Skip to main content
Log in

Sensory ERP effects in auditory distraction: did we miss the main event?

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Event-related potentials (ERPs) offer unique insights into processes related to involuntary attention changes triggered by rare, unpredictably occurring sensory events, that is, distraction. Contrasting ERPs elicited by distracters and frequent standard stimuli in oddball paradigms allowed the formulation of a three-stage model describing distraction-related processing: first, the distracting event is highlighted by a sensory filter. Second, attention is oriented towards the event, and finally, the task-optimal attention set is restored, or task priorities are changed. Although this model summarizes how distracting stimulus information is processed, not much is known about the cost of taking this exceptional route of processing. The present study demonstrates the impact of distraction on sensory processing. Participants performed a Go/NoGo tone-duration discrimination task, with infrequent pitch distracters. In the two parts of the experiment the duration-response mapping was reversed. Contrasts of distracter and standard ERPs revealed higher P3a- and reorienting negativity amplitudes for short than for long tones, independently from response type. To understand the cause of these asymmetries, short vs. long ERP contrasts were calculated. The ERP pattern showed that short standards elicited an attention-dependent offset response, which was abolished for short distracters. That is, the apparent P3a- and RON enhancements were caused by the removal of a task-related attentional sensory enhancement. This shows that the disruption of task-optimal attention set precedes the elicitation of the P3a, which suggests that P3a does not reflect a process driving the initial distraction-related attention change.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bakeman, R. (2005). Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs. Behavior Research Methods, 37(3), 379–384. doi:10.3758/BF03192707.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barcelo, F., Escera, C., Corral, M. J., & Periáñez, J. A. (2006). Task switching and novelty processing activate a common neural network for cognitive control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(10), 1734–1748.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berti, S., & Schröger, E. (2001). A comparison of auditory and visual distraction effects: behavioral and event-related indices. Cognitive Brain Research, 10(3), 265–273.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dien, J., Spencer, K. M., & Donchin, E. (2004). Parsing the late positive complex: mental chronometry and the ERP components that inhabit the neighborhood of the P300. Psychophysiology, 41(5), 665–678. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00193.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Escera, C., Alho, K., Schröger, E., & Winkler, I. (2000). Involuntary attention and distractibility as evaluated with event-related brain potentials. Audiology and Neurotology, 5(3–4), 151–166.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Escera, C., & Corral, M.-J. (2003). The distraction potential (DP), an electrophysiological tracer of involuntary attention control and its dysfunction. In Reinvang, I., Greenlee, M. W., & Herrmann, M. (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of individual differences (pp. 63–76). Bibliotheksund Informationssystem der Universität Oldenburg, Oldenberg, Germany.

  • Escera, C., Yago, E., & Alho, K. (2001). Electrical responses reveal the temporal dynamics of brain events during involuntary attention switching. European Journal of Neuroscience, 14(5), 877–883.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, D., Cycowicz, Y. M., & Gaeta, H. (2001). The novelty P3: an event-related brain potential (ERP) sign of the brain’s evaluation of novelty. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25(4), 355–373.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hillyard, S. A., Hink, R. F., Schwent, V. L., & Picton, T. W. (1973). Electrical signs of selective attention in the human brain. Science, 182(4108), 177–180. doi:10.1126/science.182.4108.177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horváth, J., & Winkler, I. (2010). Distraction in a continuous-stimulation detection task. Biological Psychology, 83(3), 229–238. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.01.004.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horváth, J., Czigler, I., Birkás, E., Winkler, I., & Gervai, J. (2009). Age-related differences in distraction and reorientation in an auditory task. Neurobiology of Aging, 30(7), 1157–1172. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.10.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horváth, J., Roeber, U., Bendixen, A., & Schröger, E. (2008). Specific or general? The nature of attention set changes triggered by distracting auditory events. Brain Research, 1229, 193–203. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.06.096.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horváth, J., Maess, B., Berti, S., & Schröger, E. (2008). Primary motor area contribution to attentional reorienting after distraction. NeuroReport, 19(4), 443–446.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horváth, J., Winkler, I., & Bendixen, A. (2008). Do N1/MMN, P3a, and RON form a strongly coupled chain reflecting the three stages of auditory distraction? Biological Psychology, 79(2), 139–147. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.04.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Li, B., Parmentier, F. B. R., & Zhang, M. (2013). Behavioral distraction by auditory deviance is mediated by the sound’s informational value. Experimental Psychology, 60, 260–268. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ljungberg, J. K., Parmentier, F. B. R., Leiva, A., & Vega, N. (2012). The informational constraints of behavioral distraction by unexpected sounds: the role of event information. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 1461–1468. doi:10.1037/a0028149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2005). Detection theory: a user’s guide (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Munka, L., & Berti, S. (2006). Examining task-dependencies of different attentional processes as reflected in the P3a and reorienting negativity components of the human event-related brain potential. Neuroscience Letters, 396(3), 177–181. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2005.11.035.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Näätänen, R., Gaillard, A. W., & Mäntysalo, S. (1978). Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychologica, 42(4), 313–329.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Näätänen, Risto, & Picton, T. (1987). The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: a review and an analysis of the component structure. Psychophysiology, 24(4), 375–425. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1987.tb00311.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Näätänen, Risto, Simpson, M., & Loveless, N. E. (1982). Stimulus deviance and evoked potentials. Biological Psychology, 14(1), 53–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nuwer, M. R., Comi, G., Emerson, R., Fuglsang-Frederiksen, A., Guérit, J.-M., Hinrichs, H., Ikeda, A., Luccas, F. J. C., Rappelsburger, P. (1998). IFCN standards for digital recording of clinical EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 106(3), 259–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Okamoto, H., Stracke, H., Wolters, C. H., Schmael, F., & Pantev, C. (2007). Attention improves population-level frequency tuning in human auditory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(39), 10383–10390. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2963-07.2007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olejnik, S., & Algina, J. (2003). Generalized eta and omega squared statistics: measures of effect size for some common research designs. Psychological Methods, 8(4), 434–447. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.8.4.434.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parmentier, F. B. R., Elsley, J. V., & Ljungberg, J. K. (2010). Behavioral distraction by auditory novelty is not only about novelty: the role of the distracter’s informational value. Cognition, 115(3), 504–511. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.03.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ritter, W., Paavilainen, P., Lavikainen, J., Reinikainen, K., Alho, K., Sams, M., et al. (1992). Event-related potentials to repetition and change of auditory stimuli. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 83(5), 306–321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roeber, U., Berti, S., Müller, D., Widmann, A., & Schröger, E. (2009). Disentangling effects of auditory distraction and of stimulus-response sequence. Psychophysiology, 46(2), 425–438. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00766.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roeber, U., Berti, S., & Schröger, E. (2003). Auditory distraction with different presentation rates: an event-related potential and behavioral study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114(2), 341–349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roeber, U., Berti, S., Widmann, A., & Schröger, E. (2005). Response repetition vs. response change modulates behavioral and electrophysiological effects of distraction. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(3), 451–456. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.10.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roeber, U., Widmann, A., & Schröger, E. (2003). Auditory distraction by duration and location deviants: a behavioral and event-related potential study. Cognitive Brain Research, 17(2), 347–357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SanMiguel, I., Linden, D., & Escera, C. (2010). Attention capture by novel sounds: distraction versus facilitation. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(4), 481–515. doi:10.1080/09541440902930994.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SanMiguel, I., Morgan, H. M., Klein, C., Linden, D., & Escera, C. (2010). On the functional significance of Novelty-P3: Facilitation by unexpected novel sounds. Biological Psychology, 83(2), 143–152. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.11.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schröger, E. (1996). A neural mechanism for involuntary attention shifts to changes in auditory stimulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(6), 527–539.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schröger, E., & Wolff, C. (1998a). Attentional orienting and reorienting is indicated by human event-related brain potentials. NeuroReport, 9(15), 3355–3358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schröger, E., & Wolff, C. (1998b). Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: A new distraction paradigm. Cognitive Brain Research, 7(1), 71–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wetzel, N., Schröger, E., & Widmann, A. (2013). The dissociation between the P3a event-related potential and behavioral distraction. Psychophysiology. doi:10.1111/psyp.12072 (in press).

  • Wetzel, N., Widmann, A., & Schröger, E. (2012). Distraction and facilitation—two faces of the same coin? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 664–674. doi:10.1037/a0025856.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winkler, I. (2007). Interpreting the mismatch negativity. Journal of Psychophysiology, 21(3), 147–163. doi:10.1027/0269-8803.21.34.147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The experiment was conducted at the University of Leipzig, Institute for Psychology I, in the Cognitive including Biological Psychology Research Group. The study was supported the European Commission under the Marie Curie Intra-European Fellowship Project MEIF-CT-2006-023924, and the János Bolyai Research Scholarship of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences. The experiment was realized using Cogent 2000 developed by the Cogent 2000 team at the FIL and the ICN. I thank Jenny Kokinous for assistance in data collection. I also thank three anonymous reviewers for constructive comments and critiques.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to János Horváth.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Horváth, J. Sensory ERP effects in auditory distraction: did we miss the main event?. Psychological Research 78, 339–348 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0507-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0507-7

Keywords

Navigation