Skip to main content
Log in

Pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic responses to caffeine in poor and normal sleepers

  • Original Investigation
  • Published:
Psychopharmacology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic responses to caffeine (2.5 mg/kg) were compared between ten healthy self-rated poor sleepers and ten normal sleepers. Sleep pattern assessed by the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI). There was no significant difference in mean estimated daily caffeine consumption between the groups. The poor sleepers had significantly higher scores for neuroticism on the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ) and anxiety on the Hospital Anxiety Depression (HAD) scale, compared with normal sleepers. Caffeine pharmacokinetics were assessed by measurement of saliva caffeine concentrations. Poor sleepers showed significantly greater variability in caffeine Cmax, clearance and half-life, compared to normal sleepers. Pharmacodynamic measures included heart rate, blood pressure, visual analogue scales for concentration, vigilance and relaxation, psychomotor performance [Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) and tapping rate (TR)] and EEG activity [Contingent negative variation (CNV), auditory evoked potential and power spectral analysis]. Prior to caffeine administration, poor sleepers compared to normal sleepers had faster heart rates, lower ratings for concentration and relaxation, poorer performance on the DSST, greater CNV magnitude, faster peak alpha frequency and lower delta, theta and beta power. These differences persisted after caffeine ingestion and overall differences between the groups on these measures were significant (P<0.01–0.001). Post-dose, but not pre-dose, scores for vigilance and TR were significantly lower overall in poor compared with normal sleepers. Despite the baseline differences between poor and normal sleepers, the changes following caffeine administration were similar in direction and magnitude in both groups.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Badcock NR (1990) Simpler measurement of caffeine and paraxanthine in saliva. Clin Chem 36:391

    Google Scholar 

  • Balter MB, Uhlenuth EH (1992) New epidemiological findings about insomnia and its treatment. J Clin Psychiatry 53:34–42

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barone JJ, Roberts H (1984) Human consumption of caffeine. In: Dews PB (ed) Caffeine: perspectives for recent research. Springer, Berlin, pp 59–73

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman SR, Buysse DJ, Kupfer DJ, Monk TH, Reynolds CF (1988) The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 28:193–213

    Google Scholar 

  • Boulenger J-P, Uhde T (1982) Caffeine consumption and anxiety: preliminary results of a survey comparing patients with anxiety disorders and normal controls. Psychopharmacol Bull 18:53–57

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boulenger J-P, Uhde TW, Wolff A, Post RM (1984) Increased sensitivity to caffeine in patients with panic disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 41:1067–1071

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruce M, Scott N, Shire P, Lader M (1992) Anxiogenic effects of caffeine in patients with anxiety disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49:867–869

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carskadon MA, Roth T (1991) Sleep restriction. In: Mont TH (ed) Sleep, sleepiness and performance. Wiley, New York, pp 155–167

    Google Scholar 

  • Charney DS, Heninger GR, Jatlow PI (1985) Increased anxiogenic effects of caffeine in panic disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 42:233–243

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper R, Osselton JW, Shaw JC (1980) EEG technology, 3rd edn. Butterworth, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Dorsey CM (1991) Failing to sleep: psychological and behavioural underpinning of insomnia. In: Mont TH (ed) Sleep, sleepiness and performance. Wiley, New York, pp 223–247

    Google Scholar 

  • Dorsey C, Bootzin RS (1987) Subjective and psychophysiologic insomnia: multiple sleep latency test, sleep latency and personality. Sleep Res 16:328

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck HJ, Eysenck SBG (1975) Eysencl personality questionnaire. Hodder & Stoughton, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Fink M (1978) Psychoactive drugs and the waking EEG. In: Lipton MA, DiMascio A, Killam KF (eds) Psychopharmacology: a generation of progress. Raven Press, New York, pp 691–698

    Google Scholar 

  • Freedman RR, Sattler HL (1982) Physiological and psychological factors in sleep-onset insomnia. J Abnorm Psychol 91:380–389

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fuhr V, Rost KL (1994) Simple and reliable CYP1A2 phenotyping by the paraxanthine/caffeine ratio in plasma and saliva. Pharmacogenetics 4:109–116

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein A, Kaizer S (1969) Psychotropic effects of caffeine in man. III A questionnaire survey of coffee drinking and effects on a group of housewives. Clin Pharmacol Ther 10:477–488

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grant DM, Tang BK, Kalow W (1983) Variability in caffeine metabolism. J Clin Pharmacol Ther 33:591–602

    Google Scholar 

  • Greden JF (1974) Anxiety or caffeinism: a diagnostic dilemma. Am J Psychiatry 131:1089–1092

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haynes SN, Adams A, Franzen M (1981) The effects of presleep stress on sleep-onset insomnia. J Abnorm Psychol 90:601–606

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horne JA (1991) Dimensions to sleepiness. In: Mont TH (ed) Sleep, sleepiness and performance. Wiley, New York, pp 169–196

    Google Scholar 

  • James JE (1991) Caffeine and health. Academic Press, London

    Google Scholar 

  • James JE, Crosbie J (1987) Somatic and psychological implications of heavy caffeine use. Br J Addict 82:503–509

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kales A, Caldwell AB, Soldatos CR, Bixler EO, Kales JD (1983) Biopsychobehavioural correlates of insomnia. II. Pattern specificity and consistency with the Minnesota multiphasic personality inventory. Psychosom Med 45:341–356

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lader MH, Bruce MS (1989) The human pharmacology of the methylxanthines. In: Hindmarsh I, Storier PD (eds) Human psychopharmacology: measures and methods, vol. 2. Wiley London, pp 179–200

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin D, Bertelson AD, Lacks P (1984) MMPI differences among mild and severe insomniacs and good sleepers. J Person Assess 48:126–129

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy M, Zylber-Katz E (1983) Caffeine metabolism and coffee-attributed sleep disturbances. Clin Pharmacol Ther 33:770–774

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews JNS, Altman DG, Campbell MJ, Royston P (1990) Analysis of serial measurements in medical research. BMJ 300:230–235

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monroe LJ (1967) Psychological and physiological differences between good and poor sleepers. J Abnorm Psychol 72:255–264

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan K, Dallasso H, Ebrahim S, Arie T, Pentem PH (1988) Characteristics of subjective insomnia in the elderly living at home. Age Aging 17:1–7

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan K, Healey DW, Healey PJ (1989) Factors influencing persistent subjective insomnia in old age: a follow-up study of good and poor sleepers aged 65 to 74. Age Ageing 18:117–122

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • National Institute of Mental Health (1984) Consensus conference report: drugs and insomnia: the use of medication to promote sleep. JAMA 251:2410–2414

    Google Scholar 

  • Nino-Murcia C (1992) Diagnosis and treatment of insomnia and risks associated with lack of treatment. J Clin Psychiatry 53 [12 (Suppl)]:43–49

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pritchard WS (1981) Psychophysiology of P300. Psychol Bull 89:506–540

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shagass C, Straumenis JJ (1978) Drugs and human sensory evoked potentials. In: Lipton MA, DiMascio A, Killam KF (eds) Psychopharmacology: a Generation of Progress. Raven Press, New York, pp 699–709

    Google Scholar 

  • Tecce JJ, Cole JO (1976) The distreaction-arousal hypothesis, CNV, and schizophrenia. In: Mostofsky DI (ed) Behaviour control and modification of physiological activity. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, N.J., pp 162–219

    Google Scholar 

  • Walter WG, Cooper R, Aldridge VJ, McCallum, WC, Winter AL (1964) Contingent negative variation: and electric sign of sensorimotor association and expectancy in the human brain. Nature 203:380–384

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welstein L, Dement WC, Redington D, Guilleminault C (1983) Insomnia in the San Francisco Bay area: a telephone survey. In: Guillemault C, Lugaresi E (eds) Sleep/Wake Disorders: Natural History, Epidemiology, and Long-term Evolution. Raven Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Zigmond AS, Snaith RP (1983) The hospital anxiety and depression (HAD) scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 67:361–370

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Tiffin, P., Ashton, H., Marsh, R. et al. Pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic responses to caffeine in poor and normal sleepers. Psychopharmacology 121, 494–502 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02246500

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02246500

Key words

Navigation