Skip to main content
Studie

Die langfristigen Auswirkungen von Frühgeburtlichkeit auf kognitive Entwicklung und Schulerfolg

Gibt es einen protektiven Effekt mütterlicher Responsivität?

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/0942-5403/a000235

Zusammenfassung. In einer prospektiven Längsschnittstudie wurde der Zusammenhang zwischen früher Responsivität der Mutter und kognitiver Entwicklung ihrer früh- bzw. reifgeborenen Kinder untersucht. Im Alter von drei Monaten wurde dafür die Mutter-Kind-Interaktion mittels Verhaltensbeobachtung erfasst. Bei n=351 der teilnehmenden Kinder (101 frühgeboren) wurde die allgemeine Intelligenz (IQ) im Alter von 11 Jahren und bei n=313 (85 frühgeboren) zusätzlich der höchste erreichte Schulabschluss bis 25 Jahren erhoben. Frühgeborene wiesen mit 11 Jahren einen signifikant niedrigeren IQ als Reifgeborene auf, nachdem für mögliche konfundierende Faktoren kontrolliert worden war. Nur bei Früh-, nicht aber bei Reifgeborenen zeigte sich ein signifikanter positiver Zusammenhang zwischen mütterlicher Responsivität und IQ. Für die Wahrscheinlichkeit einen höheren Schulabschluss (mind. Fachabitur) zu erreichen, fand sich weder ein signifikanter Effekt von Frühgeburtlichkeit noch von mütterlicher Responsivität.


Long-Term Consequences of Preterm Birth on Cognitive Development and Academic Achievement: Is There a Protective Effect of Maternal Responsiveness?

Abstract. Preterm birth is associated with adverse long-term consequences regarding cognitive development. Whereas children born very preterm represent a subgroup at special risk, so-called late preterms are also affected to a lesser degree. Effects of prematurity can be observed until adulthood. For example, decreased wealth was reported in adults born preterm, which was mediated by decreased intelligence during childhood and lower educational qualifications during young adulthood. Hence, it is highly relevant to examine whether certain factors can buffer against the adverse effects of preterm birth on cognitive development. Parenting might play an important role here. There is evidence suggesting a protective effect of sensitive parenting during childhood on later cognitive outcome in preterms. In the current study, we examined whether early responsive maternal care was associated with later intelligence and academic achievement in children born preterm versus full term. As part of an ongoing cohort study, early maternal responsiveness was assessed at the child’s age of 3 months (adjusted for gestational age) during a nursing and playing situation. At age 11 years, general intelligence (IQ) was determined in n = 351 children (101 born preterm; 168 male). Until age 25 years, educational qualification was assessed in n = 313 participants (85 born preterm; 145 male). IQ at age 11 was significantly lower in preterms compared with full-term subjects after adjusting for potential confounders like maternal educational background and early psychosocial risk. A significant interaction between preterm birth and early maternal responsiveness was detected. In preterms only, higher levels of early maternal responsiveness were significantly associated with higher child IQ. Lower IQs in children born preterm as compared with those born full term were observed in the subaverage-to-average range of maternal responsiveness. Interestingly, preterms exposed to very high levels of maternal responsiveness showed slightly higher IQs when compared with children born at term. With regard to academic achievement, neither a significant effect of preterm birth nor of early maternal responsiveness occurred after adjusting for potential confounders. The results of the current study replicate and extend earlier findings with regard to a protective effect of sensitive parenting on childhood cognitive outcome in preterms. The lacking impact of prematurity on academic achievement may be explained by the exclusion of participants with IQs outside the normal range in the current study. Interventions enhancing early responsive care in parents of preterms may be advisable. More studies on long-term outcomes of such interventions on cognitive development are encouraged.

Literatur

  • Aarnoudse-Moens, C. S., Weisglas-Kuperus, N., Goudoever, J. B. van & Oosterlaan, J. (2009). Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics, 124, 717 – 728. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Andreias, L., Borawski, E., Schluchter, M., Taylor, H. G., Klein, N. & Hack, M. (2010). Neighborhood influences on the academic achievement of extremely low birth weight children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 35, 275 – 283. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Basten, M., Jaekel, J., Johnson, S., Gilmore, C. & Wolke, D. (2015). Preterm birth and adult wealth: mathematics skills count. Psychological Science, 26, 1608 – 1619. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Belsky, J. & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 885 – 908. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bilgin, A. & Wolke, D. (2015). Maternal sensitivity in parenting preterm children: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 136, e177 – 193. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Breeman, L. D., Jaekel, J., Baumann, N., Bartmann, P. & Wolke, D. (2017). Neonatal predictors of cognitive ability in adults born very preterm: A prospective cohort study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 59, 477 – 483. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brisch, K. H. (2017). SAFE® – Sichere Ausbildung für Eltern: Sichere Bindung zwischen Eltern und Kind. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chan, E., Leong, P., Malouf, R. & Quigley, M. A. (2016). Long-term cognitive and school outcomes of late-preterm and early-term births: A systematic review. Child: Care, Health and Development, 42, 297 – 312. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • D’Onofrio, B. M., Class, Q. A., Rickert, M. E., Larsson, H., Langstrom, N. & Lichtenstein, P. (2013). Preterm birth and mortality and morbidity: A population-based quasi-experimental study. JAMA Psychiatry, 70, 1231 – 1240. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Feldman, R. (2007). Maternal versus child risk and the development of parent-child and family relationships in five high-risk populations. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 293 – 312. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Goldenberg, R. L., Culhane, J. F., Iams, J. D. & Romero, R. (2008). Epidemiology and causes of preterm birth. Lancet, 371, 75 – 84. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jaekel, J. (2016). Commentary: Supporting preterm children’s parents matters: A reflection on Treyvaud et al. (2016). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 57, 822 – 823. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jaekel, J., Pluess, M., Belsky, J. & Wolke, D. (2015). Effects of maternal sensitivity on low birth weight children’s academic achievement: A test of differential susceptibility versus diathesis stress. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 56, 693 – 701. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jörg, M., Dinter, R., Rose, F., Villalba-Yantorno, P., Esser, G., Schmidt, M. H. & Laucht, M. (1994). Kategoriensystem zur Mikroanalyse der frühen Mutter-Kind-Interaktion. Zeitschrift für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 22, 97 – 106. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Laucht, M., Schmidt, M. H. & Esser, G. (2002). Motorische, kognitive und sozial-emotionale Entwicklung von 11-Jährigen mit frühkindlichen Risikobelastungen: Späte Folgen. Zeitschrift für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 30, 5 – 19. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Milgrom, J., Newnham, C., Anderson, P. J., Doyle, L. W., Gemmill, A. W., Lee, K., Hunt, R. W., Bear, M. & Inder, T. (2010). Early sensitivity training for parents of preterm infants: Impact on the developing brain. Pediatric Research, 67, 330 – 335. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moster, D., Lie, R. T. & Markestad, T. (2008). Long-term medical and social consequences of preterm birth. New England Journal of Medicine, 359, 262 – 273. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Patra, K., Greene, M. M., Patel, A. L. & Meier, P. (2016). Maternal education level predicts cognitive, language, and motor outcome in preterm infants in the second year of life. American Journal of Perinatology, 33, 738 – 744. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Poehlmann, J., Schwichtenberg, A. J., Bolt, D. M., Hane, A., Burnson, C. & Winters, J. (2011). Infant physiological regulation and maternal risks as predictors of dyadic interaction trajectories in families with a preterm infant. Developmental Psychology, 47, 91 – 105. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Poustka, L., Zohsel, K., Blomeyer, D., Buchmann, A. F., Schmid, B. & Hohmann, S., et al. (2015). Interacting effects of maternal responsiveness, infant regulatory problems and dopamine D4 receptor gene in the development of dysregulation during childhood: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 70, 83 – 90. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rommel, A. S., James, S. N., McLoughlin, G., Brandeis, D., Banaschewski, T., Asherson, P. & Kuntsi, J. (2017). Association of preterm birth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-like and wider-ranging neurophysiological impairments of attention and inhibition. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 56, 40 – 50. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Saigal, S. & Doyle, L. W. (2008). An overview of mortality and sequelae of preterm birth from infancy to adulthood. Lancet, 371, 261 – 269. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmid, B., Blomeyer, D., Buchmann, A. F., Trautmann-Villalba, P., Zimmermann, U. S. & Schmidt, M. H., et al. (2011). Quality of early mother-child interaction associated with depressive psychopathology in the offspring: A prospective study from infancy to adulthood. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45, 1387 – 1394. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shah, P. E., Robbins, N., Coelho, R. B. & Poehlmann, J. (2013). The paradox of prematurity: The behavioral vulnerability of late preterm infants and the cognitive susceptibility of very preterm infants at 36 months post-term. Infant Behavior & Development, 36, 50 – 62. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spittle, A. J., Barton, S., Treyvaud, K., Molloy, C. S., Doyle, L. W. & Anderson, P. J. (2016). School-age outcomes of early intervention for preterm infants and their parents: A randomized trial. Pediatrics, 138, e20161363. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spittle, A. J., Orton, J., Doyle, L. W. & Boyd, R. (2007). Early developmental intervention programs post hospital discharge to prevent motor and cognitive impairments in preterm infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev, CD005495. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spittle, A. J. & Treyvaud, K. (2016). The role of early developmental intervention to influence neurobehavioral outcomes of children born preterm. Seminars in Perinatology, 40, 542 – 548. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Treyvaud, K., Anderson, V. A., Howard, K., Bear, M., Hunt, R. W. & Doyle, L. W., et al. (2009). Parenting behavior is associated with the early neurobehavioral development of very preterm children. Pediatrics, 123, 555 – 561. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Treyvaud, K., Doyle, L. W., Lee, K. J., Ure, A., Inder, T. E., Hunt, R. W. & Anderson, P. J. (2016). Parenting behavior at 2 years predicts school-age performance at 7 years in very preterm children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 57, 814 – 821. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Voss, W., Jungmann, T., Wachtendorf, M. & Neubauer, A. P. (2012). Long-term cognitive outcomes of extremely low-birth-weight infants: The influence of the maternal educational background. Acta Paediatrica, 101, 569 – 573. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weiß, R. (1987a). Grundintelligenztest CFT 20. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Weiß, R. (1987b). Wortschatztest WS. Ergänzungstest zum Grundintelligenztest CFT 20. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wolke, D., Jaekel, J., Hall, J. & Baumann, N. (2013). Effects of sensitive parenting on the academic resilience of very preterm and very low birth weight adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 53, 642 – 647. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zohsel, K., Hohm, E., Schmidt, M. H., Brandeis, D., Banaschewski, T. & Laucht, M. (2017). Langfristige Folgen früher psychosozialer Risiken. Child Behavior Checklist-Dysregulationsprofil als vermittelnder Faktor. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 26, 203-209. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar