyses for Sweden showed participants were similar to national averages on socioeconomic status and birth outcomes and permission to contact the teacher was not related to maternal ratings of child behavior, gender, and socioeconomic status [24]. In Denmark, 65% of parents provided permission to contact teachers and of these 85% participated. In Finland, the teacher questionnaires were originally sent to the parents who forwarded them to the teachers; 92% of the teachers responded. There were no differences in response rates according to neonatal risk [25].
The unadjusted mean sum score (95% CI) for the three hyperactivity-inattention symptoms for boys in Swedish, Danish, and Finnish cohorts were 1.9 (1.50, 2.30), 1.6 (1.47, 1.63), 1.3 (1.25, 1.35), respectively. The Finnish mean was significantly different from Swedish and Danish means at a probability level of at least .05. Girls' means were 0.7 (0.47, 0.93), 0.6 (0.51, 0.60), 0.5 (0.47, 0.53), respectively and did not differ significantly.
Unadjusted prevalence of high scorers on each of the inattention and hyperactivity core symptoms is presented in table 2 separately for cohort country and gender. A larger portion of boys received a severe score, i.e. a rating of "2" (certainly true) in comparison to girls. The percentage of boys receiving a high score on any core symptom was lowest in the Finnish cohort. Inattention was most prevalent for children in Denmark.
Table 2
Unadjusted percentages of children scoring high (rated as certainly true) on core symptoms per cohort country and gender
 
Boys
Girls
 
Inattention
Fidgetiness
Restlessness
Inattention
Fidgetiness
Restlessness
Sweden
18.3
18.3
12.5
1.0
4.2
1.9
Age 7–8, 2001–2
      
Denmark
21.5
9.3
13.1
7.4
1.6
2.8
Age 10–12 2001
      
Finland
6.2
7.8
10.7
1.5
1.7
2.6
Age 7–8 1993–4
      
Prevalence of hyperactivity symptoms did not differ significantly across cohorts after stratification by maternal education and family structure (detailed data available on request). Approximately 15–20% of boys from disadvantaged homes (low maternal education and disrupted families) were rated high on hyperactivity symptoms in all cohorts. Well below 10% of girls received such a rating. Less than 10% of boys and much less than 5% of girls in the most advantaged strata (high maternal education and intact families) were high on hyperactivity symptoms in all cohorts. However, inattention differed between cohorts. Fewer disadvantaged boys in the Finnish cohort received severe ratings on inattention than in the Danish, 10% vs. 34% respectively. Danish and Swedish cohorts did not differ. Inattention among girls was clearly most prevalent in the Danish cohort in comparison to Finnish cohort, 13% vs. 3%, and there were no differences between the Swedish and Finnish cohorts.
Twenty eight percent of children in the Swedish and Danish cohorts and 22% in the Finnish had impaired scholastic performance on at least one skill. Figure 1 shows the relation between total sum score on the core symptoms (range = 0–6) and the percent of children having at least one scholastic impairment. There was a positive relation in all cohorts. There was greater variability in the Swedish cohort in comparison to the others, presumably due to limited sample size.
In order to determine whether the association between the sum of the core symptoms and impairment applied only to particular skills, we examined the association with each skill separately in each cohort and for each sex. Further, we were also interested in whether the associations held for both boys and girls. Table 3 shows the results adjusted for maternal education, and family structure. Symptoms and impairment were related in all cohorts among both genders. Generally, for each unit increase in symptom scores there was approximately a 50% increase in the risk of impairment. Although, the associations were not significant for the most part in Sweden, as seen from the confidence intervals, the β values indicate that the associations were in the same direction as in the other cohorts. The association for girls seemed particularly weak in Sweden, but this could be explained by the low sample size that makes the estimates less accurate. The association was significantly stronger for girls in both Denmark and Finland. For example, Danish girls had a two-fold increase in risk of impairment in reading (95% CI: 1.8 – 2.2), whereas Danish boys had a 60% increase (95% CI: 1.5 – 1.7).
Table 3
Multiple logistic regression analyses for scholastic impairment by the sum of core ADHD symptoms1 adjusted for maternal education2 and family structure3 for each cohort country
 
Boys
Girls
 
Sweden
Denmark
Finland
Sweden
Denmark
Finland
 
β
OR
95% CI
β
OR
95% CI
β
OR
95% CI
β
OR
95% CI
β
OR
95% CI
β
OR
95% CI
Reading
.24
1.2
1.0, 1.6
.49
1.6
1.5, 1.7
.35
1.5
1.4, 1.5
.18
1.3
.8, 2.2
.41
2.0
1.8, 2.2
.33
1.7
1.6, 1.9
Writing
.48
1.5
1.2, 1.9
.36
1.4
1.3, 1.5
.34
1.4
1.4, 1.5
.15
1.3
.8, 1.9
.35
1.8
1.6, 1.9
.32
1.7
1.6, 1.8
Mathematics
.28
1.1
.9, 1.7
.44
1.5
1.4, 1.6
.40
1.5
1.4, 1.6
.15
1.3
.9, 1.8
.37
1.8
1.7, 2.0
.32
1.7
1.6, 1.8
1the sum of the symptoms ranged from 0–6
2 No university education versus at least some university education
3 Intact biological family versus disrupted (single-parent or step-parent present)
To assess whether severity in either type of symptom was related to impairment, we examined severe ratings as these may have more clinical relevance. Table 4 presents the association between severe ratings of inattention and hyperactivity (high score on both restlessness and fidgetiness) symptoms and each scholastic skill separately adjusted for gender, maternal education, and family structure. Both inattention and hyperactivity were strongly related to scholastic impairment. The relative strength of the associations was consistently stronger between inattention and impairment across all basic skills and cohorts in comparison with hyperactivity. The pattern of results was replicated in each of the cohorts for all of the skills.
Table 4
Multiple logistic regression analyses for scholastic impairment by inattention and hyperactivity1 core symptoms adjusted for maternal education2, family structure3, and gender for each cohort country
 
Sweden
Denmark
Finland
 
Wald χ2
p <
OR
95% CI
Wald χ2
p <
OR
95% CI
Wald χ2
p <
OR
95% CI
Reading impairment
            
Inattention
6
.01
4.2
1.3, 13.0
469
.0001
10.5
8.5, 12.9
245
.0001
7.9
5.8, 9.5
Hyperactivity
1
.25
1.9
0.6, 5.7
118
.0001
3.8
2.9, 4.8
144
.0001
4.4
3.5, 5.7
Writing impairment
            
Inattention
20
.0001
12.5
4.1, 38.1
309
.0001
5.4
4.5, 6.5
220
.0001
6.7
5.2, 8.7
Hyperactivity
11
.001
4.9
2.0, 12.5
73
.0001
2.6
2.1, 3.2
107
.0001
3.5
2.8, 4.5
Mathematics impairment
            
Inattention
4
.05
4.0
1.1, 14.7
304
.0001
7.3
5.9, 9.2
318
.0001
10.6
8.2, 13.7
Hyperactivity
.5
.50
1.5
0.5, 5.1
72
.0001
3.1
2.4, 4.0
176
.0001
5.7
4.4, 7.4
1 High score on both restlessness and fidgetiness (see Analyses section)
2 No university education versus at least some university education
3 Intact biological family versus disrupted (single-parent or step-parent present)

Discussion

In general population based cohorts from three European countries including over 13,000 children, we found a strong and consistent pattern of associations between core ADHD symptoms and scholastic impairment. These results are in line with previous research showing comorbidity between ADHD and learning problems [26, 27], and extend previous findings by confirming an association between core ADHD symptoms and impairment in general population samples. The same associations are similar to what has been shown for clinical cases of ADHD indicating that ADHD diagnoses are just extreme values from a continuous distribution (e.g. like hypertension).
Prior work has focused on literacy skills and found an association with ADHD [28]. However, we found that mathematics was also strongly associated with core symptoms, even when taking into account indices related to disadvantage (maternal education and family structure) and child gender. The association between inattention and scholastic impairment was more pronounced than the association between hyperactivity symptoms and impairment. Similarly, others [29] have found reading difficulties were more strongly associated with the ADHD inattentive than the hyperactive subtype.
Girls had a lower prevalence of severe symptoms in all cohorts, which is in line with the gender disparity in ADHD diagnosis. The larger cohorts showed significantly stronger relative associations between core symptoms and impairment among girls as compared to boys.
A major change in the DSM-IV from earlier versions was the addition of impairment requirement. Symptoms must generate impairment in order to be considered a psychiatric disorder. Several reports show that many children are impaired but do not reach the threshold number of symptoms and, therefore, do not meet the diagnostic criteria [30]. Impairment should weigh heavily in the diagnostic process even when children have fewer symptoms than required [31]. Our data do not allow us to identify children meeting the ADHD diagnosis and many of the children that scored high on the core symptoms we recorded most likely do not meet the full criteria. Still, children who scored high on the core symptoms and have impairment are in need of support whether or not they fulfill diagnostic requirements. Children with psychiatric symptoms are more likely to dropout of secondary school [32] and children with ADHD have poor long-term academic underachievement [33]. Further, scholastic impairment is related to delinquency [34, 35]. Leaving children's needs unmet (behaviorally or pharmacologically) could result in many of them not reaching their full potential.
Initial screening procedures could be conducted in schools as teachers are in a good position to first detect learning problems and notice behavioral deviations because children with ADHD symptoms often disrupt the classroom. Ideally screening for ADHD symptoms could be a part of the school health surveys. Alternatively, teacher ratings on core symptoms could be incorporated into routine progress reports given to parents. Early support in the form of academic assistance, teaching coping strategies to children (e.g. organizational skills training), and teachers' monitoring behaviors in the classroom would be important first steps. This would also facilitate an alliance between teachers and parents to work on shared goals. This strategy is in keeping with a greater emphasis being placed on support rather than assessment [36]. Multimodal interventions alleviate symptoms and help in reducing impairment [37, 38]. Until then, however, parents or clinicians should request this information from teachers and make it available when determining whether further evaluation is warranted. This study is in line with previous research showing the value of teacher and parent rating scales and indicates that teacher ratings of a few core symptoms and impairment are informative and can be an initial step in following the ADHD evaluation guidelines [3941].
Prevalence of core symptoms was related to living conditions in all three cohorts. Our results converge with previous reports showing that disadvantaged children are more likely to receive high ratings on behavioral problems [42, 43]. Thus, policy makers can increase surveillance of ADHD core symptoms and scholastic impairment among disadvantaged children who may be more vulnerable for negative outcomes.
The finding that inattention was lowest in Finland (even after stratification on living conditions) merits further research to discern whether etiological factors or other unmeasured factors related to living conditions are at the root of the observed difference. It is unclear to what extent methodological differences between cohorts could have contributed to the differences.
There are some limitations to consider. First, methodology differed somewhat between the cohorts e.g., data collection took place 6 years earlier in the Finnish cohort. This difference in time of data collection may explain why Finnish children were rated less inattentive than in the other two cohorts. It may be possible that awareness of ADHD symptoms has been steadily increasing within the last decade and may have affected teachers' willingness to endorse symptoms. However, it seems unlikely that increased awareness would pertain only to inattention and not to hyperactivity symptoms. A Swedish community study [44], which collected data at the time of the Finnish data collection (i.e. 6 years earlier), found similar prevalence rates as those presently reported for the Swedish cohort. Our Finnish prevalence rates are practically identical to results found from a later date from a study conducted in another part of Finland [45]. Because Swedish and Finnish children were the same age, differences in inattention rate cannot be attributed to developmental effects. Thus, neither developmental differences nor time effects between cohorts can fully explain prevalence differences.
Second, we were limited to teacher-reported core symptoms and scholastic impairment. It may be that teachers tend to rate a child poorly in one area if he or she is rated poorly in another or that behavioral ratings bias perceptions of scholastic performance, e.g. due to a negative halo effect. However, teachers within each cohort rated various behaviors and outcomes pertaining to well-being and development, consequently the connection between the three core symptoms that we study and scholastic impairment was not likely to have been made. The majority of children in our study were first-graders, therefore, official grades or national test scores are not available with which to compare teacher ratings. Nonetheless, our measure has ecological validity as teachers are in the best position to rate academic performance and do so in reality. Teacher ratings of behavior have been found to reflect age-appropriate evaluations [46]. Symptoms may present differently according to environment and it is suggested that inattention and hyperactivity symptoms are more reliably observed in a school setting [41]. In this respect, core ADHD symptoms at school may be more relevant for scholastic impairment than in other environments. Recently, Caroll and colleagues [28] found that child literacy impairments were related equally to both teacher and parent ratings of ADHD symptoms using the SDQ. Further, there is a school effect related to child performance [47], however, teachers in our samples were not confined to any particular school, but came from large geographic areas.
Third, we related only three core symptoms to scholastic performance rather than clinical diagnosis. Our results show that functional impairment is not only limited to children who fulfill all the clinical criteria for ADHD, but suggest that increases in symptomatology and impairment go hand-in-hand in the general population.
Fourth, we used cross-sectional data although we expect a relation between ADHD symptoms and scholastic impairment to develop over time as has been suggested in clinical studies [3, 48]. It is likely that there is a dual pathway connecting behavioral symptoms and scholastic difficulties. Our follow-ups were initiated during the acquisition of scholastic skills (Swedish and Finnish cohorts). Thus, there may be a greater likelihood for the causal pathway to be from hyperactivity-inattention symptoms to scholastic deficits, which is consistent with findings showing inattention symptoms contribute to later reading difficulties [49].

Conclusion

Despite these limitations, the relation between hyperactivity and inattention symptoms and scholastic impairment was replicated in three general population samples of nonreferred children from different countries, which indicates a robust relation. Symptoms were related to all the skills we investigated suggesting overall scholastic difficulties. Scholastic impairment is a powerful predictor of adverse developmental trajectory.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by Nordic Council of Ministers research program on Longitudinal Epidemiology (NORFA nr. 020056). Cohorts were supported by The Swedish Research Council (345-2004-156), The Academy of Finland (103451), Sigrid Juselius Foundation, Finland, Thule Institute, University of Oulu, Finland, and the Danish Medical Research Council.
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

AR participated in the design of the study and drafted the manuscript. KP, JM, and AR performed the statistical analyses. CO provided the Danish, AT and IM the Finnish, and AR the Swedish cohort datasets. JO and TBH, MRJ, and AR supervised the cohort studies in Denmark, Finland, and Sweden respectively. All authors contributed to the manuscript, read and approved the final version.
Anhänge

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 1994, Washington, DC, 4 American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 1994, Washington, DC, 4
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Busch B, Biederman J, Cohen LG, Sayer JM, Monuteaux MC, Mick E, Zallen B, Faraone SV: Correlates of ADHD among children in pediatric and psychiatric clinics. Psychiatr Serv. 2002, 53 (9): 1103-1111. 10.1176/appi.ps.53.9.1103.CrossRefPubMed Busch B, Biederman J, Cohen LG, Sayer JM, Monuteaux MC, Mick E, Zallen B, Faraone SV: Correlates of ADHD among children in pediatric and psychiatric clinics. Psychiatr Serv. 2002, 53 (9): 1103-1111. 10.1176/appi.ps.53.9.1103.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Biederman J, Faraone S, Milberger S, Guite J, Mick E, Chen L, Mennin D, Marrs A, Ouellette C, Moore P, Spencer T, Norman D, Wilens T, Kraus I, Perrin J: A prospective 4-year follow-up study of attention-deficit hyperactivity and related disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996, 53 (5): 437-446.CrossRefPubMed Biederman J, Faraone S, Milberger S, Guite J, Mick E, Chen L, Mennin D, Marrs A, Ouellette C, Moore P, Spencer T, Norman D, Wilens T, Kraus I, Perrin J: A prospective 4-year follow-up study of attention-deficit hyperactivity and related disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996, 53 (5): 437-446.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Biederman J: Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a selective overview. Biol Psychiatry. 2005, 57 (11): 1215-1220. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.020.CrossRefPubMed Biederman J: Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a selective overview. Biol Psychiatry. 2005, 57 (11): 1215-1220. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.020.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Leibson CL, Katusic SK, Barbaresi WJ, Ransom J, O'Brien PC: Use and costs of medical care for children and adolescents with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA. 2001, 285 (1): 60-66. 10.1001/jama.285.1.60.CrossRefPubMed Leibson CL, Katusic SK, Barbaresi WJ, Ransom J, O'Brien PC: Use and costs of medical care for children and adolescents with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA. 2001, 285 (1): 60-66. 10.1001/jama.285.1.60.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Fischer M, Barkley RA, Smallish L, Fletcher K: Hyperactive children as young adults: Driving abilities, safe driving behavior, and adverse driving outcomes. Accident Analysis & Prevention. 2007, 39 (1): 94-105. 10.1016/j.aap.2006.06.008.CrossRef Fischer M, Barkley RA, Smallish L, Fletcher K: Hyperactive children as young adults: Driving abilities, safe driving behavior, and adverse driving outcomes. Accident Analysis & Prevention. 2007, 39 (1): 94-105. 10.1016/j.aap.2006.06.008.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat DiScala C, Lescohier I, Barthel M, Li G: Injuries to children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 1998, 102 (6): 1415-1421. 10.1542/peds.102.6.1415.CrossRefPubMed DiScala C, Lescohier I, Barthel M, Li G: Injuries to children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 1998, 102 (6): 1415-1421. 10.1542/peds.102.6.1415.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Frick PJ, Kamphaus RW, Lahey BB, Loeber R, Christ MA, Hart EL, Tannenbaum LE: Academic underachievement and the disruptive behavior disorders. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1991, 59 (2): 289-294. 10.1037/0022-006X.59.2.289.CrossRefPubMed Frick PJ, Kamphaus RW, Lahey BB, Loeber R, Christ MA, Hart EL, Tannenbaum LE: Academic underachievement and the disruptive behavior disorders. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1991, 59 (2): 289-294. 10.1037/0022-006X.59.2.289.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Levy F, Hay DA, McStephen M, Wood C, Waldman I: Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997, 36 (6): 737-744. 10.1097/00004583-199706000-00009.CrossRefPubMed Levy F, Hay DA, McStephen M, Wood C, Waldman I: Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997, 36 (6): 737-744. 10.1097/00004583-199706000-00009.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat World Health Organization: The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva. 1992 World Health Organization: The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva. 1992
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Moulton JL: Young adult outcome of children with "situational" hyperactivity: a prospective, controlled follow-up study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2002, 30 (2): 191-198. 10.1023/A:1014761401202.CrossRefPubMed Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Moulton JL: Young adult outcome of children with "situational" hyperactivity: a prospective, controlled follow-up study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2002, 30 (2): 191-198. 10.1023/A:1014761401202.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Power TJ, Doherty BJ, Panichelli Mindel SM, Karustis JL, Eiraldi RB, Anastopoulos AD, DuPaul GJ: The predictive validity of parent and teacher reports of ADHD symptoms. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 1998, 20: 57-81. 10.1023/A:1023035426642.CrossRef Power TJ, Doherty BJ, Panichelli Mindel SM, Karustis JL, Eiraldi RB, Anastopoulos AD, DuPaul GJ: The predictive validity of parent and teacher reports of ADHD symptoms. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 1998, 20: 57-81. 10.1023/A:1023035426642.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Tripp G, Schaughency EA, Clarke B: Parent and teacher rating scales in the evaluation of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: contribution to diagnosis and differential diagnosis in clinically referred children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2006, 27 (3): 209-218. 10.1097/00004703-200606000-00006.CrossRefPubMed Tripp G, Schaughency EA, Clarke B: Parent and teacher rating scales in the evaluation of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: contribution to diagnosis and differential diagnosis in clinically referred children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2006, 27 (3): 209-218. 10.1097/00004703-200606000-00006.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Delvaux T, Buekens P, Godin I, Boutsen M: Barriers to prenatal care in Europe. Am J Prev Med. 2001, 21 (1): 52-59. 10.1016/S0749-3797(01)00315-4.CrossRefPubMed Delvaux T, Buekens P, Godin I, Boutsen M: Barriers to prenatal care in Europe. Am J Prev Med. 2001, 21 (1): 52-59. 10.1016/S0749-3797(01)00315-4.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat World Health Organization: Maternal mortality in 2000 : estimates developed by WHO, UNICEF and UNFPA. 2004, Geneva: Department of Reproductive Health and Research, 1-38. World Health Organization: Maternal mortality in 2000 : estimates developed by WHO, UNICEF and UNFPA. 2004, Geneva: Department of Reproductive Health and Research, 1-38.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat United Nations Children's Fund and World Health Organization: Low birthweight: country, regional and global estimates. 2004, New York: UNICEF United Nations Children's Fund and World Health Organization: Low birthweight: country, regional and global estimates. 2004, New York: UNICEF
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Goodman R: The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997, 38 (5): 581-586. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x.CrossRefPubMed Goodman R: The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997, 38 (5): 581-586. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Rutter M: A children's behaviour questionnaire for completion by teachers: preliminary findings. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1967, 8 (1): 1-11. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1967.tb02175.x.CrossRefPubMed Rutter M: A children's behaviour questionnaire for completion by teachers: preliminary findings. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1967, 8 (1): 1-11. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1967.tb02175.x.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Goodman R: A modified version of the Rutter parent questionnaire including extra items on children's strengths: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1994, 35 (8): 1483-1494. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01289.x.CrossRefPubMed Goodman R: A modified version of the Rutter parent questionnaire including extra items on children's strengths: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1994, 35 (8): 1483-1494. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01289.x.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Goodman R, Ford T, Simmons H, Gatward R, Meltzer H: Using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample. Br J Psychiatry. 2000, 177: 534-539. 10.1192/bjp.177.6.534.CrossRefPubMed Goodman R, Ford T, Simmons H, Gatward R, Meltzer H: Using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample. Br J Psychiatry. 2000, 177: 534-539. 10.1192/bjp.177.6.534.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Fabiano GA, Pelham WE, Waschbusch D, Gnagy EM, Lahey BB, Chronis AM, Onyango AN, Kipp H, Lopez-Williams A, Burrows-MacLean L: A practical impairment measure: Psychometric properties of the Impairment Rating Scale in samples of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and two school-based samples. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2006, 35: 369-385. 10.1207/s15374424jccp3503_3.CrossRefPubMed Fabiano GA, Pelham WE, Waschbusch D, Gnagy EM, Lahey BB, Chronis AM, Onyango AN, Kipp H, Lopez-Williams A, Burrows-MacLean L: A practical impairment measure: Psychometric properties of the Impairment Rating Scale in samples of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and two school-based samples. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2006, 35: 369-385. 10.1207/s15374424jccp3503_3.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Gresham FM, MacMillan DL, Bocian KM: Teachers as "tests": Differential validity of teacher judgements in identifying students at-risk for learning difficulties. School Psychology Review. 1997, 26: 47-60. Gresham FM, MacMillan DL, Bocian KM: Teachers as "tests": Differential validity of teacher judgements in identifying students at-risk for learning difficulties. School Psychology Review. 1997, 26: 47-60.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Schneider H, Eisenberg D: Who receives a diagnosis of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in the United States elementary school population?. Pediatrics. 2006, 117 (4): e601-609. 10.1542/peds.2005-1308.CrossRefPubMed Schneider H, Eisenberg D: Who receives a diagnosis of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in the United States elementary school population?. Pediatrics. 2006, 117 (4): e601-609. 10.1542/peds.2005-1308.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Rodriguez A, Bohlin G: Are maternal smoking and stress during pregnancy related to ADHD symptoms in children?. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005, 46 (3): 246-254. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00359.x.CrossRefPubMed Rodriguez A, Bohlin G: Are maternal smoking and stress during pregnancy related to ADHD symptoms in children?. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005, 46 (3): 246-254. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00359.x.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Yliherva A, Olsen P, Maki-Torkko E, Koiranen M, Jarvelin MR: Linguistic and motor abilities of low-birthweight children as assessed by parents and teachers at 8 years of age. Acta Paediatr. 2001, 90 (12): 1440-1449. 10.1080/08035250152708879.CrossRefPubMed Yliherva A, Olsen P, Maki-Torkko E, Koiranen M, Jarvelin MR: Linguistic and motor abilities of low-birthweight children as assessed by parents and teachers at 8 years of age. Acta Paediatr. 2001, 90 (12): 1440-1449. 10.1080/08035250152708879.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Carlson CL, Tamm L, Gaub M: Gender differences in children with ADHD, ODD, and co-occurring ADHD/ODD identified in a school population. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997, 36 (12): 1706-1714. 10.1097/00004583-199712000-00019.CrossRefPubMed Carlson CL, Tamm L, Gaub M: Gender differences in children with ADHD, ODD, and co-occurring ADHD/ODD identified in a school population. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997, 36 (12): 1706-1714. 10.1097/00004583-199712000-00019.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Schonwald A: Update: attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the primary care office. Curr Opin Pediatr. 2005, 17 (2): 265-274. 10.1097/01.mop.0000156983.71532.eb.CrossRefPubMed Schonwald A: Update: attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the primary care office. Curr Opin Pediatr. 2005, 17 (2): 265-274. 10.1097/01.mop.0000156983.71532.eb.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Carroll JM, Maughan B, Goodman R, Meltzer H: Literacy difficulties and psychiatric disorders: evidence for comorbidity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005, 46 (5): 524-532. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00366.x.CrossRefPubMed Carroll JM, Maughan B, Goodman R, Meltzer H: Literacy difficulties and psychiatric disorders: evidence for comorbidity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005, 46 (5): 524-532. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00366.x.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Willcutt EG, Pennington BF: Comorbidity of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: differences by gender and subtype. J Learn Disabil. 2000, 33 (2): 179-191. 10.1177/002221940003300206.CrossRefPubMed Willcutt EG, Pennington BF: Comorbidity of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: differences by gender and subtype. J Learn Disabil. 2000, 33 (2): 179-191. 10.1177/002221940003300206.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Pickles A, Rowe R, Simonoff E, Foley D, Rutter M, Silberg J: Child psychiatric symptoms and psychosocial impairment: relationship and prognostic significance. Br J Psychiatry. 2001, 179: 230-235. 10.1192/bjp.179.3.230.CrossRefPubMed Pickles A, Rowe R, Simonoff E, Foley D, Rutter M, Silberg J: Child psychiatric symptoms and psychosocial impairment: relationship and prognostic significance. Br J Psychiatry. 2001, 179: 230-235. 10.1192/bjp.179.3.230.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Angold A, Costello EJ, Farmer EM, Burns BJ, Erkanli A: Impaired but undiagnosed. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1999, 38 (2): 129-137. 10.1097/00004583-199902000-00011.CrossRefPubMed Angold A, Costello EJ, Farmer EM, Burns BJ, Erkanli A: Impaired but undiagnosed. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1999, 38 (2): 129-137. 10.1097/00004583-199902000-00011.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Vander Stoep A, Weiss NS, McKnight B, Beresford SAA, Cohen P: Which measure of adolescent psychiatric disorder-diagnosis, number of symptoms, or adaptive functioning-best predicts adverse young adult outcomes?. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2002, 56: 56-65. 10.1136/jech.56.1.56.CrossRefPubMed Vander Stoep A, Weiss NS, McKnight B, Beresford SAA, Cohen P: Which measure of adolescent psychiatric disorder-diagnosis, number of symptoms, or adaptive functioning-best predicts adverse young adult outcomes?. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2002, 56: 56-65. 10.1136/jech.56.1.56.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Todd RD, Sitdhiraksa N, Reich W, Ji THC, Joyner CA, Heath AC, Neuman RJ: Discrimination of DSM IV and latent class attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder subtypes by educational and cognitive performance in a population based sample of child and adolescent twins. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002, 41 (7): 820-828. 10.1097/00004583-200207000-00014.CrossRefPubMed Todd RD, Sitdhiraksa N, Reich W, Ji THC, Joyner CA, Heath AC, Neuman RJ: Discrimination of DSM IV and latent class attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder subtypes by educational and cognitive performance in a population based sample of child and adolescent twins. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002, 41 (7): 820-828. 10.1097/00004583-200207000-00014.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Grigorenko EL: Learning disabilities in juvenile offenders. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2006, 15 (2): 353-371. 10.1016/j.chc.2005.11.001.CrossRefPubMed Grigorenko EL: Learning disabilities in juvenile offenders. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2006, 15 (2): 353-371. 10.1016/j.chc.2005.11.001.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Järvelin M-R, Läärä E, Rantakallio P, Moilanen I, Isohanni M: Juvenile delinquency, education, and mental disability. Exceptional Children. 1995, 61: 230-241. Järvelin M-R, Läärä E, Rantakallio P, Moilanen I, Isohanni M: Juvenile delinquency, education, and mental disability. Exceptional Children. 1995, 61: 230-241.
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Pelham WE, Fabiano GA, Massetti GM: Evidence-based assessment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2005, 34 (3): 449-476. 10.1207/s15374424jccp3403_5.CrossRefPubMed Pelham WE, Fabiano GA, Massetti GM: Evidence-based assessment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2005, 34 (3): 449-476. 10.1207/s15374424jccp3403_5.CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat American Academy of Pediatrics Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Committee on Quality and Improvement: Clinical practice guideline: treatment of the school-aged child with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2001, 108 (4): 1033-1044. 10.1542/peds.108.4.1033.CrossRef American Academy of Pediatrics Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Committee on Quality and Improvement: Clinical practice guideline: treatment of the school-aged child with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2001, 108 (4): 1033-1044. 10.1542/peds.108.4.1033.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Diamond G, Siqueland L: Current status of family intervention science. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2001, 10 (3): 641-661.PubMed Diamond G, Siqueland L: Current status of family intervention science. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2001, 10 (3): 641-661.PubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat American Academy of Pediatrics: Clinical practice guideline: diagnosis and evaluation of the child with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. American Academy of Pediatrics. Pediatrics. 2000, 105 (5): 1158-1170. 10.1542/peds.105.5.1158.CrossRef American Academy of Pediatrics: Clinical practice guideline: diagnosis and evaluation of the child with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. American Academy of Pediatrics. Pediatrics. 2000, 105 (5): 1158-1170. 10.1542/peds.105.5.1158.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor E, Dopfner M, Sergeant J, Asherson P, Banaschewski T, Buitelaar J, Coghill D, Danckaerts M, Rothenberger A, Sonuga-Barke E, Steinhausen HC, Zuddas A: European clinical guidelines for hyperkinetic disorder – first upgrade. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004, 13 (Suppl 1): 17-30. Taylor E, Dopfner M, Sergeant J, Asherson P, Banaschewski T, Buitelaar J, Coghill D, Danckaerts M, Rothenberger A, Sonuga-Barke E, Steinhausen HC, Zuddas A: European clinical guidelines for hyperkinetic disorder – first upgrade. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004, 13 (Suppl 1): 17-30.
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Frazier TW, Youngstrom EA: Evidence-based assessment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: using multiple sources of information. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006, 45 (5): 614-620. 10.1097/01.chi.0000196597.09103.25.CrossRefPubMed Frazier TW, Youngstrom EA: Evidence-based assessment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: using multiple sources of information. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006, 45 (5): 614-620. 10.1097/01.chi.0000196597.09103.25.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Counts CA, Nigg JT, Stawicki JA, Rappley MD, Eye AV: Family adversity in DSM-IV ADHD combined and inattentive subtypes and associated disruptive behavior problems. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005, 44 (7): 690-698. 10.1097/01.chi.0000162582.87710.66.CrossRefPubMed Counts CA, Nigg JT, Stawicki JA, Rappley MD, Eye AV: Family adversity in DSM-IV ADHD combined and inattentive subtypes and associated disruptive behavior problems. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005, 44 (7): 690-698. 10.1097/01.chi.0000162582.87710.66.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Kalff AC, Kroes M, Vles JSH, Hendriksen JGM, Feron FJM, Steyaert J, van Zeben TMCB, Jolles J, van OsJ: Neighbourhood level and individual level SES effects on child problem behaviour: a multilevel analysis. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2001, 55 (4): 246-250. 10.1136/jech.55.4.246.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kalff AC, Kroes M, Vles JSH, Hendriksen JGM, Feron FJM, Steyaert J, van Zeben TMCB, Jolles J, van OsJ: Neighbourhood level and individual level SES effects on child problem behaviour: a multilevel analysis. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2001, 55 (4): 246-250. 10.1136/jech.55.4.246.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Smedje H, Broman JE, Hetta J, von Knorring AL: Psychometric properties of a Swedish version of the "Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire". Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999, 8 (2): 63-70. 10.1007/s007870050086.CrossRefPubMed Smedje H, Broman JE, Hetta J, von Knorring AL: Psychometric properties of a Swedish version of the "Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire". Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999, 8 (2): 63-70. 10.1007/s007870050086.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Almqvist F, Kumpulainen K, Ikaheimo K, Linna SL, Henttonen I, Huikko E, Tuompo-Johansson E, Aronen E, Puura K, Piha J, Tamminen T, Rasanen E, Moilanen I: Behavioural and emotional symptoms in 8–9-year-old children. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999, 8 (Suppl 4): 7-16. 10.1007/PL00010698.CrossRefPubMed Almqvist F, Kumpulainen K, Ikaheimo K, Linna SL, Henttonen I, Huikko E, Tuompo-Johansson E, Aronen E, Puura K, Piha J, Tamminen T, Rasanen E, Moilanen I: Behavioural and emotional symptoms in 8–9-year-old children. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999, 8 (Suppl 4): 7-16. 10.1007/PL00010698.CrossRefPubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Nolan EE, Gadow KD, Sprafkin J: Teacher reports of DSM-IV ADHD, ODD, and CD symptoms in schoolchildren. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001, 40 (2): 241-249. 10.1097/00004583-200102000-00020.CrossRefPubMed Nolan EE, Gadow KD, Sprafkin J: Teacher reports of DSM-IV ADHD, ODD, and CD symptoms in schoolchildren. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001, 40 (2): 241-249. 10.1097/00004583-200102000-00020.CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Sellström E, Bremberg S: Is there a "school effect" on pupil outcomes? A review of multilevel studies. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006, 60: 149-155. 10.1136/jech.2005.036707.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Sellström E, Bremberg S: Is there a "school effect" on pupil outcomes? A review of multilevel studies. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006, 60: 149-155. 10.1136/jech.2005.036707.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
48.
Zurück zum Zitat McGee R, Partridge F, Williams S, Silva PA: A twelve-year follow-up of preschool hyperactive children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1991, 30 (2): 224-232. 10.1097/00004583-199103000-00010.CrossRefPubMed McGee R, Partridge F, Williams S, Silva PA: A twelve-year follow-up of preschool hyperactive children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1991, 30 (2): 224-232. 10.1097/00004583-199103000-00010.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ: Attention deficit and reading achievement. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1992, 33 (2): 375-385. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00873.x.CrossRefPubMed Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ: Attention deficit and reading achievement. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1992, 33 (2): 375-385. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00873.x.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Do inattention and hyperactivity symptoms equal scholastic impairment? evidence from three European cohorts
verfasst von
Alina Rodriguez
Marjo-Riitta Järvelin
Carsten Obel
Anja Taanila
Jouko Miettunen
Irma Moilanen
Tine Brink Henriksen
Katri Pietiläinen
Hanna Ebeling
Arto J Kotimaa
Karen Markussen Linnet
Jørn Olsen
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2007
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Public Health / Ausgabe 1/2007
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2458
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-7-327

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2007

BMC Public Health 1/2007 Zur Ausgabe