Skip to main content
Log in

Bias and Accuracy of Children's Perceptions of Peer Acceptance: Prospective Associations with Depressive Symptoms

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Are depressive symptoms in middle childhood associated with more or less realistic social self-perceptions? At the beginning and end of the school year, children in grades 3 through 5 (n=667) rated how much they liked their classmates, predicted the acceptance ratings they would receive from each of their classmates, and completed self-report measures of perceived acceptance and depressive symptoms. Accuracy of perceived acceptance was indexed by the mean difference between pairs of predicted and received ratings (absolute values). Standardized residual scores created by regressing self-reported perceived acceptance (either predicted ratings or children's responses to a questionnaire measure of perceived peer acceptance) onto peer acceptance ratings formed two measures of bias. Bi-directional associations were found for accuracy of perceived acceptance and depressive symptoms; inaccurate perceptions predicted increases in depressive symptoms and depressive symptoms predicted decreased accuracy. Neither measure of bias predicted changes in depressive symptoms. Depressive symptoms predicted increases in negatively biased perceptions as assessed via questionnaire.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The same pattern of results was found when analyses were conducted with perceived acceptance (i.e., Social Acceptance scores) instead of Bias/SA (results available on request). Perceived acceptance did not predict changes in depressive symptoms but depressive symptoms predicted more negative perceptions of acceptance.

  2. The same pattern of results was found when analyses were conducted with Predicted Ratings instead of Bias/PR (results available on request). Predicted ratings did not predict changes in depressive symptoms and depressive symptoms did not predict changes in predicted ratings.

  3. Additional data analysis conducted to determine if Time 2 peer-rated acceptance mediated the prospective link between accuracy and changes in depressive symptoms failed to support this hypothesis (results available upon request). Accuracy did not predict changes in peer acceptance across the school year, one of the conditions for testing a mediation model (Baron & Kenny, 1986), nor was the contribution of accuracy to changes in depressive symptoms diminished when Time 2 peer acceptance was included in the regression analysis.

REFERENCES

  • Ackerman, R., & DeRubeis, R. J. (1991). Is depressive realism real? Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 565–584.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgement of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 109, 441–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

  • Anderson, J. C., Williams, S., McGee, R., & Silva, P. A. (1987). DSM-III disorders in preadolescent children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 69–76.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Asendorpf, J. B., & Ostendorf, F. (1998). Is self-enhancement healthy? Conceptual, psychometric, and empirical analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 955–966.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Balthazor, M. J. (1995). Adolescents’ perceptions of their social acceptance: The relationship between illusory self-perceptions and depressive symptomology. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Florida State University, Tallahassee.

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper and Row.

  • Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford.

  • Campbell, J. D., & Fehr, B. (1990). Self-esteem and perceptions of conveyed impressions: Is negative affectivity associated with greater realism? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 122–133.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cillesen, A. H. N., & Bellmore, A. D. (1999). Accuracy of social self-perceptions and peer competence in middle childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 650–676.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, D. A., Maxwell, S. E., & Martin, J. M. (1997). Reflected self-appraisals: Strength and structure of the relation of teacher, peer, and parent ratings to children's self-perceived competencies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 55–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, D. A., Martin, J. M., Peeke, L. G., Seroczynski, A. D., & Fier, J. (1999). Children's over- and underestimation of academic competence: A longitudinal study of gender differences, depression, and anxiety. Child Development, 70, 459–473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, D. A., Martin, J. M., Peeke, L. G., Seroczynski, A. D., & Hoffman, K. (1998). Are cognitive errors of underestimation predictive or reflective of depressive symptoms in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 481–496.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colvin, C. R., Block, J., & Funder, D. C. (1995). Overly positive self-evaluations and personality: Negative implications for mental health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 1152–1162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coyne, J. C. (1976). Toward an interactional description of depression. Psychiatry, 39, 28–40.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children's social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damon, W., & Hart, D. (1988). Self-understanding in childhood and adolescence. New York: Cambridge University Press.

  • Dodge, K. A. (1986). A social information-processing model of social competence in children. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.), The Minnesota symposia on child psychology, Vol. 18. Cognitive perspectives in children's social and behavioral development (pp. 77–125). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Giesler, R. B., Josephs, R. A., & Swann, W. B. (1996). Self-verification in clinical depression: The desire for negative evaluation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 358–368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haaga, D. A. F., & Beck, A. T. (1995). Perspectives on depressive realism: Implications for cognitive theory of depression. Behavior Research and Therapy, 33, 41–48

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammen, C., & Compas, B. (1994). Unmasking masked depression: The problem of co morbidity in child and adolescent depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 14, 585–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harter, S. (1985). Self-Perception Profile for Children. Denver, CO: University of Denver.

  • Harter, S. (1998). The development of self-representations. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 103–196). New York: Wiley.

  • Hartup, W. (1983). Peer relations. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.) & P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development (pp. 103–196). New York: Wiley.

  • Hoffman, K. B., Cole, D. A., Martin, J. M., Tram, J., & Seroczynski, A. D. (2000). Are the discrepancies between self- and others’ appraisals of competence predictive or reflective of depressive symptoms in children and adolescents: A longitudinal study, Part II. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 651–662.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jahoda, M. (1958). Current concepts of positive mental health. New York: Basic Books.

  • Jorm A. (2000). Is depression a risk factor for dementia or cognitive decline? A review. Gerontology, 46, 219–227.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A., Esveldt-Dawson, K., Unis, A., & Rancurello, M. (1983). Child and parent evaluations of depression among psychiatrically disturbed children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 11, 401–413.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, D. A., & DePaulo, B. M. (1993). Do people know how others view them? An empirical and theoretical account. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 145–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kistner, J. A., Balthazor, M., Risi, S., & David, C. (2001). Adolescents’ perceptions of peer acceptance: Is dysphoria associated with greater realism? Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 20, 69–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kistner, J. A., David, C. F., Lonigan, C. J., & Hooe, E. (2000). Accuracy of children's perceptions of their peer acceptance: Is ignorance bliss? Unpublished manuscript.

  • Kovacs, M. (1992). Children's depression inventory manual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.

  • Lewinsohn, P. M., Hops, H., Roberts, R. E., Seeley, J. R., & Andrews, J. A. (1993). Adolescent psychopathology: I. Prevalence and incidence of depression and other DSM-III-R disorders in high school students. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 133–144.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewinsohn, P. M., Steinmetz, J., Larson, D., & Franklin, J. (1981). Depression related cognitions: Antecedents or consequences? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 213–219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malloy, T. E., Yarlas, A., Montvilo, R. K., & Sugarman, D. B. (1996). Agreement and accuracy in children's interpersonal perceptions: A social relations analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 692–702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGee, R., Feehan, M., Williams, S., Partridge, F., Silva, P. A., & Kelly, J. (1990). DSM-III disorders in a large sample of adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 611–619.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGrath, E. P., & Repetti, R. L. (2002). A longitudinal study of children's depressive symptoms, self-perceptions, and cognitive distortions about the self. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 77–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Girgus, J. S., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1992). Predictors and consequences of childhood depressive symptoms: A 5 year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 405–422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rohde, P., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Seeley, J. R. (1990). Are people changed by the experience of having a depressive episodes? A further test of the scar hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 264–271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, C. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality, and interpersonal relations, developed in the client-centered framework. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 3, pp. 185–256). New York: McGraw-Hill.

  • Saylor, C. F., Finch, A. J., Spirito, A., & Bennett, B. (1984). The Children's Depression Inventory: A systematic evaluation of psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 955–967.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Segal, Z. V. (1988). Appraisal of the self-schema construct in cognitive models of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 147–162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swann, W. B., Jr. (1983). Self-verification: Bringing social reality into harmony with the self. In J. Suls & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Social psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 2, pp. 33–66). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Taylor, S. E., Lerner, J. S., Sherman, D. K., Sage, R. M., & McDowell, N. K. (2003). Portrait of the self-enhancer: Well-adjusted and well-liked or maladjusted and friendless? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 165–176.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verhulst, F. C., Van Der Ende, J., Ferdinand, R. F., & Kasius, M. C. (1997). The prevalence of DSM-III-R diagnoses in a national sample of Dutch adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 329–336.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weisz, J. R., Rudolph, K. D., Granger, D. A., & Sweeney, L. (1992). Cognition, competence, and coping in child and adolescent depression: Research findings, developmental concerns, therapeutic implications. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 627–653.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitman, P. B., & Leitenberg, H. (1990). Negatively biased recall in children with self-reported symptoms of depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 15–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zeiss, A. M., & Lewinsohn, P. J. (1988). Enduring deficits after remissions of depression: A test of the scar hypothesis. Behavior Research and Therapy, 26, 151–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Janet A. Kistner.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kistner, J.A., David-Ferdon, C.F., Repper, K.K. et al. Bias and Accuracy of Children's Perceptions of Peer Acceptance: Prospective Associations with Depressive Symptoms. J Abnorm Child Psychol 34, 336–348 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-006-9028-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-006-9028-9

Key Words:

Navigation