Skip to main content
Log in

Body Odor Quality Predicts Behavioral Attractiveness in Humans

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Growing effort is being made to understand how different attractive physical traits co-vary within individuals, partly because this might indicate an underlying index of genetic quality. In humans, attention has focused on potential markers of quality such as facial attractiveness, axillary odor quality, the second-to-fourth digit (2D:4D) ratio and body mass index (BMI). Here we extend this approach to include visually-assessed kinesic cues (nonverbal behavior linked to movement) which are statistically independent of structural physical traits. The utility of such kinesic cues in mate assessment is controversial, particularly during everyday conversational contexts, as they could be unreliable and susceptible to deception. However, we show here that the attractiveness of nonverbal behavior, in 20 male participants, is predicted by perceived quality of their axillary body odor. This finding indicates covariation between two desirable traits in different sensory modalities. Depending on two different rating contexts (either a simple attractiveness rating or a rating for long-term partners by 10 female raters not using hormonal contraception), we also found significant relationships between perceived attractiveness of nonverbal behavior and BMI, and between axillary odor ratings and 2D:4D ratio. Axillary odor pleasantness was the single attribute that consistently predicted attractiveness of nonverbal behavior. Our results demonstrate that nonverbal kinesic cues could reliably reveal mate quality, at least in males, and could corroborate and contribute to mate assessment based on other physical traits.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Brown, W. M., Cronk, L., Grochow, K., Jacobson, A., Liu, C. K., Popovic, Z., & Trivers, R. (2005). Dance reveals symmetry especially in young men. Nature, 438, 1148–1150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burriss, R. P., Little, A. C., & Nelson, E. C. (2007). 2D:4D and sexually dimorphic facial characteristics. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 377–384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feinberg, D. R., DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., & Little, A. C. (2008). Correlated preferences for men’s facial and vocal masculinity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 233–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Moore, F. R., Law Smith, M. J., Cornwell, R. E., … Perrett, D. I. (2005). The voice and face of woman: One ornament that signals quality? Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 398–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferdenzi, C., Lemaître, J.-F., Leongómez, J. D., & Roberts, S. C. (2011). Digit ratio (2D:4D) predicts facial, but not voice or body odour, attractiveness in men. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.0544 .

  • Ferdenzi, C., Schaal, B., & Roberts, S. C. (2009). Human axillary odor: Are there side-related perceptual differences? Chemical Senses, 34, 565–571.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fink, B., Manning, J. T., Neave, N., & Grammer, K. (2004). Second to fourth digit ratio and facial asymmetry. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 125–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fink, B., Seydel, H., Manning, J. T., & Kappeler, P. M. (2007). A preliminary investigation of the associations between digit ratio and women’s perception of men’s dance. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 381–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gangestad, S. W., & Scheyd, G. J. (2005). The evolution of human physical attractiveness. Annual Review of Anthropology, 34, 523–548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 573–644.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1998). Menstrual cycle variation in women’s preferences for the scent of symmetrical men. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 265, 927–933.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (2003). Facial masculinity and fluctuating asymmetry. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 231–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grammer, K., Fink, B., Moller, A. P., & Thornhill, R. (2003). Darwinian aesthetics: Sexual selection and the biology of beauty. Biological Reviews, 78, 385–407.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grammer, K., Honda, M., Juette, A., & Schmitt, A. (1999). Fuzziness of nonverbal courtship communication unblurred by motion energy detection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 487–508.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Havlicek, J., Dvorakova, R., Bartos, L., & Flegr, J. (2006). Non-advertized does not mean concealed: Body odour changes across the human menstrual cycle. Ethology, 112, 81–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Havlicek, J., Roberts, S. C., & Flegr, J. (2005). Women’s preference for dominant male odour: Effects of menstrual cycle and relationship status. Biology Letters, 1, 256–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hugill, N., Fink, B., & Neave, N. (2010). The role of human body movements in mate selection. Evolutionary Psychology, 8, 66–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lander, K. (2008). Relating visual and vocal attractiveness for moving and static faces. Animal Behaviour, 75, 817–822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lenochova, P., Roberts, S. C., & Havlicek, J. (2009). Methods of human body odor sampling: The effect of freezing. Chemical Senses, 34, 127–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2002). Partnership status and the temporal context of relationships influence human female preferences for sexual dimorphism in male face shape. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 269, 1095–1100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., Waitt, C., Tiddeman, B. P., Feinberg, D. R., Perrett, D. I., … Marlowe, F. W. (2008). Symmetry is related to sexual dimorphism in faces: Data across culture and species. PLos One, 3, e2106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Little, A. C., & Perrett, D. I. (2002). Putting beauty back in the eye of the beholder: Evolution and individual differences in face preference. The Psychologist, 15, 28–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maisey, D. S., Vale, E. L. E., Cornelissen, P. L., & Tovee, M. J. (1999). Characteristics of male attractiveness for women. Lancet, 353, 1500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Manning, J. T. (2002). Digit ratio: A pointer to fertility behavior and health. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manning, J. T., Fink, B., Neave, N., & Caswell, N. (2005). Photocopies yield lower digit ratios (2D:4D) than direct finger measurements. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 329–333.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McFadden, D., Loehlin, J. C., Breedlove, S. M., Lippa, R. A., Manning, J. T., & Rahman, Q. (2005). A reanalysis of five studies on sexual orientation and the relative length of the 2nd and 4th fingers (the 2D:4D ratio). Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 341–356.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, E. R., Gralewski, L., Campbell, N., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2007). Facial movement varies by sex and is related to attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 186–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neave, N., Laing, S., Fink, B., & Manning, J. T. (2003). Second to fourth digit ratio, testosterone and perceived male dominance. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 270, 2167–2172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paul, S. N., Kato, B. S., Cherkas, L. F., Andrew, T., & Spector, T. D. (2006). Heritability of the second to fourth digit ratio (2d:4d): A twin study. Twin Research in Human Genetics, 9, 215–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pawlowski, B., & Jasienska, G. (2008). Women’s body morphology and preferences for sexual partners’ characteristics. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 19–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penton-Voak, I. S., Allen, T., Morrison, E. R., Gralewski, L., & Campbell, N. (2007). Performance on a face perception task is associated with empathy quotient scores, but not systemizing scores or participant sex. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 2229–2236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penton-Voak, I. S., & Chang, H. Y. (2008). Attractiveness judgements of individuals vary across emotional expression and movement conditions. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 6, 89–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renninger, L. A., Wade, T. J., & Grammer, K. (2004). Getting that female glance: Patterns and consequences of male nonverbal behavior in courtship contexts. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 416–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 199–226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riggio, R. E., Widaman, K. F., Tucker, J. S., & Salinas, C. (1991). Beauty is more than skin deep—Components of attractiveness. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 12, 423–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rikowski, A., & Grammer, K. (1999). Human body odour, symmetry and attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 266, 869–874.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, S. C. (2008). Nonverbal communication of mate quality in humans. In P. D’Ettorre & D. P. Hughes (Eds.), Sociobiology of communication: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 157–170). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, S. C., Gosling, L. M., Carter, V., & Petrie, M. (2008). MHC-correlated odour preferences in humans and the use of oral contraceptives. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 275, 2715–2722.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, S. C., Gosling, L. M., Spector, T. D., Miller, P., Penn, D. J., & Petrie, M. (2005). Body odor similarity in non-cohabiting twins. Chemical Senses, 30, 651–656.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, S. C., Havlicek, J., Flegr, J., Hruskova, M., Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., … Petrie, M. (2004). Female facial attractiveness increases during the fertile phase of the menstrual cycle. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 271(Suppl.), S270–S272.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, S. C., & Little, A. C. (2008). Good genes, complementary genes and human mate choice. Genetica, 132, 309–321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, S. C., Little, A. C., Gosling, L. M., Jones, B. C., Perrett, D. I., Carter, V., & Petrie, M. (2005). MHC-assortative facial preferences in humans. Biology Letters, 1, 400–403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, S. C., Little, A. C., Lyndon, A., Roberts, J., Havlicek, J., & Wright, R. L. (2009). Manipulation of body odor alters men’s self-confidence and judgements of their visual attractiveness by women. International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 31, 47–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, S. C., Saxton, T. K., Murray, A. K., Burriss, R. P., Rowland, H. M., & Little, A. C. (2009). Static and dynamic facial images cue similar attractiveness judgements. Ethology, 115, 588–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubenstein, A. J. (2005). Variation in perceived attractiveness: Differences between dynamic and static faces. Psychological Science, 16, 759–762.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saxton, T. K., Burriss, R. P., Murray, A. K., Rowland, H. M., & Roberts, S. C. (2009). Face, body and speech cues independently predict judgments of attractiveness. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 7, 23–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saxton, T. K., Caryl, P. G., & Roberts, S. C. (2006). Vocal and facial attractiveness judgments of children, adolescents and adults: The ontogeny of mate choice. Ethology, 112, 1179–1185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saxton, T. K., DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., & Roberts, S. C. (2009). Face and voice attractiveness judgments change during adolescence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 398–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B., Tesser, A., & Powell, E. (1982). Dominance cues in nonverbal behavior. Social Psychology Quarterly, 45, 114–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1999). Facial attractiveness. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3, 452–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornhill, R., Gangestad, S. W., Miller, R., Scheyd, G., McCullough, J. K., & Franklin, M. (2003). Major histocompatibility genes, symmetry and body scent attractiveness in men and women. Behavioral Ecology, 14, 668–678.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornhill, R., & Grammer, K. (1999). The body and face of woman: One ornament that signals quality? Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 105–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgment

JH is supported by the Czech Science Foundation grant GACR 406/09/0647.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to S. Craig Roberts.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Roberts, S.C., Kralevich, A., Ferdenzi, C. et al. Body Odor Quality Predicts Behavioral Attractiveness in Humans. Arch Sex Behav 40, 1111–1117 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-011-9803-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-011-9803-8

Keywords

Navigation