Skip to main content
Log in

The cognitive psychology of gambling

  • Articles
  • Published:
Journal of Gambling Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A number of recent studies have shown there may be a strong cognitive bias in explaining persistent gambling. Theories that have been put forward include the illusion of control, “cognitive regret,” biased evaluations and the “psychology of the near miss.” Two exploratory studies examining the acquisition, development and maintenance of gambling behaviour involving adolescent fruit machine gamblers were carried out. Those factors which directly relate to the cognitive biases (notably erroneous beliefs about skill) during gambling activity are discussed with reference to the above cognitive influences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association (1987).Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, (3rd Edition-revised), Author: Washington D.C.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amsel, A. (1958). The role of frustrative non-reward in non-continuous reward situations.Psychological Bulletin, 55, 102–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergler, E. (1957).The psychology of gambling. Hill and Wang: New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaszczynski, A.P., McConaghy, N. & Winter, S.W. (1986). Plasma endorphin levels in pathological gambling.Journal of Gambling Behavior, 2, 3–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R.I.F. (1986). Arousal and sensation seeking components in the general explanation of gambling and gambling addictions.International Journal of the Addictions, 21, 1001–1016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1928). Dostoyevsky and parracide. In J. Strachey (Ed.).The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud, 21, 177–121. Hogarth Press: London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Furnham, A. & Lewis, A. (1983).The economic mind. Harvester Press: London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaboury, A. & Ladouceur, R. (1988).Erroneous perceptions and gambling. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Gilovich, T. (1983). Biased evaluations and persistence in gambling.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1110–1126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilovich, T. & Douglas, C. (1986). Biased evaluations of randomly determined gambling outcomes.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 228–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1967).Interaction ritual. Anchor: New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, H. (1980). Psychology of gambling. In Kaplan, H.I., Feedman, A.M., and Sadcock, B.J. (Eds.):Comprehensive textbook of psychiatry, 3rd Edition. New York: Williams and Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, M.D. (1988a, September).The acquisition, development and maintenance of fruit machine gambling in adolescents. Paper presented at the Social Psychology Section Conference, British Psychological Society, University of Kent, Canterbury.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, M.D. (1988b). Fruit machine gambling in addicted adolescents.Society for the Study of Gambling Newsletter, 14, 17–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, M.D. (1989). Gambling in children and adolescents.Journal of Gambling Behavior, 5, 66–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henslin, J.M. (1967). Craps and magic.American Journal of Sociology, 73, 316–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. (1982). The psychology of preferences.Scientific American, January, 136–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladouceur, R. & Gaboury, A. (1988). Effects of limited and unlimited stakes on gambling behavior.Journal of Gambling Behavior, 4, 119–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladouceur, R., Gaboury, A., Dumont, M. and Rochette, P. (1988). Gambling: Relationship between the frequency of wins and irrational thinking.Journal of Psychology, 122, 409–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladouceur, R. & Mireault, M. (1988). Gambling behaviors among high school students in the Quebec area.Journal of Gambling Behavior, 4, 3–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E.J. (1975). The illusion of control.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 311–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E.J. & Roth, J. (1975). The illusion of control as a function of the sequence of outcomes in a purely chance task.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 951–955.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lesieur, H.R. & Klein, R. (1987). Pathological gambling among high school students.Addictive Behaviors, 12, 129–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, G.R. & Loftus, E.K. (1983).Mind at play. Basic Books: New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oldman, D. (1974). Chance and skill: A study of roulette.Sociology, 8, 407–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reid, R.L. (1986). The psychology of the near miss.Journal of Gambling Behavior, 2, 32–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B.F. (1953).Science and human behavior. Macmillan: New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strickland, L.H., Lewicki, R.J. & Katz, A.M. (1966). Temporal orientation and perceived control as determinants of risk-taking.Journal of Experimental and Social Psychology, 2, 143–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stumphauzer, J.S. (1980). Learning to drink: Adolescents and alcohol.Addictive Behaviors, 5, 277–283.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

The author would like to thank the United Kingdom Economic and Social Research Council for funding this work through a research studentship. The author would also like to thank S.E.G. Lea for his invaluable advice during the preparation of this manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Griffiths, M.D. The cognitive psychology of gambling. J Gambling Stud 6, 31–42 (1990). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01015747

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01015747

Keywords

Navigation