Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth 1/2022

Open Access 01.12.2022 | Research article

Maternal infections during pregnancy and child cognitive outcomes

verfasst von: Janell Kwok, Hildigunnur Anna Hall, Aja Louise Murray, Michael Vincent Lombardo, Bonnie Auyeung

Erschienen in: BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth | Ausgabe 1/2022

Abstract

Background

Maternal prenatal infections have been linked to children’s neurodevelopment and cognitive outcomes. It remains unclear, however, whether infections occurring during specific vulnerable gestational periods can affect children’s cognitive outcomes. The study aimed to examine maternal infections in each trimester of pregnancy and associations with children’s developmental and intelligence quotients. The ALSPAC birth cohort was used to investigate associations between maternal infections in pregnancy and child cognitive outcomes.

Methods

Infection data from mothers and cognition data from children were included with the final study sample size comprising 7,410 mother-child participants. Regression analysis was used to examine links between maternal infections occurring at each trimester of pregnancy and children’s cognition at 18 months, 4 years, and 8 years.

Results

Infections in the third trimester were significantly associated with decreased verbal IQ at age 4 (p < .05, adjusted R2 = 0.004); decreased verbal IQ (p < .01, adjusted R2 = 0.001), performance IQ (p < .01, adjusted R2 = 0.0008), and total IQ at age 8 (p < .01, adjusted R2 = 0.001).

Conclusion

Results suggest that maternal infections in the third trimester could have a latent effect on cognitive development, only emerging when cognitive load increases over time, though magnitude of effect appears to be small. Performance IQ may be more vulnerable to trimester-specific exposure to maternal infection as compared to verbal IQ. Future research could include examining potential mediating mechanisms on childhood cognition, such as possible moderating effects of early childhood environmental factors, and if effects persist in future cognitive outcomes.
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
ALSPAC
Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children
DQ
Development Quotient
IQ
Intelligence Quotient
GMDS
Griffiths Mental Development Scales
WPPSI-RUK
Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence – Revised UK edition
WISC-III
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd edition

Background

Research has suggested links between a mother’s immune system during pregnancy and fetal growth and brain development [1, 2]. Central nervous system infections in pregnancy, such as herpes simplex virus and rubella, have been associated with intrauterine growth restriction, prematurity, and long-term neurodevelopmental deficits or disability [3]. Maternal prenatal bacterial infections have been linked with lower mean IQ scores at age 7 [4], and viral infections have shown associations with decreased IQ scores at age 7 with strong effect sizes [5] and learning disabilities [6]. In addition to reported effects on children’s cognition [7] and socioemotional difficulties [8], maternal infections have also been linked to a variety of psychiatric conditions such as bipolar disorder and schizophrenia [9, 10]. These studies demonstrate associations between maternal infections and general difficulties in cognition, behavior, and psychopathology.
The occurrence of viral maternal infection is proposed to influence the pathogenesis of infant central nervous system abnormalities via placental transfer-mediated vertical transmission [11]. This transmission may further disrupt the fetus’ organ and brain tissue development, negatively affecting the infant’s immune system, resulting in further pathology of a compromised immune system and increased risk of childhood inflammation [2]. Childhood low-grade inflammation then activates a systemic immune response associated with negative effects on brain development and function, with research showing small to moderate effect sizes on general intelligence [12]. These processes reflect multiple potential mechanisms of harm acting upon the child when maternal infection is present during pregnancy.
Even though trimester-specific associations have been reported for maternal infections and fetal neurodevelopment [5], evidence is still lacking on whether cognitive outcomes depend on the specific timing of maternal infection exposure during gestation. The idea of fetal neurodevelopment being stage-dependent is based on the fact that brain myelination only begins during the second trimester of pregnancy with further growth reinforcements in the third trimester, followed by postnatal growth [10, 13]. Therefore, inflammation such as infections during specific gestation periods may differentially affect the fetal growth process. However, only a limited number of studies have found that maternal inflammation during later gestational periods negatively affects a child’s cognitive-developmental infrastructure [14] and functional connections such as working memory [15].
Current research shows converging evidence between animal and human studies, with a review of large cohort studies suggesting a causal relationship between prenatal maternal inflammation and child outcomes [16], particularly with effects on the fetal brain development [17]. Examining the links between prenatal infection at different stages of gestation and cognitive outcomes is, however, subject to significant methodological challenges. In particular, it is not possible to conduct randomized experiments in humans for obvious ethical reasons. This makes it difficult to attribute causality to a factor such as prenatal infection. On the other hand, animal studies in which experimental manipulations are possible have been criticized for potential problems with generalization to humans [18]. As such, the field must rely on triangulation across these different evidence sources with complementary strengths and weaknesses [1921].
Despite the fact that human studies must rely on observational evidence, they can provide valuable predictive information about which children may be most at risk of poorer cognitive outcomes. For example, identifying an association between prenatal infection at a particular stage of pregnancy and poorer cognitive ability can help identify children who may benefit from a higher level of monitoring and intervention early in life, such as screening for learning difficulties or prioritization for early-life preventive interventions. This study seeks to utilize observed evidence to provide predictive evidence of mother-fetal associations, despite limitations in examining specific underlying processes or mechanisms.
The aim of this study is to use a cohort sample to investigate whether the occurrence of maternal infections in each trimester of pregnancy is associated with children’s verbal, performance, and total IQ scores.

Methods

Setting

Data for this project were from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC), a population-based cohort study based in Avon, United Kingdom (UK), focusing on pregnant women with expected dates of delivery 1st April 1991 to 31st December 1992 [15, 22]. Regular follow-up clinic assessments were conducted over the years to track development over time. The ALSPAC dataset is a large, nationally representative sample of parents and children from the UK. Detailed medical information were gathered from pregnant women and their children from 8 weeks of gestation up to 22 years of age. Participants in ALSPAC have a slightly higher sociodemographic profile on average as compared to the general population in Avon and Great Britain [23]. For example, ALSPAC mothers are more likely to be married, live in owner-occupied accommodation, have a car in their household, and less likely to be non-White.

Participants

The final sample of data collected and used in this study was for when the children were eight years old was n = 15,645. For the purposes of this study, participants with missing infection data across all trimesters (n = 2,125) were excluded from the sample, as well as those who had missing outcome data across all cognitive domains (n = 6,110). The final sample size comprised n = 7,410 mother-child participants.

Outcome measures

Prenatal infections

Data on prenatal infections from the first, second and third trimesters were gathered in weeks 18 and 32 of gestation, and 8 weeks post-partum [23]. Retrospective assessment was completed at 8 weeks after birth for infections occurring in the third trimester of pregnancy. Women were asked whether they experienced any of the following infections: urinary tract infection, influenza, rubella, thrush, genital herpes, or other infections. Response options provided at 18 weeks were: ‘Yes, in 1st to 3 months’, ‘Yes, 4 months to now’, ‘Yes for both time periods’ and ‘Not at all’. Response options at week 32 and 8 weeks postpartum were: ‘Yes’, ‘No’, and ‘Don’t Know’. Response options for infections reported occurring during the first, second, or third trimester of pregnancy were coded and termed as ‘any trimester’. Infection data were coded into dichotomous variables, where any incidence of infection was coded as a ‘yes’, and no incidence was coded as a ‘no’. Infections were further coded according to trimesters, with any occurrence of infection coded as present (e.g., rubella or urinary tract infection at 18 weeks = infection present in the first trimester).

Childhood IQ scores

Developmental and cognitive measures included the Griffiths Mental Development Scales (GMDS), administered at 18 months, the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence – Revised UK edition (WPPSI-RUK), administered at 4 years, and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd edition (WISC-III), administered at 8 years. The GMDS evaluates early development and consists of five subscales (locomotion, personal/social skills, hearing and speech, hand-eye coordination, and performance) [24]. The hearing and speech scale (language, response, speech development) [25] and the performance scale (skill development and manipulation), comprising 86 items each, were selected for use in the current study. Standardised developmental quotients were calculated through this formula: Developmental Quotient (DQ) = (Mental age x 100) / Chronological age. A new average GMDS development score variable was then calculated using the mean scores across all 5 scales by the ALSPAC team. All variables were age-adjusted by the ALSPAC research team. The study further included two measures of cognitive functioning: WPPSI-RUK for children aged 3–7 years [26] and a short-form version of the WISC-III for children aged 6–17 years [27, 28]. Both tests comprise two scales: verbal and performance (non-verbal ability and executive function), with each scale containing five subtests. Scores on both the WPPSI-RUK and the WISC-III range from 40 to 160. All IQ scores were used as continuous variables. Descriptive statistics for verbal IQ, performance IQ, and fullscale IQ scores are provided in Table 1.

Confounders and covariates

Data on other perinatal and social factors which may be associated with prenatal infections and/or children’s cognitive outcomes were gathered from assessments from mothers and children. Child factors included child sex (male/female), gestational age (weeks) and birthweight (grams). Standard cutoffs for prematurity were applied to gestational age (≥ 37 weeks/<37 weeks) and birthweight (≥ 2500 g/<2500 g) as per clinical standards of falling within the 10th percentile of pregnancies, with increased infant mortality risk [29, 30]. Maternal and socioeconomic factors included maternal age at birth, maternal education, maternal history of smoking, maternal psychiatric history, and deprivation indices. Maternal education was assessed at 32 weeks of gestation through interviews and categorized as A-levels or lower. Maternal smoking was coded as ‘yes' or ‘no' and assessed through questionnaires for every trimester of pregnancy (18 weeks, 32 weeks, 8 weeks postpartum for the third trimester). Maternal psychiatric history was assessed in the first trimester, where women were asked if they had any psychiatric conditions (anorexia, bulimia, severe depression, schizophrenia, other psychiatric issues), and coded as yes if they indicated having any psychiatric illness. Deprivation indices were sorted into quintile ranks and measured by the official Indices of Multiple Deprivation, an area-level deprivation index measured in England accounting for several domains of deprivation: income, employment, health, education, housing, and living environment [31].
Previous literature has identified these factors to be associated with children’s cognitive development [3234]. These factors were classified into potential confounders and covariates for the regression models in this study, with confounders being selected based on whether previous literature showed associations between infection exposure and cognitive outcomes (such as deprivation), and covariates (such as child’s sex) selected based on their potential association with children’s cognitive outcomes. Adjusting for the latter was of interest to determine the unique contribution of infection exposure over and above other established influences.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted using R (version 3.4.1) [35]. Data was inspected for and met parametric statistical assumptions. Linear regression models were used to examine the relations between infections occurring at each trimester and children’s cognitive scores at ages 18 months, 4 years, and 8 years. Three types of models were fit: an unadjusted model, a model adjusted for confounders, and a model adjusted for confounders and additional covariates. These models were run in a hierarchical manner, with increasingly adjusted models specified by adding variables to models from previous steps. The fully adjusted model included possible confounders of maternal and socioeconomic factors, and the additional covariates of child sex, gestational age, birthweight, maternal psychiatric history, and maternal smoking (Table 2). As the models were trimester-specific, regression analyses for infections in each trimester were also adjusted for infections in the other two trimesters.
In the primary analysis, missing data were handled with full information maximum likelihood estimator, and an alpha level of 0.05 indicated statistical significance (p < .05). Sensitivity analysis included multiple imputation models using the MICE package in R, with 25 imputations applied in order to avoid Monte Carlo error [36, 37]. Models were fit from the multiple imputed data sets, with estimates pooled into a single set of estimate and standard errors, which was then used for the analysis.

Results

Table 1
Descriptive statistics for predictors and outcomes
 
N
%
 
Maternal Prenatal Infections
   
 Any time during pregnancy ( = 6,849)
   
   No
3107
45.4
 
   Yes
3742
54.6
 
 1st trimester ( = 6,889)
   
   No
5288
76.8
 
   Yes
1601
23.2
 
 2nd trimester (n = 6,860)
   
   No
4468
65.1
 
   Yes
2392
34.9
 
 3rd trimester (n = 7,091)
   
   No
5588
78.8
 
   Yes
1503
21.2
 
 Child scores
Mean
SD
Range
 GMDS (Mean age = 1.5 years)
   
   Hearing /Speech (Verbal)
100
16.1
62–114
   Performance
113
12.7
64–135
   Average
108
8.8
65–114
 WPPSI-RUK (Mean age = 4.1 years)
   Verbal Scale
100
13.6
54–152
   Performance Scale
108
14.6
55–151
   Fullscale
104
14.2
52–113
 WISC-III (Mean age = 8.6 years)
   Verbal Scale
107
16.7
46–155
   Performance Scale
100
17.1
46–151
   Total Scale
104
16.5
45–151
Table 2
Confounders and covariates (n = 7,410)
Confounders
  
 
Mean (SD)
Range
Maternal age at childbirth (years)
29.1 (1.23)
15–44
 
N
%
Maternal education
  
  (≥ A’ levels)
3068
41.3
  (< A’ levels)
4111
55.5
  Missing
231
3.2
Deprivation index
  
  Highest quintile
1965
26.5
  60–80%
1162
15.7
  40–60%
1054
14.2
  20–40%
885
11.9
  Lowest quintile
765
10.3
  Missing
1579
21.3
Covariates
  
Child Gender
  
  Male
3740
50.4
  Female
3670
49.6
Child Gestation
  
  ≥ 37 weeks
6982
94.2
  < 37 weeks
428
5.8
Child Birthweight
  
  ≥ 2500 g
6970
94.0
  < 2500 g
349
4.7
  Missing
91
1.3
Maternal psychiatric history
  
  No
5955
80.4
  Yes
829
11.2
  Missing
626
8.4
Maternal smoking (any point in pregnancy)
  
  No
5073
68.5
  Yes
1488
20.1
  Missing
849
11.4
Table 3
Hierarchical regression models on trimester-based infections in pregnancy and cognitive outcomes (n = 7,410)
   
Any trimester
1st trimester
2nd trimester
3rd trimester
   
B values ( SE )
Model 1 a
GMDS
Hearing & Speech (verbal)
− 1.55 (.985)
− 0.429 (1.20)
− 1.32 (1.15)
− 0.940 (1.31)
  
Performance
− 0.099 (.778)
0.029 (.943)
− 0.191 (.901)
− 0.177 (1.03)
  
Average
− 0.854 (.543)
− 0.647 (.655)
− 0.623 (.626)
− 0.297 (.717)
 
WPPSI-RUK
Verbal
− 1.45 (.887)
− 0.547 (1.07)
− 0.051 (1.02)
− 2.51 (1.16) *
  
Performance
− 0.438 (.951)
0.647 (1.15)
0.430 (1.10)
− 0.810 (1.25)
  
Fullscale
− 1.07 (.923)
0.037 (1.11)
− 0.061 (1.07)
− 1.95 (1.21)
 
WISC-III
Verbal
− 0.287 (.418)
0.945 (.509)
− 0.120 (.482)
− 1.48 (.562) **
  
Performance
− 0.378 (.427)
0.449 (.517)
− 0.062 (.491)
− 1.49 (.571) **
  
Total
− 0.368 (.412)
0.796 (.501)
− 0.116 (.475)
− 1.64 (.553) **
Model 2 b
GMDS
Hearing & Speech (verbal)
− 0.150 (.148)
− 1.09 (1.38)
− 1.54 (1.33)
− 0.915 (1.55)
  
Performance
− 0.228 (.885)
− 0.169 (1.06)
0.078 (1.02)
− 0.606 (1.19)
  
Average
− 0.637 (.605)
− 0.913 (.729)
− 0.313 (.706)
− 0.530 (.820)
 
WPPSI-RUK
Verbal
− 1.37 (.960)
− 1.14 (1.15)
− 1.42 (1.10)
− 1.75 (1.26)
  
Performance
− 0.508 (1.04)
0.163 (1.25)
− 0.116 (1.20)
− 0.490 (1.38)
  
Fullscale
− 1.04 (.993)
− 0.614 (1.19)
− 0.871 (1.14)
− 1.30 (1.31)
 
WISC-III
Verbal
− 0.063 (.431)
0.317 (.523)
− 0.262 (.498)
− 0.859 (.584)
  
Performance
− 0.192 (.457)
0.151 (.552)
0.228 (.524)
− 1.65 (.615) **
  
Total
− 0.139 (.425)
0.289 (.515)
− 0.070 (.489)
− 1.34 (.574) *
Model 3 c
GMDS
Hearing & Speech (verbal)
− 1.71 (1.22)
− 1.21 (1.44)
− 0.349 (1.39)
− 1.61 (1.64)
  
Performance
− 0.132 (.952)
− 0.077 (1.11)
0.396 (1.08)
− 0.315 (1.27)
  
Average
− 0.612 (.642)
− 0.791 (.756)
0.286 (.734)
− 0.565 (.862)
 
WPPSI-RUK
Verbal
− 1.08 (1.05)
0.852 (1.22)
− 0.983 (1.17)
− 1.64 (1.36)
  
Performance
− 0.167 (1.13)
0.682 (1.32)
− 0.028 (1.27)
− 0.448 (1.47)
  
Fullscale
− 0.692 (1.08)
− 0.150 (1.26)
− 0.540 (1.21)
− 1.24 (1.40)
 
WISC-III
Verbal
0.304 (.467)
0.415 (.557)
− 0.036 (.531)
− 0.603 (.623)
  
Performance
0.172 (.493)
0.259 (.587)
0.496 (.559)
− 1.41 (.655) *
  
Total
0.255 (.459)
0.412 (.548)
0.188 (.522)
− 1.34 (.574)
Linear regression analyses and beta coefficients for trimester-based infections in pregnancy and cognitive outcomes
*p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001
a Unadjusted models
b Adjusted for potential confounds of maternal education, maternal age, deprivation, infections in other trimesters
c Adjusted for confounds, and additional covariates of maternal smoking, maternal psychiatric history, gestation, birth weight, and child sex
Descriptive statistics showed that the occurrence of maternal prenatal infections at any time during pregnancy was 54.6%, with most infections occurring during the 2nd trimester (34.9%) (Table 1). Mean developmental quotient and intelligence quotient scores were similar to the general population (GMDS = 108, WPPSI-RUK = 104, WISC-III = 104) (Table 1). Table 2 shows further descriptive statistics for maternal variables and children’s birth outcomes.
Unadjusted models found no links between infections occurring in the first or second trimesters and children’s outcomes. Significant associations were found between third trimester infections and WPPSI-RUK verbal scores (β= − 0.076, p = .031) at age 4, and WISC-III verbal IQ scores (β= − 0.036, p = .008), performance IQ scores (β= − 0.036, p = .009), and total IQ scores (β= − 0.040, p = .003) at age 8 (Table 3). These results showed that an infection during the third trimester of pregnancy was associated with a 2.5 IQ point decrease for WPPSI-RUK verbal scores at age 4, 1.5 IQ point decrease in WISC-III verbal scores, 1.5 IQ point decrease for WISC-III performance scores, and 1.6 IQ point decrease for WISC-III total scores at age 8. No significant associations were found between prenatal infections in all three trimesters and any of the children’s cognitive outcomes (Model 1).
Significant associations were attenuated after the first adjustment for confounders maternal age, maternal education, deprivation, and infections in the other two trimesters (Model 2). Associations were no longer found for WPPSI-RUK verbal IQ scores (β= − 0.053, p = .166) at 4 years and WISC-III verbal IQ scores (β= − 0.021, p = .141) at 8 years. Significant associations remained between third-trimester infections and WISC-III performance IQ scores (β= − 0.039, p = .007) and WISC-III total IQ scores (β= − 0.033, p = .020) at 8 years (Table 3). These results showed that an infection during the third trimester of pregnancy was associated with a 1.7 IQ point decrease in WISC-III performance scores, 1.3 IQ point decrease for WISC-III total scores at age 8.
Fully adjusted models (Model 3) with confounders (maternal age, maternal education, deprivation indices, other two trimesters) and additional covariates (maternal smoking, maternal psychiatric history, gestation, birth weight, and child sex) showed that infections in the third trimester were associated with lower WISC-III performance scores at 8 years (β= − 0.033, p = .032) (Table 3). These results represent a 1.4 IQ point decrease in WISC-III performance IQ scores.

Sensitivity analysis

A sensitivity analysis of the study models found consistent associations between infections occurring at the third trimester and outcomes for WISC-III verbal IQ scores (β= -1.33, p = .022), performance IQ scores (β= -1.19, p = .028), and total IQ scores (β= -1.39, p = .009) at age 8 in the unadjusted model. While no significant associations were shown between occurrence of infections at any trimester and cognitive outcome scores for the partially and fully adjusted models, lower maternal education (p < .05) and highest deprivation (p < .05) were generally seen to have associations with lowered cognitive scores across all trimesters.

Discussion

A prospective birth cohort was used to examine associations between maternal infections occurring at each trimester of pregnancy and children’s developmental and intelligence scores at 18 months, 4 years, and age 8. Results from unadjusted models provided evidence for maternal infections during the third trimester being associated with verbal IQ scores at age 4, and verbal, performance, and total IQ scores at age 8. The associations between third-trimester infections and cognitive outcomes were seen to be attenuated after adjustment for confounders, with significant associations seen only for performance and total IQ scores at age 8 after the first adjustment. Further adjustment with additional covariates left only significant associations for performance IQ scores at age 8. A similar prevalence of maternal infections during pregnancy was shown in this study as compared to other cohort samples [3, 18].
Children’s cognitive outcomes appear to depend on critical periods in gestation, with evidence pointing to effects of infection limited to later gestation. Animal models have provided evidence on potential mechanisms, showing that inflammatory cytokines produced in response to infection may be linked to fetal brain development changes. Mice treated with analogues of bacterial or viral infections at different gestational periods showed different neurodevelopmental profiles of hippocampal reelin and GAD67 cell number expression in the hippocampal dorsal or ventral stratum oriens of offspring [18]. Studies investigating infection-associated immunological events on the fetus showed early maternal inflammation to be linked with negative effects on the development of a fetus’ dopaminergic system at multiple levels such as cell distribution and connectivity [38]. In contrast, later brain insults during pregnancy have been linked to deficits in cell organization and maturation of synapses which occur over an extended period, affecting cognitive function [14].
While animal models provide a possible explanation as to how brain neurochemistry and structure can potentially be affected by different times of infection exposure, evidence from human studies, especially looking at specific trimester effects and cognitive outcomes, is lacking. Further, while some genetic patterns have been found in maternal intellectual ability and child IQ scores [39], no firm conclusions can be drawn that suggests genetics fully accounts for children’s cognitive abilities. Instead, there is current growing emphasis on using the gene-environment interaction to examine the role of maternal inflammation and biological pathways that lead to fetal brain development and cognition [40, 41].
Literature on human studies has been mixed, showing varying results on vulnerability to inflammation occurring in different gestational periods, especially on a child’s cognitive development [10, 13, 15]. The significant associations between third trimester infection and cognitive outcomes at age 8 identified in this study suggest a possible latent effect of maternal systemic inflammation which may possibly influence cognitive development. This could be due to an interaction with unaccounted environmental factors after birth, such as lowered socioeconomic status, which could confound child development as it is also considered an inflammatory process during childhood [42]. The link with maternal infections during later gestation shows that despite having fetal brain infrastructure in place, environmental factors such as deprivation could still affect brain structure or functional connections after birth.
This suggests that infections in late pregnancy may influence the postnatal processes of cognitive development. With differences shown in results using the developmental (GMDS) and intelligence quotients (WPPSI-RUK, WISC-III), it could be possible that effects of prenatal maternal infection are not evident in early development, with no identification of potential developmental delay as reflected in GMDS. However, results from formal IQ assessments such as the WPPSI-RUK and WISC-III could possibly be showing effects that emerge only under increased cognitive load, with cognitive IQ assessments created to assess advanced skills such as abstract reasoning or working memory.
While this study could not directly infer causality, study results are still consistent with previous research on maternal-fetal associations, where clinical evidence shows how presence of maternal inflammation during pregnancy using elevated cytokine profiles contribute to prenatal risk programming [43, 44]. Other studies using these same maternal cytokines found negative influences on prenatal central nervous system development [45], with animal models showing further evidence for negative neurobehavioral outcomes [46]. In sum, research supports how prenatal programming consists of multiple variables such as occurrence and regulation of inflammation, genetic susceptibility, and environmental stressors; all of which interact in a complex manner that is still not yet fully understood in humans [16], which could explain why study results were mixed. Nevertheless, this study contributes further evidence through the specific scope of how maternal infections during different trimesters of pregnancy could possibly affect childhood developmental and intelligence quotient outcomes.

Strengths and limitations

Main study strengths were the use of a large high-quality longitudinal design and a well-characterized sample. While cohort studies such as the ALSPAC tend to have considerable attrition due to reasons such as loss to follow-up related to socioeconomic status, it has been shown that differences in estimates when comparing full or restricted cohorts tended to be small [47]. Similar infection rates during pregnancy and study attrition allowed for comparability with other cohort studies also looking at mother-child associations. Study limitations include a reliance on mother self-reported infection data; that the records of infections were not classified by severity; and that causes of infections (e.g. bacterial/viral/fungal) were not recorded and could not be stratified by pathogen type for analysis. In addition, while the study has included a list of confounders and covariates, not all factors such as parental attitudes, styles or other variables related to the quality of care of the child, or genetic factors could be accounted for in this study. In addition, this version of GMDS was created to identify developmental delays and therefore should be interpreted with caution as to whether it is a true assessment of cognition at 18 months. Despite not being able to account for these above factors, study results still showed predictive associations for the cognitive measures assessing IQ, which was consistent with research from both animal and human studies focusing on maternal-fetal interactions. Finally, assessments from the dataset were only measured up to age 8, so lasting effects of cognitive development could not be analyzed beyond those years.

Conclusion

Our study highlighted possible third-trimester specific effects of maternal infection on children’s cognitive abilities. However, effect sizes were small, suggesting a weak link between maternal infections during the third trimester of pregnancy and childhood cognitive outcomes. One potential direction of future research could include examining mechanisms which mediate these effects of prenatal infections on childhood cognition such as fetal brain gene expression after maternal immune activation [48]. Other research could also explore temporal dependency and specificity to performance IQ scores from 4 years onwards, whether the effect of infection is moderated by specific early childhood environmental factors, and if its effect persists in future cognitive outcomes.

Acknowledgements

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurse. We further thank the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program for their generous support.

Declarations

Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees, with informed consent for the use of data collected via questionnaires and clinic records obtained from participants following the recommendations of the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee at the time. Ethics approval was obtained from the Bristol and Weston Health Authority: E1808, and the United Bristol Healthcare Trust: E4445 ALSPAC Focus at Eight. Further information can be found on the ALSPAC study website: http://​www.​bristol.​ac.​uk/​alspac/​researchers/​our-data/​ or http://​www.​bristol.​ac.​uk/​alspac/​researchers/​research-ethics/​.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

All authors declare no competing interests and have approved of the manuscript for submission to undergo full editorial and peer review.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Mulkey SB, Bulas DI, Vezina G, Fourzali Y, Morales A, Arroyave-Wessel M, Swisher CB, Cristante C, Russo SM, Encinales L, Pacheco N, Kousa YA, Lanciotti RS, Cure C, DeBiasi RL, du Plessis AJ. Sequential Neuroimaging of the Fetus and Newborn With In Utero Zika Virus Exposure. JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(1):52–9.CrossRefPubMed Mulkey SB, Bulas DI, Vezina G, Fourzali Y, Morales A, Arroyave-Wessel M, Swisher CB, Cristante C, Russo SM, Encinales L, Pacheco N, Kousa YA, Lanciotti RS, Cure C, DeBiasi RL, du Plessis AJ. Sequential Neuroimaging of the Fetus and Newborn With In Utero Zika Virus Exposure. JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(1):52–9.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Griffiths R. The abilities of young children: A comprehensive system of mental measurement for the first eight years of life. London: Child Development Research Centre; 1970. Griffiths R. The abilities of young children: A comprehensive system of mental measurement for the first eight years of life. London: Child Development Research Centre; 1970.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D. Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence—Revised. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1989. Wechsler D. Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence—Revised. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1989.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd ed.). (WISC-III): Manual. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1991. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd ed.). (WISC-III): Manual. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1991.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D. Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions. London: The Psychological Corporation; 1993. Wechsler D. Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions. London: The Psychological Corporation; 1993.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Eskes M, Ensing S, Groenendaal F, Abu-Hanna A, Ravelli ACJ. The risk of intrapartum/neonatal mortality and morbidity following birth at 37 weeks of gestation: a nationwide cohort study. BJOG. 2019;126:1252–7.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Eskes M, Ensing S, Groenendaal F, Abu-Hanna A, Ravelli ACJ. The risk of intrapartum/neonatal mortality and morbidity following birth at 37 weeks of gestation: a nationwide cohort study. BJOG. 2019;126:1252–7.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
35.
Zurück zum Zitat R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013.
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Buuren S. Flexible Imputation of Missing Data. 1st ed. Hoboken: Taylor and Francis; 2012.CrossRef Van Buuren S. Flexible Imputation of Missing Data. 1st ed. Hoboken: Taylor and Francis; 2012.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Royston P. Multiple imputation of missing values. STATA J. 2004;4(3):227–41.CrossRef Royston P. Multiple imputation of missing values. STATA J. 2004;4(3):227–41.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Maternal infections during pregnancy and child cognitive outcomes
verfasst von
Janell Kwok
Hildigunnur Anna Hall
Aja Louise Murray
Michael Vincent Lombardo
Bonnie Auyeung
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2022
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth / Ausgabe 1/2022
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2393
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12884-022-05188-8

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2022

BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth 1/2022 Zur Ausgabe

Antikörper-Wirkstoff-Konjugat hält solide Tumoren in Schach

16.05.2024 Zielgerichtete Therapie Nachrichten

Trastuzumab deruxtecan scheint auch jenseits von Lungenkrebs gut gegen solide Tumoren mit HER2-Mutationen zu wirken. Dafür sprechen die Daten einer offenen Pan-Tumor-Studie.

Mammakarzinom: Senken Statine das krebsbedingte Sterberisiko?

15.05.2024 Mammakarzinom Nachrichten

Frauen mit lokalem oder metastasiertem Brustkrebs, die Statine einnehmen, haben eine niedrigere krebsspezifische Mortalität als Patientinnen, die dies nicht tun, legen neue Daten aus den USA nahe.

S3-Leitlinie zur unkomplizierten Zystitis: Auf Antibiotika verzichten?

15.05.2024 Harnwegsinfektionen Nachrichten

Welche Antibiotika darf man bei unkomplizierter Zystitis verwenden und wovon sollte man die Finger lassen? Welche pflanzlichen Präparate können helfen? Was taugt der zugelassene Impfstoff? Antworten vom Koordinator der frisch überarbeiteten S3-Leitlinie, Prof. Florian Wagenlehner.

Gestationsdiabetes: In der zweiten Schwangerschaft folgenreicher als in der ersten

13.05.2024 Gestationsdiabetes Nachrichten

Das Risiko, nach einem Gestationsdiabetes einen Typ-2-Diabetes zu entwickeln, hängt nicht nur von der Zahl, sondern auch von der Reihenfolge der betroffenen Schwangerschaften ab.

Update Gynäkologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert – ganz bequem per eMail.