Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Within-person Changes in Co-rumination and Rumination in Adolescence: Examining Heterogeneity and the Moderating Roles of Gender and Time

  • Empirical Research
  • Published:
Journal of Youth and Adolescence Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although previous work has consistently identified positive associations between co-rumination and rumination during adolescence, little to no research has examined how this relationship operates on the person-specific level. The current study aimed to extend current developmental theories of co-rumination and rumination by examining within-person associations between these constructs. Survey data was collected from 1502 adolescents (Mage = 13.20; 52% girls; 52% non-Hispanic White) every six-months across the span of 3.5 years. The results showed that at time-points when adolescents reported co-ruminating more than their usual level, they reported concurrent increases in rumination. This association was stronger for boys and strengthened over time. Despite substantial between-person heterogeneity, 97% of adolescents showed positive associations between co-rumination and rumination. This research has important implications for mental health professionals, school systems, and parents who may look to teach adolescents about effective emotion-regulation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Concurrent associations are the focus of the current manuscript, but lagged analyses can be found in supplemental material on OSF (https://osf.io/cu89f/?view_only=9c5fc91b43964db5bd8d0d3b110d41fa).

  2. Although most prior research suggests that co-rumination precedes rumination, and not the other way around, an alternative model was also run where the association between within-subject rumination and co-rumination was assessed; It was hypothesized that results would be conceptually very similar across these models.

References

  • Abela, J. R. Z., & Hankin, B. L. (2011). Rumination as a vulnerability factor to depression during the transition from early to middle adolescence: A multiwave longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120(2), 259–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aldrich, J. T., Lisitsa, E., Chun, S. K., & Mezulis, A. H. (2019). Examining the relationship between daily co-rumination and rumination in response to negative events among adolescents using ecological momentary assessment. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 38(8), 704–719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bastin, M., Luyckx, K., Raes, F., & Bijttebier, P. (2021). Co-Rumination and Depressive Symptoms in Adolescence: Prospective Associations and the Mediating Role of Brooding Rumination. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 50(5), 1003–1016. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-021-01412-4.

  • Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641–1660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolger, N., & Laurenceau, J. P. (2013). Intensive longitudinal methods: An introduction to diary and experience sampling research. Guilford Press.

  • Bolger, N., Zee, K. S., Rossignac-Milon, M., & Hassin, R. R. (2019). Causal processes in psychology are heterogeneous. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(4), 601–618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brausch, A. M., & Decker, K. M. (2014). Self-esteem and social support as moderators of depression, body image, and disordered eating for suicidal ideation in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42, 779–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bürkner, P. C. (2017). brms: An R Package for Bayesian multilevel models using stan. Journal of Statistical Software, 80(1), 1–28. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v080.i01.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cranford, J. A., Shrout, P. E., Iida, M., Rafaeli, E., Yip, T., & Bolger, N. (2006). A procedure for evaluating sensitivity to within-person change: Can mood measures in diary studies detect change reliably? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(7), 917–929.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiGiovanni, A. M., Vannucci, A., Ohannessian, C. M., & Bolger, N. (2021). Modeling heterogeneity in the simultaneous emotional costs and social benefits of co-rumination. Emotion, 21(7), 1470–1482. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DiGiovanni, A. M., Cornelius, T., & Bolger, N. (accepted). Decomposing variance in co-rumination using dyadic daily diary data. https://psyarxiv.com/sm6xr/. Social Psychological and Personality Science.

  • du Pont, A., Rhee, S. H., Corley, R. P., Hewitt, J. K., & Friedman, N. P. (2019). Rumination and executive functions: Understanding cognitive vulnerability for psychopathology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 256, 550–559. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2019.06.026.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Felton, J. W., Cole, D. A., Havewala, M., Kurdziel, G., & Brown, V. (2019). Talking together, thinking alone: relations among co-rumination, peer relationships, and rumination. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 48(4), 731–743.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, A., Carlin, J. B., Stern, H. S., Dunson, D. B., Vehtari, A., & Rubin, D. B. (2013). Bayesian data analysis. CRC press.

  • Goodman, E., Adler, N. E., Kawachi, I., Frazier, A. L., Huang, B., & Colditz, G. A. (2001). Adolescents’ perceptions of social status: development and evaluation of a new indicator. Pediatrics, 108(2), e31–e31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hankin, B. L. (2008). Rumination and depression in adolescence: Investigating symptom specificity in a multiwave prospective study. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 37(4), 701–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hankin, B. L., Stone, L., & Wright, P. A. (2010). Co-rumination, interpersonal stress generation, and internalizing symptoms: Accumulating effects and transactional influences in a multi-wave study of adolescents. Development and Psychopathology, 22(1), 217–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jose, P. E., & Brown, I. (2008). When does the gender difference in rumination begin? Gender and age differences in the use of rumination by adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37, 180–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krause, E. D., Vélez, C. E., Woo, R., Hoffmann, B., Freres, D. R., Abenavoli, R. M., & Gillham, J. E. (2018). Rumination, depression, and gender in early adolescence: a longitudinal study of a bidirectional model. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 38(7), 923–946.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McMillan, C., Felmlee, D., & Osgood, D. W. (2018). Peer influence, friend selection, and gender: How network processes shape adolescent smoking, drinking, and delinquency. Social Networks, 55, 86–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, E. E., Jarcho, J. M., & Guyer, A. E. (2016). Social re-orientation and brain development: An expanded and updated view. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 118–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(4), 569–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/.

  • Raes, F., Griffith, J. W., Van der Gucht, K., & Williams, J. M. G. (2014). School-based prevention and reduction of depression in adolescents: A cluster-randomized controlled trial of a mindfulness group program. Mindfulness, 5(5), 477–486. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-013-0202-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rood, L., Roelofs, J., Bögels, S. M., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schouten, E. (2009). The influence of emotion-focused rumination and distraction on depressive symptoms in non-clinical youth: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(7), 607–616. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.07.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rood, L., Roelofs, J., Bögels, S. M., & Meesters, C. (2012). Stress-reactive rumination, negative cognitive style, and stressors in relationship to depressive symptoms in non-clinical youth. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41(4), 414–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, A. J. (2002). Co–rumination in the friendships of girls and boys. Child Development, 73, 1830–1843.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, A. J., Carlson, W., & Waller, E. M. (2007). Prospective associations of co-rumination with friendship and emotional adjustment: Considering the socioemotional trade-offs of co-rumination. Developmental Psychology, 43(4), 1019–1031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schäfer, J. Ö., Naumann, E., Holmes, E. A., Tuschen-Caffier, B., & Samson, A. C. (2017). Emotion regulation strategies in depressive and anxiety symptoms in youth: A meta-analytic review. Journal of youth and adolescence, 46(2), 261–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spendelow, J. S., Simonds, L. M., & Avery, R. E. (2017). The relationship between co‐rumination and internalizing problems: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 24(2), 512–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinberg, L., Dahl, R., Keating, D., Kupfer, D. J., Masten, A., Pine, D. Cicchetti, D. (2006). “Adolescent psychopathology.” In Handbook of Developmental Psychopathology. New York: Wiley & Sons.

  • Stone, L. B., & Gibb, B. E. (2015). Brief report: Preliminary evidence that co-rumination fosters adolescents’ depression risk by increasing rumination. Journal of Adolescence, 38, 1–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, L. B., Hankin, B. L., Gibb, B. E., & Abela, J. R. (2011). Co-rumination predicts the onset of depressive disorders during adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120(3), 752–757.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tompkins, T. L., Hockett, A. R., Abraibesh, N., & Witt, J. L. (2011). A closer look at co-rumination: Gender, coping, peer functioning and internalizing/externalizing problems. Journal of Adolescence, 34(5), 801–811.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Twenge, J. M., Cooper, A. B., Joiner, T. E., Duffy, M. E., & Binau, S. G. (2019). Age, period, and cohort trends in mood disorder indicators and suicide-related outcomes in a nationally representative dataset, 2005–2017. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 128(3), 185–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Census Bureau (2016). Income and Poverty in the United States: 2016. Retrieved from https://www.census.gov/library/publications/2017/demo/p60-259.html.

  • Vijayakumar, N., & Pfeifer, J. H. (2020). Self-disclosure during adolescence: exploring the means, targets, and types of personal exchanges. Current Opinion in Psychology, 31, 135–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1970). The age variable in psychological research. Psychological Review, 77(1), 49–64. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0028600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, H. X., Chen, X., Shen, Y. Q., Li, L., Chen, N. X., Zhu, Z. C., Castellanos, F. X., & Yan, C. G. (2020). Rumination and the default mode network: Meta-analysis of brain imagingstudies and implications for depression. NeuroImage, 206, 116287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegert, D. I., & Kistner, J. A. (2002). Response styles theory: Downward extension to children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31(3), 325–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Skinner, E. A. (2011). Review: The development of coping across childhood and adolescence: An integrative review and critique of research. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35(1), 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the Alvord Foundation for funding data collection. We would also like to thank all of the schools and adolescents who participated over the past four years.

Authors' Contributions

A.D. executed the statistical analyses, drafted the manuscript, and substantially contributed to the revisions of the manuscript; T.F. executed statistical analyses, drafted the manuscript, and substantially contributed to the revisions of the manuscript; A.V. collected all of the data, assisted T.F. in preliminary analyses, and edited all drafts of the manuscript; C.O. managed data collection, procured funding for the study, participated in the design of the study, and assisted in editing all drafts of the manuscript; N.B. assisted with analyses and data visualizations and provided consultation on revisions regarding statistical analyses. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by the Alvord foundation.

Data Sharing and Declaration

The datasets needed to run the analyses and all R scripts are available on OSF (https://osf.io/cu89f/?view_only=9c5fc91b43964db5bd8d0d3b110d41fa). All supplemental materials are also included on this page.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ana M. DiGiovanni.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Participants’ parents were informed about the study and about the possibility to exclude their children from participating (passive consent).

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

These authors contributed equally: Ana M. DiGiovanni, Tessa Fagle

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

DiGiovanni, A.M., Fagle, T., Vannucci, A. et al. Within-person Changes in Co-rumination and Rumination in Adolescence: Examining Heterogeneity and the Moderating Roles of Gender and Time. J Youth Adolescence 51, 1958–1969 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-022-01647-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-022-01647-9

Keywords:

Navigation