The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: The broader autism phenotype includes relatives of individuals with autism who display social and language deficits that are qualitatively similar to those of autism but less severe. In previous studies of monozygotic twins discordant for autism, more than 75% of the twins without autism displayed the broader phenotype. Differences in neuroanatomy between discordant monozygotic twins might be associated with the narrow and broader behavioral phenotypes. The authors examined the relationship of twin pair differences in clinical phenotype to differences in neuroanatomic phenotype. METHOD: The subjects were 16 monozygotic twin pairs between the ages of 5 and 14 years and 16 matched singleton comparison subjects. Seven twin pairs were clinically concordant and nine twin pairs were clinically discordant for strictly defined autism. After magnetic resonance imaging, a semiautomated procedure was applied to images in which the brain tissue was subdivided into neurofunctional regions and segmented into gray, white, and ventricular compartments. RESULTS: Both the concordant and discordant twin pairs exhibited concordance in cerebral gray and white matter volumes. However, only the clinically concordant pairs exhibited concordance in cerebellar gray and white matter volumes. Within the discordant twin pairs, both the twins with autism and their co-twins exhibited frontal, temporal, and occipital white matter volumes that were lower than those of the comparison subjects. CONCLUSIONS: These findings support the role and the limits of genetic liability in autism. Continuing to clarify the neuroanatomic pathways in autistic spectrum disorders could illuminate the etiology of autism and, ultimately, contribute to treatments.

Autism is a behaviorally defined neurodevelopmental disorder that includes persistent deficits in communication, reciprocal social interaction, and play. During the past two decades, family and twin studies have provided evidence for a significant genetic component in autism. The risk of autism in siblings of autistic probands is approximately 45 times greater than that in the general population (1). Moreover, the concordance rate for autism ranges from 36% to 91% in monozygotic twin pairs and from 0% to 24% in dizygotic twin pairs (24).

Several reports have described a subset of relatives of individuals with autism who display social and language deficits that are qualitatively similar to those of the autistic proband but milder in severity (5, 6). This constellation of impairments, which relatives of probands exhibit at higher rates than do people in the general population, has been characterized as the broader phenotype for autism (711). Although evidence for the broad phenotype in families varies across studies (3, 12, 13), investigations of monozygotic twins (7) and of families with multiple-incidence autism (9, 10) have demonstrated support for the concept.

In comparisons of families with multiple-incidence autism to families with Down’s syndrome, Piven and colleagues reported significantly higher rates of social and communication deficits and stereotyped behaviors (9) and of psychiatric disorder (10) in the families of individuals with autism. Following up on a study by Folstein and Rutter (2), Le Couteur and co-workers (7) evaluated a group of 28 monozygotic pairs and 20 dizygotic same-sex pairs in whom one or both twins had autism, and they found social and communication deficits in 77% of the monozygotic co-twins (compared to 5% of the dizygotic twins) who were discordant for autism. Rutter and colleagues (8) have suggested that in such families a genetic substrate for autism may produce the social and cognitive deficits that are included in the broader phenotype but that an interaction between the genetic substrate and unknown deleterious environmental factors culminates in a “second hit” that ultimately produces the narrow phenotype for autism.

Little is known about the differences in neuroanatomy that could ultimately be associated with the narrow and broader behavioral phenotypes for autism. The findings from imaging studies of individuals with the narrow phenotype have not been consistent. Although several studies have indicated higher than normal overall brain volume (1416) and high regional (parietal, temporal, occipital) brain volumes (17), a cross-sectional study demonstrated that high volumes of cerebral gray, cerebral white, and cerebellar white matter in early childhood were followed by low volumes in late childhood and adolescence (18).

In a case report that compared the neuroanatomy of one monozygotic twin pair discordant for the narrow phenotype of autism to five age- and gender-matched unaffected comparison subjects, we found dysfunction of two separate brain clusters (19). Relative to the comparison subjects, both the twin with autism and his co-twin exhibited lower volumes in the frontal lobe and the superior temporal gyrus. The twin with autism also exhibited lower volumes in noncortical structures, including the caudate nucleus, the amygdala, the hippocampus, and the cerebellar vermis. On the basis of the findings from that preliminary report, in the present study we investigated the neuroanatomic phenotypes of 16 pairs of monozygotic twins of which seven twin pairs were concordant and nine twin pairs were discordant for the narrow phenotype for autism.

We sought to examine the extent to which twin pair differences in clinical phenotype were associated with differences in neuroanatomic phenotype. In this study we examined volumetric differences in lobar regions and the cerebellum. Since large ventricles have been found in previous studies of individuals with autism (14, 15), ventricular volumes were also included in the analyses. (Mesial temporal and subcortical structures will be investigated in a subsequent study.) We hypothesized that 1) clinically concordant twin pairs would exhibit a higher degree of neuroanatomic concordance than clinically discordant twin pairs and 2) all co-twins within discordant pairs would exhibit volumetric differences from comparison subjects in the lobes of the cerebral cortex, whereas only the autistic co-twins within the discordant pairs would exhibit differences in the ventricles and the cerebellum.

Method

Study Group

The original group consisted of 54 children, including 18 pairs of monozygotic twins and 18 age- and gender-matched unaffected peers. One twin pair was excluded from the analyses for this report because their scans were acquired in the sagittal plane, thereby producing segmented data that were not comparable to segmented data acquired in the coronal plane (20). A second twin pair was excluded for reasons to be described.

The final study group consisted of 48 children, including 16 pairs of monozygotic twins (N=32) and 16 age- and gender-matched unaffected peers. Fourteen of the twin pairs, and their matched comparison subjects, were boys. The mean age of the twin pairs was 8.4 years (SD=2.6, range=5.3–13.8). The mean age of the comparison subjects was 8.3 years (SD=2.4, range=5.4–13.9).

Families were recruited through the Autism Society of America, the National Alliance for Autism Research, the Kennedy Krieger Center for Autism and Related Disorders, and clinical neurologists at Kennedy Krieger Institute and Johns Hopkins Hospital. Subject recruitment followed a two-step process. A medical history form and the Autism Behavior Checklist (21) were initially mailed to interested families in order to screen twin pairs for potential participation in the study. Subjects with a genetic disorder or a history of status epilepticus were excluded. The behavioral criterion for potential participation in the study was based on the Autism Behavior Checklist: if at least one of the twins scored above 57 (representing the narrow phenotype for autism) on the Autism Behavior Checklist, the family was contacted by telephone by a project staff member. The Autism Diagnostic Inventory—Revised (22) was administered (separately for each co-twin) in a series of telephone interviews with a parent of the twins by a project staff member (L.A.S.) who was trained in the reliable administration of the instrument. If the diagnosis of autism was confirmed by the Autism Diagnostic Inventory for at least one co-twin of each twin pair, the family was invited to the Kennedy Krieger Institute for additional assessment. At our research center, the Autism Diagnostic Observational Scale—Generic (23) was administered to each co-twin by a reliable examiner (L.A.S.). The final diagnosis of each child was based on both the Autism Diagnostic Inventory and the Autism Diagnostic Observational Scale. In order to qualify for the diagnosis of autism, each child had to meet the criteria for autism on the Autism Diagnostic Inventory and score within one point of meeting the criteria for autism on the Autism Diagnostic Observational Scale.

No twin pair that qualified for the study on the basis of the Autism Behavior Checklist was then excluded on the basis of the Autism Diagnostic Inventory. However, for three twin pairs, the two instruments produced discrepant results regarding concordance between co-twins: one twin pair identified as discordant by the Autism Behavior Checklist (i.e., only one co-twin in the pair obtained a score higher than 57) was concordant according to the Autism Diagnostic Inventory, and two twin pairs identified as concordant by the Autism Behavior Checklist were discordant according to the Autism Diagnostic Inventory. In addition, one of the twin pairs whose scores indicated concordance on the Autism Behavior Checklist and discordance on the Autism Diagnostic Inventory was ultimately excluded from the study on the basis of the Autism Diagnostic Observational Scale, on which neither co-twin scored within the autistic range.

The unaffected singleton comparison subjects, individually matched by age and gender to each twin pair, were recruited from the office of a local pediatrician. Letters describing the study were sent by the pediatrician to parents of patients who, according to their medical records, did not have a history of developmental delays, emotional disturbance, or learning disabilities. Parents then contacted one of us (W.R.K. or C.P.B.) if they were interested in participating in the study. They were screened first by telephone (in order to rule out current emotional, developmental, or learning difficulties) and then invited to participate in the study if the telephone interview confirmed that their children met the inclusion criteria. Developmental diagnoses, including language delay, and psychiatric diagnoses (e.g., attention deficit hyperactivity disorder) were ruled out by using parental report and child psychiatric checklists (e.g., the Child Behavior Checklist). Accordingly, none of the comparison subjects had any known neurologic, developmental, learning, or psychiatric disorder.

Parents of all subjects signed informed consent forms approved by the institutional review board of Johns Hopkins Hospital.

DNA fingerprinting probes were used to confirm zygosity in all twin pairs. Eight independent loci were tested for each twin pair. The DNA profiles for all twin pairs were statistically identical at every locus, indicating that the probability of monozygosity for each twin pair was approximately 99.99%.

Image Acquisition and Processing

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was performed on a 1.5-T GE Signa scanner (GE Medical Systems, Milwaukee). A three-dimensional high-resolution MRI scan was acquired for each subject by using a T1-weighted spoiled gradient echo sequence (TR=35–45 msec, TE=5–7 msec, number of excitations=1) with a flip angle of 45°, matrix size of 256×128, and field of view of 20–24 cm, yielding 124 coronal slices with a slice thickness of 1.5 mm and an in-plane resolution of 0.131 mm.

The image data were imported into the program BrainImage (A.L. Reiss, Stanford University; http://spnl.stanford.edu/tools/brainimage.htm) for visualization, processing, and quantitation. The data were prepared for measurement by correcting for radio frequency inhomogeneity artifacts, removing voxels representing nonbrain material, and reformatting the images to produce data sets representing cubic voxels (24). These data sets were opened into the multiplanar visualization module of BrainImage, so that three orthogonal representations of the data could be viewed simultaneously.

Image Measurement

The isolated brain tissue was subdivided into cerebral lobes and subcortical, brainstem, and cerebellar regions according to a method based on the revised Talairach atlas (25) that is specific for measurement in pediatric study groups (2628). With this approach, high levels of sensitivity and specificity are achieved for all volumes of lobar brain regions based on the revised Talairach atlas (28). Each region was then segmented into gray, white, and ventricular compartments by using a constrained fuzzy algorithm that assigns voxels to one or more tissue categories on the basis of intensity values and tissue boundaries (29).

Statistical Analyses

Intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) were calculated in order to determine the degree of concordance in brain volumes in both the concordant and discordant twin pairs. We used r-to-z transformations to compare the magnitudes of the ICCs for the two groups. Multiple analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) were conducted in order to compare the brain volumes of the discordant twin pairs and singleton comparison subjects. Although limiting our volumetric analyses to the discordant pairs and comparison subjects reduced statistical power to some extent, this strategy permitted us to focus specifically on the extent to which discordance in clinical phenotype was reflected in differences in cerebral, cerebellar, and ventricular regions of interest. These analyses were conducted with diagnostic group as the independent variable, age and whole brain volume as the covariates, and volumes of neuroanatomic regions of interest as the dependent variables. Post hoc analyses were conducted with the Bonferroni-Dunn statistic to correct for multiple comparisons. Linear regression analyses were conducted in order to determine the relation between brain volumes and severity of autism (represented by scores on the Autism Diagnostic Inventory and Autism Diagnostic Observational Scale) in all twins with autism and their co-twins.

Results

Behavioral Phenotype

Seven pairs of twins had scores on the Autism Diagnostic Inventory and Autism Diagnostic Observational Scale that qualified both of them for the diagnosis of autism; those twin pairs were designated as concordant for the narrow phenotype for autism. The remaining nine pairs of twins were designated as discordant.

Analyses of variance were conducted to compare the scores on the Autism Diagnostic Inventory and Autism Diagnostic Observational Scale (Table 1) of the twin pairs. The differences between scores of the twins within the concordant pairs were not significant. Within the discordant twin pairs, Bonferroni-Dunn post hoc analyses revealed that the autistic co-twin received significantly higher scores on all but one summary measure; the exception was the rating of abnormal development on the Autism Diagnostic Inventory.

Within the discordant pairs, six of the nine co-twins who did not meet the criteria for autism on the Autism Diagnostic Inventory were reported by a parent to have exhibited language or social delays before participation in the study. In at least three cases, the delays were severe enough to warrant a diagnosis (before enrollment in the study) of developmental delay or pervasive developmental disorder. Two of the three remaining children whose histories were negative for social and language delay nevertheless exhibited subthreshold scores on the Autism Diagnostic Observational Scale that indicated mild impairment of communication and social skills. According to these criteria, the co-twins of the twins with autism were characterized as exhibiting the broad autism phenotype. We refer to these co-twins as “broad-phenotype co-twins.” (It should be noted, however, that one twin pair was discordant for both traditional autism and the broader phenotype.)

The mean age of the concordant twin pairs was 9.6 years (SD=2.6), and the mean age of the discordant pairs was 7.6 years (SD=2.4). This difference was not statistically significant.

Valid intellectual testing was completed on all comparison subjects, as well as on eight of the 14 twins in the concordant pairs and 17 of the 18 in the discordant pairs. (IQ testing could not be completed on the remaining seven children because of behavioral, attentional, and severe language deficits.) The IQs of the co-twins in the concordant pairs did not differ significantly (p=0.21) from each other, whereas the IQs of the co-twins in the discordant pairs did (t=–2.98, df=13, p=0.01). The IQs of the twins in the concordant and discordant pairs differed significantly from those of the comparison subjects. The twins in the concordant pairs had a mean IQ of 63.2 (SD=22.2), the co-twins with autism in the discordant pairs had a mean IQ of 69.6 (SD=18.7), the broad-phenotype co-twins in the discordant pairs had a mean IQ of 94.4 (SD=13.4), and the comparison subjects had a mean IQ of 123.6 (SD=9.7). After reviewing school records of the participants whose IQs we were unable to assess directly, we found that the frequency of mental retardation was somewhat higher for the twins in the concordant pairs (11 of 14 twins) than for the twins affected with autism in the discordant pairs (three of nine twins) (χ2=5.25, df=3, p=0.07).

Comparison of Concordant and Discordant Twin Pairs

As already stated, the degree of neuroanatomic concordance within twin pairs was determined with the ICC. The Fisher r-to-z transformation was then used to compare the difference in magnitude of the ICCs for the clinically concordant and discordant pairs. As Table 2 illustrates, the ICCs for cerebral gray and white volumes were high for all sets of twin pairs, regardless of degree of concordance in clinical phenotype. In contrast, the ICCs for gray and white cerebellar volumes were significantly lower for clinically discordant twin pairs than for clinically concordant twin pairs. The ICCs for ventricular volumes were somewhat (although not significantly) lower for the clinically discordant pairs than for the concordant pairs.

Neuroanatomic Differences Between Twins in Discordant Pairs

The following analyses were conducted with data from a subgroup consisting of the nine discordant twin pairs and the total group of 16 unaffected comparison subjects. Three groups were compared: co-twins with autism, broad-phenotype co-twins, and unaffected comparison subjects (Table 3). The groups did not differ significantly in whole brain volume. At the cortical level, cerebral gray and white volumes were compared with ANCOVAs, with whole brain volume entered as the covariate. (Although the group differences in whole brain volume were not significant, it was included in the model in order to investigate the extent to which its variation accounted for differences in specific tissue and regional volumes.) Cerebral gray matter volume did not differ between groups. Cerebral white matter volume was 11.5% lower in the co-twins with autism and 7.5% lower in the broad-phenotype co-twins than in the comparison subjects. Both diagnostic group (F=9.2, df=2, 1, 30, p=0.0008) and whole brain volume (F=212.9, df=2, 1, 30, p=0.0001) contributed significantly to the variance in cerebral white matter volume. Follow-up multiple ANCOVAs were conducted to determine whether specific lobar regions accounted for the differences in total cerebral white matter volume among the children with autism, their broad-phenotype co-twins, and the unaffected comparison subjects. Diagnostic group (autistic co-twin, broad-phenotype co-twin, comparison subject) was entered as the factor, whole brain volume was entered as a covariate, and frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital white matter volumes were entered as dependent variables. The Wilks’s lambda values for both diagnostic group (lambda=0.46, F=3.3, df=8, 54, p=0.004) and whole brain volume (lambda=0.11, F=54.1, df=4, 27, p=0.0001) were significant. Relative to the typically developing children, both the autistic and broad-phenotype co-twins had significant smaller frontal white matter (F=4.8, df=2, 1, 30, p=0.02), temporal white matter (F=11.0, df=2, 1, 30, p=0.0003), and occipital white matter (F=4.3, df=2, 1, 30, p=0.03).

The cerebellar gray matter, cerebellar white matter, and ventricular volumes of the three groups (autistic co-twins, broad-phenotype co-twins, comparison subjects) were compared with an ANCOVA in which whole brain volume was entered as a covariate. The cerebellar gray and white matter volumes did not differ between groups. Both diagnostic group (F=8.2, df=2, 1, 30, p=0.002) and whole brain volume (F=17.8, df=2, 1, 30, p=0.0002) contributed significantly to the variance in ventricular volume. Planned comparisons indicated that these findings were due to significant higher ventricular volume in the co-twins with autism than in the comparison subjects (p=0.002, Bonferroni-Dunn statistic).

Association Between Behavioral Phenotype and Brain Volumes

Data from all twin pairs were used for these analyses. The association between brain volumes and the behavioral phenotype for autism was determined by calculating the within-twin-pair difference in cerebral gray matter volume, cerebral white matter volume, cerebellar gray matter volume, and cerebellar white matter volume. The resulting difference scores were correlated with within-twin-pair difference scores derived from the sum of the communication and social scores on the Autism Diagnostic Observational Scale. (The summary score was used in order to reduce the number of comparisons.) The difference score for the Autism Diagnostic Observational Scale was not associated with the difference score for either cerebral gray or white matter volume. There was a nearly significant association between the Autism Diagnostic Observational Scale difference score and the difference score for cerebellar gray matter (r=0.48, N=16, p=0.07) and a significant association with the difference score for cerebellar white matter (r=0.55, N=16, p=0.04).

Discussion

The present study is the first, to our knowledge, to use a design of monozygotic twins concordant and discordant for the narrow phenotype for autism to investigate neuroanatomic variation in children with autism spectrum disorder. We found that within discordant pairs, both the autistic and broad-phenotype co-twins exhibited lower white (but not gray) matter volumes in the frontal, temporal, and occipital lobes than did unaffected comparison subjects. The finding that both autistic and broad-phenotype co-twins exhibited low cerebral white matter volume supports our hypothesis and is consistent with the deficits in executive function, higher-order cognitive processing, and language that characterize children with autistic spectrum disorders. Although a caveat to these findings is the potential contribution of the large discrepancy in IQs between the twin pairs and comparison subjects, previous volumetric studies of children with autism provide a developmental context in which to place these findings. Several studies (18, 30, 31) give cross-sectional evidence for an overproduction of cerebral gray and white matter in very young children (<5 years of age) with autism that appears to give way to hypoplasia or “normalization” of gray and white matter in children as they approach adolescence. Our subjects were somewhat older than the subjects used in at least one of these studies, and therefore we may be observing the effects of abnormally slowed cerebral white matter growth only.

It is not clear, however, why the autism-specific low volume was limited to white matter. It has been hypothesized that autism is associated with impairment of structural (32) or functional (3335) connectivity between specific regions of the brain. In a PET study, Castelli and colleagues (35) observed that adults with autism or Asperger syndrome exhibited low functional connectivity between the extrastriate cortex and the superior temporal sulcus at the temporoparietal junction while viewing animated sequences that elicited the description of mental states. In a spectroscopy study, Murphy and coworkers (36) found high concentrations of N-acetylaspartate and choline in the prefrontal cortex of individuals with autism. Whereas N-acetylaspartate is associated with neuronal integrity, choline is found in large concentrations in myelin sheaths. Accordingly, these differences may represent alterations in axons that derail the efficient transfer of information from the prefrontal cortex to other brain regions. Future studies using diffusion tensor imaging are needed to confirm the presence of alterations in the axonal connections of individuals with autism; potential findings from such studies coupled with the present findings of structural abnormalities in white matter would support the hypothesis of impaired connectivity in autism.

The finding that co-twins did not differ from each other or from comparison subjects in gray or white cerebellar volume was unexpected, although not surprising in light of the long-standing controversy over the extent to which the cerebellum is affected in autism. Differences between individuals with autism and comparison subjects in the volumes of cerebellar hemispheres and in the area of the cerebellar vermis have not been found consistently. Although several studies have shown that either the tissue (16, 31) or the white matter (18) of the cerebellar hemispheres is higher in individuals with autism, the differences are not consistently disproportionate to differences in whole brain volume. The current findings (including the results from comparisons of concordant and discordant twin pairs, discussed in the following) suggest that the precise nature of abnormalities of the cerebellum in autism is still not well understood.

Our hypothesis that clinically concordant twin pairs would exhibit a higher degree of neuroanatomic concordance than clinically discordant twin pairs was supported to a large extent. Whereas cerebral gray and white volumes were highly correlated for both clinically concordant and discordant pairs, cerebellar and ventricular volumes were more highly correlated for clinically concordant pairs than for clinically discordant pairs. This finding of neuroanatomic discordance in monozygotic twin pairs would not have been predicted from studies of unaffected, typically developing monozygotic twin pairs, for whom both cerebral and cerebellar volumes have been shown to be highly correlated (3739). Previous studies of unaffected monozygotic twin pairs have led to the conclusion that key aspects of both cerebral and cerebellar development are under tight genetic control in typical subject groups. As we will discuss, however, our findings suggest that for individuals with autism, the cerebellum may not be under as high a degree of genetic control. Moreover, the within-twin-pair differences in cerebellar white and gray volumes (but not cerebral volumes) were associated with within-twin-pair differences in Autism Diagnostic Observational Scale scores, suggesting that cerebellar (as opposed to cerebral) anatomy may serve as an important mediating feature between genetic liability and the expression of behavioral features of autism.

We do not know the etiology or pathology of these phenotypic differences in neuroanatomy and associated behavior between individuals whose genetic makeup is identical. One explanation is that within-pair differences in the expression of susceptibility genes could lead to differences in the regulation of brain development. The overexpression of neurotrophins, which regulate axonal and dendritic development as well as synaptogenesis (40), has been found in the blood of infants who were later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (41). Accordingly, development of cerebellar tissue could be compromised in co-twins who overexpress neurotrophins, undermining the development and efficiency of the neuronal connectivity necessary for adequate language and social development. In support of this notion is the finding that brain-derived neurotrophic factor, one of the neurotrophins found at high levels in children who later were diagnosed as autistic, is a growth factor for neuronal populations such as cerebellar-based Purkinje cells (41). (However, levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor were also high in children later diagnosed with mental retardation without autism, so the association may not be specific.)

As suggested earlier, an alternative explanation is that in order for severe autism to develop in one twin, genetic susceptibility to the disorder must be accompanied by deleterious environmental events that do not necessarily affect the child’s co-twin (8). These environmental events could include prenatal or perinatal factors. Previous studies have been inconsistent in their support of an association between adverse prenatal and perinatal events and autistic spectrum disorders (4245), although a more recent study supports such an association (46). Although this association is not robust, or specific to severe autism, these environmental factors could include nutritional variation due to malposition of one co-twin (47), prenatal trauma in the form of reduced blood flow or oxygen, or twin-to-twin transfusion resulting in significantly lower birth weight in the more severely affected co-twin. These conditions could affect the extent to which certain brain regions develop concordantly in co-twins (48). Postnatal factors could include delivery complications or respiratory distress affecting one co-twin to a greater extent than the other. Although the co-twins in our study did not appear to differ in the incidence of these events, our data were based on retrospective parental responses to a general checklist of perinatal and postnatal problems as well as medical interventions required. These responses were most likely not as reliable as review of hospital birth records and pose a limitation to this study.

The finding that cerebellar volumes diverge in discordant twins and are associated with the severity of the behavioral phenotype for autism suggests that variability in cerebellar morphometry may be mediated by nongenetic risk factors. Although this interpretation would not be consistent with findings from previously cited studies of the cerebellum (37, 39), which has been found to be under significant genetic control in normal subjects, it is possible that the genes that underlie the phenotype for autism affect the cerebellum in a way that increases its susceptibility to environmental risk factors. Moreover, insofar as cerebellar development is experience dependent (49, 50), the less affected (broad-phenotype) co-twin’s capacity to adapt to environmental stimuli may result in further “experience-guided” brain development (51), including greater cerebellar-cortical connections. Accordingly, preservation of cerebellar integrity in one co-twin may result in a cascade of events that permits the development of a relatively mildly affected neuroanatomic and behavioral phenotype in that co-twin.

Neuroanatomic differences between concordant and discordant twin pairs can be elucidated further with studies that use larger groups of not only monozygotic but also dizygotic twin pairs. Such studies, which should also include unaffected twin pairs, would help identify the relative contributions of genetic liability and environmental risk factors for autism. In addition, environmental risk factors, including potential prenatal, perinatal, and postnatal disruptions in development, should be integrated systematically into such studies. Moreover, the inclusion of comparison subjects with IQs in the average range would reduce the potential contribution of IQ to findings of anatomic differences. Continuing to clarify and differentiate the neuroanatomic pathways that subserve the entire spectrum of autistic disorders ultimately will permit specific and appropriately timed clinical interventions to children who display the full range of phenotypic expression of this disorder.

TABLE 1
TABLE 2
TABLE 3

Received Feb. 21, 2003; revision received July 15, 2003; accepted July 24, 2003. From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore; the Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, Calif. Address reprint requests to Dr. Kates, Department of Psychiatry, Upstate Medical University, State University of New York, 750 East Adams St., Syracuse, NY 13210; (e-mail). The authors thank Drs. Stewart Mostofsky, Andrew Zimmerman, Walter E. Kaufmann, and Martha B. Denckla for their contributions to the conceptualization and subject recruitment for this study, and Yung Liu, Suzanna Fryers, and Kristine Ducret for data processing. Supported by a grant to Dr. Kates from the National Alliance for Autism Research and the Autism Society of America and by grants to Dr. Reiss from NIMH (MH-50047 and MH-01142) and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (HD-31715).

References

1. Lord C, Leventhal BL, Cook EH Jr: Quantifying the phenotype in autism spectrum disorders. Am J Med Genet 2001; 105:36–38Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Folstein S, Rutter M: Infantile autism: a genetic study of 21 twin pairs. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1977; 18:297–321Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. Steffenburg S, Gillberg C, Hellgren L, Andersson L, Gillberg IC, Jakobsson G, Bohman M: A twin study of autism in Denmark, Finland, Iceland, Norway and Sweden. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1989; 30:405–416Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Bailey A, Le Couteur A, Gottesman I, Bolton P, Simonoff E, Yuzda E, Rutter M: Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a British twin study. Psychol Med 1995; 25:63–77Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Landa R, Piven J, Wzorek M, Gayle JO, Chase GA, Folstein SE: Social language use in parents of autistic individuals. Psychol Med 1992; 22:245–254Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Landa R, Folstein SE, Isaacs C: Spontaneous narrative-discourse performance of parents of autistic individuals. J Speech Hearing Res 1991; 34:1339–1345Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Le Couteur A, Bailey A, Goode S, Pickles A, Robertson S, Gottesman I, Rutter M: A broader phenotype of autism: the clinical spectrum in twins. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1996; 37:785–801Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Rutter M, Bailey A, Simonoff E, Pickles A: Genetic influences and autism, in Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders. Edited by Cohen D, Volkmar F. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 1997, pp 370–387Google Scholar

9. Piven J, Palmer P, Jacobi D, Childress D, Arndt S: Broader autism phenotype: evidence from a family history study of multiple-incidence autism families. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:185–190LinkGoogle Scholar

10. Piven J, Palmer P: Psychiatric disorder and the broad autism phenotype: evidence from a family study of multiple-incidence autism families. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:557–563AbstractGoogle Scholar

11. Piven J: The broad autism phenotype: a complementary strategy for molecular genetic studies of autism. Am J Med Genet 2001; 105:34–35Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Szatmari P, Jones MB, Tuff L, Bartolucci G, Fisman S, Mahoney W: Lack of cognitive impairment in first-degree relatives of children with pervasive development disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1993; 32:1264–1273Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Spiker D, Lotspeich L, Kraemer HC, Hallmayer J, McMahon W, Petersen PB, Nicholas P, Pingree C, Wiese-Slater S, Chiotti C, Wong DL, Dimicelli S, Ritvo E, Cavalli-Sforza LL, Ciaranello RR: Genetics of autism: characteristics of affected and unaffected children from 37 multiplex families. Am J Med Genet 1994; 54:27–35Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Piven J, Arndt S, Bailey J, Havercamp S, Andreasen NC, Palmer P: An MRI study of brain size in autism. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:1145–1149LinkGoogle Scholar

15. Hardan AY, Minshew NJ, Mallikarjuhn M, Keshavan MS: Brain volume in autism. J Child Neurol 2001; 16:421–424Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Hardan AY, Minshew NJ, Harenski K, Keshavan MS: Posterior fossa magnetic resonance imaging in autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001; 40:666–672Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Piven J, Arndt S, Bailey J, Andreasen N: Regional brain enlargement in autism: a magnetic resonance imaging study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996; 35:530–536Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18. Courchesne E, Karns CM, Davis HR, Ziccardi R, Carper RA, Tigue ZD, Chisum HJ, Moses P, Pierce K, Lord C, Lincoln AJ, Pizzo S, Schreibman L, Haas RH, Akshoomoff NA, Courchesne RY: Unusual brain growth patterns in early life in patients with autistic disorder: an MRI study. Neurology 2001; 57:245–254Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19. Kates WR, Mostofsky SH, Zimmerman AW, Mazzocco MM, Landa R, Warsofsky IS, Kaufmann WE, Reiss AL: Neuroanatomical and neurocognitive differences in a pair of monozygous twins discordant for strictly defined autism. Ann Neurol 1998; 43:782–791Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20. Patwardhan AJ, Eliez S, Warsofsky IS, Glover GH, White CD, Giedd JN, Peterson BS, Rojas DC, Reiss AL: Effects of image orientation on the comparability of pediatric brain volumes using three-dimensional MR data. J Comput Asst Tomogr 2001; 25:452–457Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21. Krug DA, Arick J, Almond P: Behavior checklist for identifying severely handicapped individuals with high levels of autistic behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1980; 21:221–229Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A: Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 1994; 24:659–685Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH Jr, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, Pickles A, Rutter M: The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2000; 30:205–223Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. Subramaniam B, Hennessey J, Rubin M, Beach L, Reiss A: Software and methods for quantitative imaging in neuroscience, in Neuroinformatics. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1997, pp 335–360Google Scholar

25. Talairach J, Tournoux P: Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: Three-Dimensional Proportional System. New York, Thieme Medical, 1988Google Scholar

26. Andreasen NC, Rajarethinam R, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, Swayze VW II, Flashman LA, O’Leary DS, Ehrhardt JC, Yuh WTC: Automatic atlas-based volume estimation of human brain regions from MR images. J Comp Assist Tomogr 1996; 20:98–106Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Kaplan DM, Liu AMC, Abrams MT, Warsofsky IS, Kates WR, White CD, Kaufmann WE, Reiss AL: Application of an automated parcellation method to the analysis of pediatric brain volumes. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 1997; 76:15–27Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Kates WR, Warsofsky IS, Patwardhan A, Abrams MT, Liu AMC, Naidu S, Kaufmann WE, Reiss AL: Automated Talairach atlas-based parcellation and measurement of cerebral lobes in children. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 1999; 91:11–30Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29. Reiss AL, Hennessey JG, Rubin M, Beach L, Abrams MT, Warsofsky IS, Liu AMC, Links JM: Reliability and validity of an algorithm for fuzzy tissue segmentation of magnetic resonance images. J Comput Asst Tomogr 1998; 22:471–479Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30. Carper RA, Moses P, Tigue ZD, Courchesne E: Cerebral lobes in autism: early hyperplasia and abnormal age effects. Neuroimage 2002; 16:1038–1051Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31. Sparks BF, Friedman SD, Shaw DW, Aylward EH, Echelard D, Artru AA, Maravilla KR, Giedd JN, Munson J, Dawson G, Dager SR: Brain structural abnormalities in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Neurology 2002; 59:184–192Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32. Skoyles J: Is autism due to cerebral-cerebellum disconnection? Med Hypotheses 2002; 58:332–336Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33. Frith U: Mind blindness and the brain in autism. Neuron 2001; 32:969–979Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. Grady C, Keightley M: Studies of altered social cognition in neuropsychiatric disorders using functional neuroimaging. Can J Psychiatry 2002; 47:327–336Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35. Castelli F, Frith C, Happe F, Frith U: Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain 2002; 125:1839–1849Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36. Murphy DG, Critchley HD, Schmitz N, McAlonan G, Van Amelsvoort T, Robertson D, Daly E, Rowe A, Russell A, Simmons A, Murphy KC, Howlin P: Asperger syndrome: a proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy study of brain. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002; 59:885–891Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37. Pennington BF, Filipek PA, Lefly D, Chhabildas N, Kennedy DN, Simon JH, Filley CM, Galaburda A, DeFries JC: A twin MRI study of size variations in human brain. J Cogn Neurosci 2000; 12:223–232Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. Baare W, Hulshoff Pol HE, Boomsma DI, Posthuma D, de Geus EJ, Schnack HG, van Haren NE, van Oel CJ, Kahn RS: Quantitative genetic modeling of variation in human brain morphology. Cereb Cortex 2001; 11:816–824Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Posthuma D, de Geus EJ, Neale MC, Hulshoff Pol HE, Baare WEC, Kahn RS, Boomsma D: Multivariate genetic analysis of brain structure in an extended twin design. Behav Genet 2000; 30:311–319Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40. Webb SJ, Monk CS, Nelson CA: Mechanisms of postnatal neurobiological development: implications for human development. Dev Neuropsychol 2001; 19:147–171Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

41. Nelson KB, Grether JK, Croen LA, Dambrosia JM, Dickens BF, Jelliffe LL, Hansen RL, Phillips TM: Neuropeptides and neurotrophins in neonatal blood of children with autism or mental retardation. Ann Neurol 2001; 49:597–606Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

42. Nelson KB: Prenatal and perinatal factors in the etiology of autism. Pediatrics 1991; 87(5, part 2):761–766Google Scholar

43. Gillberg C, Gillberg IC: Infantile autism: a total population study of reduced optimality in the pre-, peri-, and neonatal period. J Autism Dev Disord 1983; 13:153–166Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

44. Mason-Brothers A, Ritvo ER, Pingree C, Petersen PB, Jenson WR, McMahon WM, Freeman BJ, Jorde LB, Spencer MJ, Mo A, Ritvo A: The UCLA-University of Utah Epidemiologic Survey of Autism: prenatal, perinatal, and postnatal factors. Pediatrics 1990; 86:514–519MedlineGoogle Scholar

45. Piven J, Simon J, Chase GA, Wzorek M, Landa R, Gayle J, Folstein S: The etiology of autism: pre-, peri- and neonatal factors. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1993; 32:1256–1263Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

46. Juul-Dam N, Townsend J, Courchesne E: Prenatal, perinatal, and neonatal factors in autism, pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified, and the general population. Pediatrics 2001; 107(4):E63Google Scholar

47. Tramo MJ, Loftus WC, Stukel TA, Green RL, Weaver JB, Gazzaniga MS: Brain size, head size, and intelligence quotient in monozygotic twins. Neurology 1998; 50:1246–1252Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

48. Kohlhauser C, Mosgoller W, Hoger H, Lubec B: Myelination deficits in brains of rats following perinatal asphyxia. Life Sci 2000; 67:2355–2368Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

49. Kleim JA, Vij K, Ballard DH, Greenough WT: Learning dependent synaptic modifications in the cerebellar cortex of the adult rat persist for at least four weeks. J Neurosci 1997; 17:717–721Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

50. Kleim JA, Swain RA, Armstrong KE, Napper RMA, Jones TA, Greenough WT: Selective synaptic plasticity within the cerebellar cortex following complex motor skill learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem 1998; 69:274–289Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

51. Klintsova AY, Greenough WT: Synaptic plasticity in cortical systems. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1999; 9:203–208Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar