Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry 1/2012

Open Access 01.12.2012 | Research article

When combat prevents PTSD symptoms—results from a survey with former child soldiers in Northern Uganda

verfasst von: Roland Weierstall, Inga Schalinski, Anselm Crombach, Tobias Hecker, Thomas Elbert

Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2012

Abstract

Background

Human beings from time immemorial have eradicated neighbouring tribes, languages, religions, and cultures. In war and crisis, the cumulative exposure to traumatic stress constitutes a predictor of the development of post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). However, homicide has evolved as a profitable strategy in man, leading to greater reproductive success. Thus, an evolutionary advantage of perpetrating violence would be eliminated if the exposure to aggressive acts would traumatize the perpetrator. We argue that perpetrating violence could actually ‘immunize’ a person against adverse effects of traumatic stressors, significantly reducing the risk of developing PTSD.

Methods

We surveyed 42 former child soldiers in Northern Uganda that have all been abducted by the Lord Resistance Army (LRA) as well as 41 non-abducted controls.

Results

Linear regression analyses revealed a dose–response effect between the exposure to traumatic events and the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale (PDS) sum score. However, the vulnerability to develop trauma related symptoms was reduced in those with higher scores on the Appetitive Aggression Scale (AAS). This effect was more pronounced in the formerly abducted group.

Conclusions

We conclude that attraction to aggression when being exposed to the victim’s struggling can lead to a substantial risk-reduction for developing PTSD.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1471-244X-12-41) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

RW: principal investigator, data analysis and manuscript preparation. IS: supervision and training of local psychologists. TH & AC: data analysis and manuscript preparation, local trainers. TE: study design, manuscript preparation and supervisor. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Background

It is estimated that at any one time, 300.000 children under the age of 18 are used as soldiers in warfare [1]. Technological advantages like the development of lightweight automatic weapons have resulted in an increase of child soldiers in armed conflicts [2]. From the mid-90’s to 2006 the Lord Resistance Army (LRA) has been fighting in Northern Uganda and is responsible for the mutilation and mass execution of civilians and the abduction and recruitment of children as soldiers in the armed rebel force [3]. Since the LRA relocated its residence to southern Sudan and the Democratic Republic of Congo, several studies have investigated the impact of the war in Northern Uganda and reported devastating effects on the mental health of former child soldiers, especially because of the development of trauma-related symptoms [47]. Several studies reported a clear dose-effect relationship between the exposure to different stressful life events and the risk of development of post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in various especially war-affected populations [811]). Thus, the cumulative exposure to life-threatening events increases the risk of development of PTSD over time. The concept of perpetration-induced traumatic stress (PITS) was introduced to describe an observable phenomenon that former perpetrators show a higher degree of PTSD [1214]. However, homicide may have evolved as a profitable strategy in man, leading to greater reproductive success and a greater evolutionary benefit of those who perpetrate aggressive acts [15, 16]. But such an evolutionary advantage of violent behavior would be eliminated if the exposure to violence cues that are related to their offensive behavior traumatized the perpetrator.
We argue that perpetrating violent acts could actually help to ‘immunize’ a person against adverse effects of traumatic stressors and significantly reduce the risk of developing PTSD [17]. During the course of evolution, not only the purposeful hunting of animals but potentially also of humans may mediate powerful social and psychological rewards and hence interact with the vulnerability of psychological disorders [17]. Most of the research on interspecies aggression stems from primates and has proven that the elimination of neighboring tribes facilitates food supply as well as reproduction and is beneficial for the adults health [18]. Similarly, in-group co-operation and out-group enmity is beneficial for the fitness of the own group in humans [19]. In research on aggression, this instrumental use of aggression for these secondary rewards have been extensively studied. Instrumental aggression has been linked to the secondary rewards, like the gain of social status [20, 21], wealth [22], or reproductive success [23]. However, the mutilation of the victims has not been defined as a primary goal of instrumental aggression yet [24, 25]. Nell [26] proposed an innate and strongly male-gendered human blood lust that is either expressed by phenomena like bystanders or game hunting in peaceful regions or culminates in the purposeful mutilation of victims in war-affected regions. However, the rewarding effects of cruelty have received only little attention in aggression research. We therefore started to investigate appetitive aggression, i.e. the perpetration of violence and/or the infliction of harm to a victim for the purpose of experiencing violence-related enjoyment, empirically. Recently, we showed in a path-analytic model involving Rwandan prison inmates who were all accused for crimes related to the 1994 genocide that a higher appetitive aggression was associated with a reduced risk to develop trauma-related symptoms [17]. To validate this finding and gain further insight into the underlying pathways that contribute to potential trauma-related illness in perpetrators, we surveyed male former child soldiers that have joined the Ugandan LRA. As we expect a higher degree of attraction to violence in men [2628] this study focused on male participants. We hypothesized that high levels of appetitive aggression could promote resilience from trauma-related symptoms. The victim’s struggling, their blood and vocalization were defined as the essential rewarding cues for the perpetrator. To control for the impact of socialization the time spend in the bush was added as a covariate to the analyses. Moreover, we supposed that the personally committed delinquency would predict the extend of appetitive aggression.

Methods

Participants

Interviews were carried out with 83 male participants in the age range of 17 to 27 years. The 42 participants of the abducted group have all been formerly abducted by the LRA and spent between 2 days and 12 years in the bush (median = 3 months). To compare our results with a non-abducted control-group, we interviewed 41 young men of comparable age that all have experienced the war in Northern Uganda but who have not spent time as a child soldier in the bush (i.e. less than one day of abduction). Table 1 shows demographic data of the two groups. Chi square tests did not reveal any difference in the observed demographic variables (Chi square tests for age, number of displacements, education and marital status all p > 0.05).
Table 1
Demographic Data
 
Abducted (n = 42)
Non-Abducted/abducted less than 24 hours (n = 41)
Age, mean (SD), [range]
21.5 (2.47) [17-26]
21.3 (2.51) [18-27]
Age at time of the first abduction
10.7 (3.86) [2-18]
-
No. (%) abducted once abducted more then once*
37 (88.1%)
-
5 (11.9%)
-
Total duration being abducted (in weeks)
93.5 (153.07) [0.2-624]
-
Period since the last demobilization (mean, (SD), [range] in years)
8.7 (4.2) [1-17]
-
No. (%) of Displacements
1 (2.4%)
0
0
9 (21.4%)
9 (22%)
1
10 (23.8%)
15 (36.6%)
2
8 (19%)
7 (17.7%)
3
13 (31.2%)
9 (22%)
 > 4missing
1 (2,4%)
0
Education, No. (%)
13 (31.2%)
15 (36.6%)
No school, some primary school
  
Primary school
5 (11.9%)
4 (9.8%)
Vocational School
4 (9.5%)
5 (12.2%)
Some Secondary School
14 (33.3%)
11 (26.8%)
Secondary school
6 (14.3%)
6 (14.6%)
Marital status, No. (%)
  
single
24 (57.1%)
25 (61%)
married
6 (14.3%)
5 (12.2%)
partner/cohabiting
12 (28,6%)
10 (24,4%)
divorced
0
1 (2,4%)
*Note. Some participants were abducted more than once, i.e. the LRA sometimes released some of the abducted children after a few days and only used them to carry goods in the bush after a village had been robbed.
The Ethical Review board of the University of Konstanz and the Uganda National Council for Science and Technology approved the study and all participants gave their informed consent. For two participants under the age of 18 the caretakers gave informed consent. Participants received financial compensation of 4,000 USh (Ugandan Shilling). No data set was excluded from the analysis.

Procedure

The survey was conducted in a camp for internally displaced people (IDP) in Pabbo, Northern Uganda. Semi-structured interviews were carried out in Acholi with the help of five local counselors, who have all been trained in the concepts of mental disorders and aggression in a two-month training [29]. Different interpreter translated all questionnaires into Acholi and back into English. The translations were discussed in detail with the interpreters before the application in the interview.

Measures

Post traumatic symptom severity

Diagnosis of PTSD and trauma symptom severity in participants was assessed with the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale (PDS), validated Acholi version [29, 30]. The PDS is a 17-item questionnaire widely used for the diagnosis of PTSD as well as the assessment of PTSD symptom severity. A recent study [31] has proven a good agreement in African conflict populations between the assessment of PTSD symptoms with the PDS and expert clinicians interviews with the CAPS [32], the international gold standard. Each item corresponds to one of the PTSD symptoms specified in DSM–IV and ratings range from 0 (“never”) to 3 (“5 times per week or more/very severe/nearly always”). The PDS assesses the frequency of current PTSD symptoms in the past four weeks related to the most stressful life event. However, the frequency is only rated when the severity of a symptom has clinical significance. Therefore, we used the PDS in a semi-structured clinical interview to assure a valid assessment of PTSD symptoms. For data analysis we used the PDS sum score.

Traumatic event types

Traumatic event types experienced were assessed with a checklist of 34 war- and non-war-related potentially life-threatening events, e.g. injury by weapon, rape, accidents, which also included the events from the PDS [10]. The number of times a specific event had been experienced was not assessed, as distorted memory in PTSD renders this measure unreliable [3336]. In a previous study in Uganda, the event list showed a high test-retest reliability (r = 0.73, p < 0.001) and significant accordance with the CIDI Event List [29]. For the analysis we further distinguished between the sum of self-experienced event types, i.e. events where the life of the participants was in acute danger and the sum of witnessed event types, i.e. where the participants witnessed another person in acute life threat.

Duration in the bush

As the duration spent in the bush could have been a potential predictor for the development of PTSD, we assessed the duration of weeks spend in the bush.

Appetitive aggression scale

We assessed a person’s propensity towards perpetrating aggressive acts using the Appetitive Aggression Scale (AAS; [37]), a semi-structured interview for the assessment of a person’s propensity towards violence that has been validated with over 1.600 ex-combatants and that has proven its good psychometric properties. It contains questions regarding the appetitive perception of aggression. Each item is scored on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (“I totally disagree“) to 4 (“I totally agree“). The items are based on the definition of the instrumental aggression subtype according to Vitiello & Stoff [38] and the ICD–10 addiction criteria. Further items were compiled on basis of interviews with perpetrators about the appetitive experience of perpetrating aggressive acts. Cronbach’s Alpha coefficient as a measure for reliability of the Scale was .85. In a principal-axis factoring analysis, all items loaded statistically significant onto a single factor accounting for 32% of the total variance. Moreover, further analyses have revealed that the scale measures a distinct construct of human aggression (for further details see [37]).

Number of types of offenses

As a further measure for self-committed aggressive behavior, we assessed the number of types of offenses from a list of 16 different types of offenses ranging from physical assault to rape or killings.

Data analysis

We used linear regression analysis to explore the association between prognostic variables and the trauma symptom (PDS sum) score. The statistical modeling and analysis was carried out using R for Mac OSX Version 2.11.1 [39].

Results

The characteristics of the development of traumatization for both groups

Only three of the 83 participants (two in the abducted group) fulfilled the full DSM–IV criteria for a PTSD-diagnosis. However, those who were formerly abducted had higher mean PDS sum scores (M = 6.6, SD = 5.6) than the non-abducted control group (M = 3.5, SD = 4.9) (t(81) = 2.73, p = 0.008, d = 0.60). Moreover, the two groups differed in the number of experienced as well as witnessed event types, with formerly abducted participants having been exposed to more life-threatening event types (self-experienced event types: M abducted = 10.71, SD abducted = 3.08; M non-bducted = 4.98, SD non-abducted = 2.10, t(81) = 9.91, p < 0.001, d = 2.17). (Witnessed event types: M abducted = 9.81, SD abducted = 2.05; M non-bducted = 7.29, SD non-abducted = 2.25, t(81) = 5.33, p < 0.001, d = 1.17). The abducted group experienced aggression to be more appetitive than the non-abducted group (AAS sum score: M non-abducted = 13.76, SD non-abducted = 11.54; M abducted = 20.79, SD abducted = 15.94, t(81) = 2.30, p = 0.024).
We performed a multiple linear regression analysis to evaluate the factors contributing to the development of trauma symptoms. PDS sum score in the full model was regressed on the number of self-experienced event types, the number of witnessed event types, their squared values, the AAS sum score and all possible two-way interactions. To mitigate multicollinearity, the predictor variables were mean-centered for calculations of interaction terms [40]. For differences in the two sub groups, group was dummy-coded with 0 for non-abducted and 1 for abducted. As stepwise regression analysis is no useful tool for model selection [41], Akaike Information Criterion was used to estimate model fit [42, 43]. For the AIC, all possible competing forced entry regression models are ranked according to their AIC, with the one having the lowest AIC being the best fitting, as the loss of information based on this model is the smallest.
The selected model (see Table 2) included the predictors AAS, self-experienced event types, the group dummy, as well as the two interactions self-experienced event types * AAS and group * AAS (R adj 2 = 0.43, p < 0.001). The effect sizes for the model as well as the power were calculated using the program G*Power 3 and were sufficient (f 2 = 0.75, (1-) = 1.00) [44]. The model revealed that the number of self-experienced event types was a strong predictor for the development of trauma related symptoms. AAS was no additive predictor. However, as implied by the interplay of the two interactions and the group effect, those who reported an appetitive perception of aggression showed less trauma symptoms, whereby this effect was greater in participants of the abducted group. Moreover, this inverse relation was only crucial as long as the number of self-experienced event types did not exceed a certain threshold.
Table 2
Results of regression analyses predicting PDS sum score (N = 83)
 
PDS sum score
 
β
p
self experienced event types
0.87
< 0.001
AAS
−0.01
n.s.
group
- 0.41
0.004
self experienced event types*AAS
0.38
0.025
group*AAS
- 0.36
0.027
Radj 2
0.43
< 0.001
Note. Uncorrected standardised regression coefficients are displayed.
Figure 1 illustrates the interplay between the three predictors. It shows the fitted PDS sum score values that lie in the convex hulls from the observed predictor values in the data sets of the two groups. The two groups are plotted separately for a better graphic representation. The quartiles for the fitted PDS sum scores were calculated from the observed predictor values of the entire sample to enable comparability of the grey scales between the two groups. As can be seen in the abducted group compared to the non-abducted group, there is a near perfect vertical change in grey-scale, i.e., the greater the aggression score, the less the trauma-related symptoms for each class of trauma exposure. However, as can be seen for the abducted group, as trauma exposure increases, ever greater AAS scores are needed, in order to not allow trauma symptoms to develop. Even at high levels of exposure to traumatic stressors there were only two respondents in the abducted group, who actually presented the full clinical picture for PTSD. Consequently, we found a decrease in the vulnerability for trauma related symptoms with an increased duration in the bush, when the correlation between trauma symptoms and the duration in the bush is controlled for the number of self-experienced event types. As Figure 1 also shows, as the number of self-experienced event types increases, the attraction to violence also increases. Interestingly, there are no participants with high number of self-experienced event types but only a low attraction to violence which raises the question to what extent exposure to violence may promote appetitive aggression in the survivor.
Table 3 shows the comparison between the expected PDS sum scores for the two groups, depending on whether a participant would have an AAS sum score and a number of self-experienced event types from the lower or upper quartile of each subgroup. As can be seen, one would await some trauma symptoms to be present in the non-abducted group for the lower quartile of the number of self-experienced event types; either they have a high or low attraction to violence. However, in the abducted group a high AAS sum score would predict that no trauma symptoms should be present, when the number of self-experienced traumatic event types lies at the lower quartile of this group. This effect decreases, when the number lies at the upper quartile, i.e. the number of traumatic event types increases.
Table 3
Predicted PDS sum scores for the lower and upper quartiles of the Appetitive Aggression Scale score (AAS) and number of self-experienced event types, depending on group membership
Non-abducted group
 
 
self-experienced event types
AAS
Lower quartile = 4
Upper quartile = 7
Lower quartile = 4
3.9
6.1
Upper quartile = 25
0 (−0.2)
5.5
Abducted group
 
 
self-experienced event types
AAS
Lower quartile = 9
Upper quartile = 13
Lower quartile = 7
6.7
9.8
Upper quartile = 38
0 (−0.3)
6.3
Note. The fitted PDS sum score for the lower quartile SET and upper quartile AVE in the abducted group falls below 0, which is a result of the underlying model; i.e. one would expect no trauma symptoms (“0”) in these participants.
Further analyses of the residuals revealed that the proposed model fulfills all necessary quality criteria for linear regression analyses. The maximum Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) did not exceed 3.9 so that multicollinearity may be neglected. The residuals did not differ significantly from normal distribution (Kolmogorov–Smirnov Z = .97, p = .300). Moreover, there was no severe influence of outliers in the proposed models (maximum Cook’s d of .199). However, there was no predictive value of witnessed traumatic event types that could have increased the model fit. To proof that the selected model fitted the data best, we tested whether the second best model according to AIC would improve model fit. However, neither the R 2-change, nor the beta value for the additional predictor (β self experienced event types*group = − .72) turned statistically significant (p > .05).
Finally, the AAS sum score was regressed on the number of witnessed event types, number of types of offenses, as well as their squared values and all possible two-way interactions. The dummy variable group was added for the analyses of potential group differences. Remarkably, the only significant predictor in all possible models was committed delinquency with an adjusted R 2 of 0.52 (p < 0.001), i.e. the reported offences are strongly related to the AAS sum score.

Discussion

In this study, we examined the relationship between appetitive aggression assessed with the AAS and trauma-related symptoms in a sample of formerly abducted child soldiers in Northern Uganda and a non-abducted control group. The Appetitive Aggression Scale [37] assesses the extend of appetitive experience with the perpetration of violence or the infliction of harm that is aimed to experience violence-related enjoyment by the exposure to violence cues such as the struggling of the victim. Appetitive aggression was present at least to some degree in both the abducted and the non-abducted group, except that the extent was more substantial in the formerly abducted group. We identified the number of personally committed crimes to be the best and only significant predictor of AAS sum score in both groups, without significant differences in this relationship between the two groups. This relationship is well explained by assumptions of a cycle of violence: abused children become murderers and perpetrators of other crimes of violence [45, 46]. The present results indicate “that violence breeds violence” not simply in the form of reactive aggressive acts but fosters appetitive aggression, which in turn increases the likelihood of violent offenses. Participants in the formerly abducted group experienced more trauma-related symptoms than participants in the non-abducted group which would be expected, given their much greater traumatic stress. However, PTSD related symptoms were reduced or even absent in those who reported violent acts as more positive. The resilience against mental illness in those who report a high propensity towards violence has also been found for perpetrators of the Rwandan genocide [17]. However, there is no ultimate resilience because the protection wanes as the exposure to traumatic stressors exceeds a certain threshold.
Studies on Vietnam Veterans have reported a high degree of PTSD in former soldiers [47]. Even professional torturers reported trauma-associated symptoms and Milgram reported that participants in his study on obedience reported distress when they applied electroshocks [48, 49]. Proponents of the PITS-concept suggest that “the human mind… is not well set up for killing” and use the results cited before for their line of argument [12]. We also found a higher PTSD symptom severity in those who perpetrated violence compared to those who were not involved in armed conflicts. However, the conclusion that can be drawn from this result takes on a different connotation when one considers the underlying mechanisms leading to traumatization. Baumeister and Campbell [50] emphasize that “to understand the psychology of perpetrators, it may be necessary to distance oneself from the victim’s view”, as the perception of violence differs remarkably between the perpetrator’s and victim’s perspective [51]. The same seems to be true for the mechanisms leading to the development of trauma-related symptoms. Moreover, to understand why some perpetrators are traumatized while others engage in killings without remarkable distress caused by their own atrocities, we propose that a differentiated view of the perception of cruelty could be crucial. The appeal of violence does not seem to override the strong mechanism of the increasing risk for trauma-related mental illness with a cumulative exposure to threats to one’s life [8, 10]. However, it can lead to a substantial risk-reduction. Moreover, it has to be emphasized that only the self-experienced but not the witnessed traumatic event types lead to trauma-related symptoms. Thus, as was indicated by army screening attempts in World War II to sort out those who are vulnerable for “stress disorders”, perpetrators differ in their relative risk of being traumatized [52]. In addition, we would not expect perpetrators to be more affected by the exposure to life-threatening situations in a qualitative manner as suggested by the PITS concept but rather the frequency of the exposure to different life-threatening situations has to be taken into account to draw inference about traumatization in perpetrators. Given the correlational nature of the cross-sectional study, the observed relationship between aggression and traumatic stressors could be also explained by the assumption that those who experience aggression to be more appetitive engage more frequently in severe atrocities. Thus, the likelihood for the exposure to life-threatening situations increases. This explanation is in line with studies that repeatedly demonstrated that the engagement in violence also increases the risk of personal victimization [5355]. A second explanation can be traced back to the sample characteristics. It has to be emphasized that two types of participants are missing in this sample: Participants with a high number of self-experienced event types and low AAS scores and participants with high AAS scores and a low number of self-experienced event types. We assume that the former ones suffer from more severe PTSD symptoms in line with the model and the risk for PTSD associated mortality would be high too [56, 57]. Thus, either this subgroup could not be obtained in a convenient community sample or it became a victim of fatal casualties. Contrary, PTSD symptoms should not be present in the latter ones. We therefore assume that they might still be engaged in fightings of the LRA.

Limitations

We collected the data retrospectively. Even if one third of the population in Uganda experienced the abduction of one of their children [58] and it is estimated that (according to the Survey of War Affected Youths (SWAY)) the Lord’s Resistance Army abducted more than 66,000 children (SWAY 2008 cited in [59]:14), it could be assumed that the LRA very good at selecting more aggressive children so those who stayed in the bush for more than two days were they actually more prone to aggression. Therefore, there could have been differences between the two groups prior to the time in the bush. In would be of great interest for future studies not only to ask for the current attraction to violence but also for the attraction to violence in the period prior to abduction. Another limitation of the sampling that has to be considered concerns the factors involved that compel former child soldiers to escape from the bush. The LRA is still engaged in severe armed conflicts in southern Sudan and the DRC. It could be speculated that those who have a high amount of appetitive aggression and a low number of self-experienced event types, i.e. subgroup in which we would expect trauma-related symptoms to be absent or negligible, are still eagerly involved in manhunt activities and thus unlikely to return to civil society.

Conclusions

The present results confirm a model that suggests that in an environment dominated by organized violence, exposure to traumatic stressors increases appetitive aggression and furthers violent behavior, thus driving a cycle of violence. The increasing appetitive aggression on the other hand seems to relate to a smaller vulnerability for trauma-related mental illness, although there seems to be no ultimate resilience. Therefore, we predict that mental illness will be a likely consequence for those entrapped in the cycle of violence. Future studies have to show if this result can be generalized to other measures of trauma-spectrum disorders.
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

RW: principal investigator, data analysis and manuscript preparation. IS: supervision and training of local psychologists. TH & AC: data analysis and manuscript preparation, local trainers. TE: study design, manuscript preparation and supervisor. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Anhänge

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Brett R: Contribution for Children and Political Violence. WHO Global Report on Violence, ‘Child Soldiering: Questions and Challenges for Health Professionals. 2000, 1- Brett R: Contribution for Children and Political Violence. WHO Global Report on Violence, ‘Child Soldiering: Questions and Challenges for Health Professionals. 2000, 1-
2.
Zurück zum Zitat MacMullin C, Loughry M: Investigating Psychosocial Adjustment of Former Child Soldiers in Sierra Leone and Uganda. J Refug Stud. 2004, 17 (4): 472-537.CrossRef MacMullin C, Loughry M: Investigating Psychosocial Adjustment of Former Child Soldiers in Sierra Leone and Uganda. J Refug Stud. 2004, 17 (4): 472-537.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Acker F: Uganda and the Lord’s Resistance Army: The new order no one ordered. Afr Aff. 2004, 103 (412): 355-357.CrossRef Van Acker F: Uganda and the Lord’s Resistance Army: The new order no one ordered. Afr Aff. 2004, 103 (412): 355-357.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Bayer CP, Klasen F, Adam H: Association of trauma and PTSD symptoms with openness to reconciliation and feelings of revenge among former Ugandan and Congolese child soldiers. JAMA. 2007, 298 (5): 555-559. 10.1001/jama.298.5.555.CrossRefPubMed Bayer CP, Klasen F, Adam H: Association of trauma and PTSD symptoms with openness to reconciliation and feelings of revenge among former Ugandan and Congolese child soldiers. JAMA. 2007, 298 (5): 555-559. 10.1001/jama.298.5.555.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Derluyn I, Broekaert E, Schuyten G, Temmerman ED: Post-traumatic stress in former Ugandan child soldiers. Lancet. 2004, 363 (9412): 861-863. 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)15734-6.CrossRefPubMed Derluyn I, Broekaert E, Schuyten G, Temmerman ED: Post-traumatic stress in former Ugandan child soldiers. Lancet. 2004, 363 (9412): 861-863. 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)15734-6.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Pham PN, Vinck P, Stover E: Returning home: Forced conscription, reintegration, and mental health status of former abductees of the Lord’s Resistance Army in Northern Uganda. BMC Psychiatry. 2009, 9: 23-10.1186/1471-244X-9-23.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pham PN, Vinck P, Stover E: Returning home: Forced conscription, reintegration, and mental health status of former abductees of the Lord’s Resistance Army in Northern Uganda. BMC Psychiatry. 2009, 9: 23-10.1186/1471-244X-9-23.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Schauer E, Elbert T: The Psychological Impact of Child Soldiering. Trauma Rehabilitation After War and Conflict. Edited by: Martz E. 2010, Springer, , 311-360.CrossRef Schauer E, Elbert T: The Psychological Impact of Child Soldiering. Trauma Rehabilitation After War and Conflict. Edited by: Martz E. 2010, Springer, , 311-360.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Catani C, Jacob N, Schauer E, Kohila M, Neuner F: Family violence, war, and natural disasters: A study of the effect of extreme stress on children’s mental health in Sri Lanka. BMC Psychiatry. 2008, 8 (33): -CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Catani C, Jacob N, Schauer E, Kohila M, Neuner F: Family violence, war, and natural disasters: A study of the effect of extreme stress on children’s mental health in Sri Lanka. BMC Psychiatry. 2008, 8 (33): -CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Catani C, Schauer E, Onyut PL, Schneider C, Neuner F, Hirth M, et al: Prevalence of PTSD and Building-Block Effect in School Children of Sri Lanka’s North-Eastern Conflict Areas. 2005, Stockholm, Sweden Catani C, Schauer E, Onyut PL, Schneider C, Neuner F, Hirth M, et al: Prevalence of PTSD and Building-Block Effect in School Children of Sri Lanka’s North-Eastern Conflict Areas. 2005, Stockholm, Sweden
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Neuner F, Schauer M, Karunakara U, Klaschik C, Robert C, Elbert T: Psychological trauma and evidence for enhanced vulnerability for posttraumatic stress disorder through previous trauma among West Nile refugees. BMC Psychiatry. 2004, 4: 34-10.1186/1471-244X-4-34.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Neuner F, Schauer M, Karunakara U, Klaschik C, Robert C, Elbert T: Psychological trauma and evidence for enhanced vulnerability for posttraumatic stress disorder through previous trauma among West Nile refugees. BMC Psychiatry. 2004, 4: 34-10.1186/1471-244X-4-34.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Steel Z, Silove D, Phan T, Bauman A: Long-term effect of psychological trauma on the mental health of Vietnamese refugees resettled in Australia: A population-based study. Lancet. 2002, 360 (9339): 1056-1062. 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)11142-1.CrossRefPubMed Steel Z, Silove D, Phan T, Bauman A: Long-term effect of psychological trauma on the mental health of Vietnamese refugees resettled in Australia: A population-based study. Lancet. 2002, 360 (9339): 1056-1062. 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)11142-1.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat MacNair RM: Psychological reverberations for the killers: preliminary historical évidence for perpetration-induced traumatic stress. J Genocide Res. 2001, 3 (2): 273-282. 10.1080/14623520120062457.CrossRefPubMed MacNair RM: Psychological reverberations for the killers: preliminary historical évidence for perpetration-induced traumatic stress. J Genocide Res. 2001, 3 (2): 273-282. 10.1080/14623520120062457.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat MacNair RM: Perpetration-induced traumatic stress (psychological dimensions to war and peace series). 2002, Praeger, Westport, 248-58. MacNair RM: Perpetration-induced traumatic stress (psychological dimensions to war and peace series). 2002, Praeger, Westport, 248-58.
14.
Zurück zum Zitat MacNair RM: Perpetration-Induced Traumatic Stress: The Psychological Consequences of Killing. 2005, Authors Choice Press, New York MacNair RM: Perpetration-Induced Traumatic Stress: The Psychological Consequences of Killing. 2005, Authors Choice Press, New York
15.
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Weierstall R, Schaal S, Schalinski I, Elbert T: The thrill of being violent as an antidote to trauma-related mental illness. European J Psychotraumatology. 2011, 2: 6345- Weierstall R, Schaal S, Schalinski I, Elbert T: The thrill of being violent as an antidote to trauma-related mental illness. European J Psychotraumatology. 2011, 2: 6345-
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Williams J, Oehlert G, Carlis J, Pusey A: Why do male chimpanzees defend a group range?. Anim Behav. 2004, 68: 523-532. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.09.015.CrossRef Williams J, Oehlert G, Carlis J, Pusey A: Why do male chimpanzees defend a group range?. Anim Behav. 2004, 68: 523-532. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.09.015.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Choi J, Bowles S: The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Science. 2007, 318: 636-640. 10.1126/science.1144237.CrossRefPubMed Choi J, Bowles S: The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Science. 2007, 318: 636-640. 10.1126/science.1144237.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Daly M, Wilson M: Homicidal tendencies. Demos. 1996, 8: 39-45. Daly M, Wilson M: Homicidal tendencies. Demos. 1996, 8: 39-45.
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Shackelford TK: An evolutionary psychological perspective on cultures of honor. Evol Psychol. 2005, 3: 381-391.CrossRef Shackelford TK: An evolutionary psychological perspective on cultures of honor. Evol Psychol. 2005, 3: 381-391.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson CA, Bushman BJ: Human aggression. Annu Rev Psych. 2002, 53: 27-51. 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231.CrossRef Anderson CA, Bushman BJ: Human aggression. Annu Rev Psych. 2002, 53: 27-51. 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Bernhardt PC: Influences of serotonin and testosterone in aggression and dominance: Convergence with social psychology. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 1997, 6 (2): 44-48. 10.1111/1467-8721.ep11512620.CrossRef Bernhardt PC: Influences of serotonin and testosterone in aggression and dominance: Convergence with social psychology. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 1997, 6 (2): 44-48. 10.1111/1467-8721.ep11512620.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Berkowitz L: Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. 1993, Mcgraw–Hill Book Company, New York Berkowitz L: Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. 1993, Mcgraw–Hill Book Company, New York
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Geen RG: Human Aggression (2nd ed). 2001, Taylor & Francis, London Geen RG: Human Aggression (2nd ed). 2001, Taylor & Francis, London
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Nell V: Cruelty’s rewards: The gratifications of perpetrators and spectators. Behav Brain Sci. 2006, 29: 211-257.PubMed Nell V: Cruelty’s rewards: The gratifications of perpetrators and spectators. Behav Brain Sci. 2006, 29: 211-257.PubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Wood W, Eagly AH: A cross-cultural analysis of the behavior of women and men: Implications for the origins of sex differences. Psychol Bull. 2002, 128 (5): 699-727.CrossRefPubMed Wood W, Eagly AH: A cross-cultural analysis of the behavior of women and men: Implications for the origins of sex differences. Psychol Bull. 2002, 128 (5): 699-727.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Elbert T, Weierstall R, Schauer M: Fascination Violence – on mind and brain of man hunters. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2010, 260 (2): 100-105. 10.1007/s00406-010-0144-8.CrossRef Elbert T, Weierstall R, Schauer M: Fascination Violence – on mind and brain of man hunters. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2010, 260 (2): 100-105. 10.1007/s00406-010-0144-8.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Ertl V, Pfeiffer A, Saile R, Schauer E, Elbert T, Neuner F: Validation of a mental health assessment in an African conflict population. Psychol Assess. 2010, 22 (2): 318-324.CrossRefPubMed Ertl V, Pfeiffer A, Saile R, Schauer E, Elbert T, Neuner F: Validation of a mental health assessment in an African conflict population. Psychol Assess. 2010, 22 (2): 318-324.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Foa EB, Cashman L, Jaycox L, Perry K: The validation of a self-report measure of posttraumatic stress disorder: The Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale. Psychol Assess. 1997, 9 (4): 445-451.CrossRef Foa EB, Cashman L, Jaycox L, Perry K: The validation of a self-report measure of posttraumatic stress disorder: The Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale. Psychol Assess. 1997, 9 (4): 445-451.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Ertl V, Pfeiffer A, Saile R, Schauer E, Elbert T, Neuner F: Validation of a mental health assessment in an African conflict population. Int Perspect in Psychol Res Pract Consultation. 2011, 1: 19-27. Ertl V, Pfeiffer A, Saile R, Schauer E, Elbert T, Neuner F: Validation of a mental health assessment in an African conflict population. Int Perspect in Psychol Res Pract Consultation. 2011, 1: 19-27.
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Blake DD, Weathers FW, Nagy LM, et al: The development of a clinician-administered PTSD scale. J Trauma Stress. 1995, 8: 75-90. 10.1002/jts.2490080106.CrossRefPubMed Blake DD, Weathers FW, Nagy LM, et al: The development of a clinician-administered PTSD scale. J Trauma Stress. 1995, 8: 75-90. 10.1002/jts.2490080106.CrossRefPubMed
33.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Foa EB, Riggs DS: Post-traumatic stress disorder in rape victims. American psychiatric press review of psychiatry. Edited by: Oldham J, Riba MB, Tasman A. 1993, American Psychiatric Press, Washington, D.C, 273-303. Foa EB, Riggs DS: Post-traumatic stress disorder in rape victims. American psychiatric press review of psychiatry. Edited by: Oldham J, Riba MB, Tasman A. 1993, American Psychiatric Press, Washington, D.C, 273-303.
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Kolassa IT, Elbert T: Structural and functional neuroplasticity in relation to traumatic stress. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2007, 16 (6): 321-325. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00529.x.CrossRef Kolassa IT, Elbert T: Structural and functional neuroplasticity in relation to traumatic stress. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2007, 16 (6): 321-325. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00529.x.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat McNally RJ: Cognitive abnormalities in posttraumatic stress disorder. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006, 10: 271-277. 10.1016/j.tics.2006.04.007.CrossRefPubMed McNally RJ: Cognitive abnormalities in posttraumatic stress disorder. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006, 10: 271-277. 10.1016/j.tics.2006.04.007.CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Weierstall R, Elbert T: The Appetitive Aggression Scale. European Journal of Psychotraumatology. 2011, 2: 8430- Weierstall R, Elbert T: The Appetitive Aggression Scale. European Journal of Psychotraumatology. 2011, 2: 8430-
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Vitielo B, Stoff DM: Subtypes of aggression and their relevance to child psychiatry. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997, 36: 307-315. 10.1097/00004583-199703000-00008.CrossRef Vitielo B, Stoff DM: Subtypes of aggression and their relevance to child psychiatry. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997, 36: 307-315. 10.1097/00004583-199703000-00008.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Kleinbaum DG, Kupper LL, Muller KE, Nizam A: Applied regression analysis and other multivariable methods. 1998, Thomson Brooks/Cole Publishing Co, Pacific Grove, California, United States, 3 Kleinbaum DG, Kupper LL, Muller KE, Nizam A: Applied regression analysis and other multivariable methods. 1998, Thomson Brooks/Cole Publishing Co, Pacific Grove, California, United States, 3
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Whittingham MJ, Stephens PA, Bradbury RB, Freckleton RP: Why do we still use stepwise modelling in ecology and behavior?. J Anim Ecol. 2006, 75: 1182-1189. 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2006.01141.x.CrossRefPubMed Whittingham MJ, Stephens PA, Bradbury RB, Freckleton RP: Why do we still use stepwise modelling in ecology and behavior?. J Anim Ecol. 2006, 75: 1182-1189. 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2006.01141.x.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Akaike H: Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika. 1987, 52 (3): 317-332. 10.1007/BF02294359.CrossRef Akaike H: Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika. 1987, 52 (3): 317-332. 10.1007/BF02294359.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Sakamoto Y, Ishiguro M, Kitagawa G: Akaike Information Criterion Statistics. 1986, D. Reidel Publishing Company, Dordrecht/Tokyo Sakamoto Y, Ishiguro M, Kitagawa G: Akaike Information Criterion Statistics. 1986, D. Reidel Publishing Company, Dordrecht/Tokyo
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A: G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007, 39: 175-191. 10.3758/BF03193146.CrossRefPubMed Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A: G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007, 39: 175-191. 10.3758/BF03193146.CrossRefPubMed
45.
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Elbert T, Rockstroh B, Kolassa IT, Schauer M, Neuner F: The influence of organized violence and terror on brain and mind – a Co-constructive perspective. Lifespan Development and the Brain: The perspective of Biocultural Co-Constructivism. Edited by: Baltes P, Reuter-Lorenz P, Rösler F. 2006, Cambridge University Press, , 326-349.CrossRef Elbert T, Rockstroh B, Kolassa IT, Schauer M, Neuner F: The influence of organized violence and terror on brain and mind – a Co-constructive perspective. Lifespan Development and the Brain: The perspective of Biocultural Co-Constructivism. Edited by: Baltes P, Reuter-Lorenz P, Rösler F. 2006, Cambridge University Press, , 326-349.CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Kulka RA, Schlenger WA, Fairbank JA, Hough RL, Jordan BK, Marmar CR, Weiss DS: Trauma and the Vietnam War Generation : Report on the Findings from the National Vietnam Veterans Readjustment Study. 1990, Brunner/Mazel, Kulka RA, Schlenger WA, Fairbank JA, Hough RL, Jordan BK, Marmar CR, Weiss DS: Trauma and the Vietnam War Generation : Report on the Findings from the National Vietnam Veterans Readjustment Study. 1990, Brunner/Mazel,
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson JT, Haritos-Fatouros M: The education of a torturer. Psychol Today. 1986, 20: 50-58. Gibson JT, Haritos-Fatouros M: The education of a torturer. Psychol Today. 1986, 20: 50-58.
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Milgram S: Behavioral study of obedience. J Abnorm Soc Psychol. 1963, 67: 371-378.CrossRef Milgram S: Behavioral study of obedience. J Abnorm Soc Psychol. 1963, 67: 371-378.CrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Baumeister RF, Campbell WK: The intrinsic appeal of eil: Sadism, sensational thrills, and threatened egotism. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 1999, 3 (3): 210-221. 10.1207/s15327957pspr0303_4.CrossRefPubMed Baumeister RF, Campbell WK: The intrinsic appeal of eil: Sadism, sensational thrills, and threatened egotism. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 1999, 3 (3): 210-221. 10.1207/s15327957pspr0303_4.CrossRefPubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Baumeister RF: Evil: Inside human violence and cruelty. 1997, Freeman, Baumeister RF: Evil: Inside human violence and cruelty. 1997, Freeman,
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Nash WP, Baker DG: Competing and complementary models of combat stress injury. Combat stress injury: Theory, research, and management. Edited by: Figley WP, Nash CR. 2007, Taylor & Francis Group, Routledge, 65-94. Nash WP, Baker DG: Competing and complementary models of combat stress injury. Combat stress injury: Theory, research, and management. Edited by: Figley WP, Nash CR. 2007, Taylor & Francis Group, Routledge, 65-94.
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Boscarino JA: External-cause mortality after psychologic trauma: The effects of stress exposure and predisposition. Compr Psychiatry. 2006, 47 (6): 503-514. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.02.006.CrossRefPubMed Boscarino JA: External-cause mortality after psychologic trauma: The effects of stress exposure and predisposition. Compr Psychiatry. 2006, 47 (6): 503-514. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.02.006.CrossRefPubMed
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Hendin H, Haas AP: Suicide and guilt as manifestations of PTSD in Vietnam combat veterans. Am J Psychiatry. 1991, 148 (5): 586-591.CrossRefPubMed Hendin H, Haas AP: Suicide and guilt as manifestations of PTSD in Vietnam combat veterans. Am J Psychiatry. 1991, 148 (5): 586-591.CrossRefPubMed
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Sampson RJ, Lauritsen JL: Deviant Lifestyles, Proximity to Crime, and the Offender–Victim Link in Personal Violence. J Res Crime Delinq. 1990, 27: 110-139. 10.1177/0022427890027002002.CrossRef Sampson RJ, Lauritsen JL: Deviant Lifestyles, Proximity to Crime, and the Offender–Victim Link in Personal Violence. J Res Crime Delinq. 1990, 27: 110-139. 10.1177/0022427890027002002.CrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Bullman TA, Kang HK: Posttraumatic stress disorder and the risk of traumatic deaths among Vietnam veterans. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1994, 182: 604-610. 10.1097/00005053-199411000-00002.CrossRefPubMed Bullman TA, Kang HK: Posttraumatic stress disorder and the risk of traumatic deaths among Vietnam veterans. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1994, 182: 604-610. 10.1097/00005053-199411000-00002.CrossRefPubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Card J: Epidemiology of PTSD in a national cohort of Vietnam veterans. Clin Psychol. 1987, 43 (1): 6-17. 10.1002/1097-4679(198701)43:1<6::AID-JCLP2270430103>3.0.CO;2-X.CrossRef Card J: Epidemiology of PTSD in a national cohort of Vietnam veterans. Clin Psychol. 1987, 43 (1): 6-17. 10.1002/1097-4679(198701)43:1<6::AID-JCLP2270430103>3.0.CO;2-X.CrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Vinck P, Pham PN, Stover E, Weinstein HM: Exposure to war crimes and implications for peace building in northern Uganda. JAMA. 2007, 298 (5): 543-554. 10.1001/jama.298.5.543.CrossRefPubMed Vinck P, Pham PN, Stover E, Weinstein HM: Exposure to war crimes and implications for peace building in northern Uganda. JAMA. 2007, 298 (5): 543-554. 10.1001/jama.298.5.543.CrossRefPubMed
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Shanahan F: Cultural Responses to the Reintegration of Formerly Abducted Girl Soldiers in Northern Uganda. Psychol and Soc. 2008, 1 (1): 14-28. Shanahan F: Cultural Responses to the Reintegration of Formerly Abducted Girl Soldiers in Northern Uganda. Psychol and Soc. 2008, 1 (1): 14-28.
Metadaten
Titel
When combat prevents PTSD symptoms—results from a survey with former child soldiers in Northern Uganda
verfasst von
Roland Weierstall
Inga Schalinski
Anselm Crombach
Tobias Hecker
Thomas Elbert
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2012
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2012
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-244X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-12-41

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2012

BMC Psychiatry 1/2012 Zur Ausgabe

Hörschwäche erhöht Demenzrisiko unabhängig von Beta-Amyloid

29.05.2024 Hörstörungen Nachrichten

Hört jemand im Alter schlecht, nimmt das Hirn- und Hippocampusvolumen besonders schnell ab, was auch mit einem beschleunigten kognitiven Abbau einhergeht. Und diese Prozesse scheinen sich unabhängig von der Amyloidablagerung zu ereignen.

So wirken verschiedene Alkoholika auf den Blutdruck

23.05.2024 Störungen durch Alkohol Nachrichten

Je mehr Alkohol Menschen pro Woche trinken, desto mehr steigt ihr Blutdruck, legen Daten aus Dänemark nahe. Ob es dabei auch auf die Art des Alkohols ankommt, wurde ebenfalls untersucht.

„Übersichtlicher Wegweiser“: Lauterbachs umstrittener Klinik-Atlas ist online

17.05.2024 Klinik aktuell Nachrichten

Sie sei „ethisch geboten“, meint Gesundheitsminister Karl Lauterbach: mehr Transparenz über die Qualität von Klinikbehandlungen. Um sie abzubilden, lässt er gegen den Widerstand vieler Länder einen virtuellen Klinik-Atlas freischalten.

ADHS-Medikation erhöht das kardiovaskuläre Risiko

16.05.2024 Herzinsuffizienz Nachrichten

Erwachsene, die Medikamente gegen das Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-Hyperaktivitätssyndrom einnehmen, laufen offenbar erhöhte Gefahr, an Herzschwäche zu erkranken oder einen Schlaganfall zu erleiden. Es scheint eine Dosis-Wirkungs-Beziehung zu bestehen.