Skip to main content
Log in

Applying evolutionary theory to human behaviour: past differences and current debates

  • Published:
Journal of Bioeconomics Aims and scope

Abstract

The aim of this paper is to provide non-specialist readers with an introduction to some current controversies surrounding the application of evolutionary theory to human behaviour at the intersection of biology, psychology and anthropology. We review the three major contemporary sub-fields; namely Human Behavioural Ecology, Evolutionary Psychology and Cultural Evolution, and we compare their views on maladaptive behaviour, the proximal mechanisms of cultural transmission, and the relationship between human cognition and culture. For example, we show that the sub-fields vary in the amount of maladaptive behaviour that is predicted to occur in modern environments; Human Behavioural Ecologists start with the expectation that behaviour will be optimal, while Evolutionary Psychologists emphasize cases of ‘mis-match’ between modern environments and domain-specific, evolved psychological mechanisms. Cultural Evolutionists argue that social learning processes are effective at providing solutions to novel problems and describe how relatively weak, general-purpose learning mechanisms, alongside accurate cultural transmission, can lead to the cumulative evolution of adaptive cultural complexity but also sometimes to maladaptative behaviour. We then describe how the sub-fields view cooperative behaviour between non-kin, as an example of where the differences between the sub-fields are relevant to the economics community, and we discuss the hypothesis that a history of inter-group competition can explain the evolution of non-kin cooperation. We conclude that a complete understanding of human behaviour requires insights from all three fields and that many scholars no longer view them as distinct.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. See a debate initiated by Steven Pinker’s essay The False Allure of Group Selection in the online magazine Edge (http://www.edge.org/conversation/the-false-allure-of-group-selection).

References

  • Alcock, J. (2001). The Triumph of Sociobiology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, E., Beckwith, B., Beckwith, J., Chorover, S., Culver, D., Duncan, M., et al. (1975). Letter. New York Review of Books, 182, 184–186.

  • Aoki, M. (2001). Toward a comparative institutional analysis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atran, S., & Ginges, J. (2012). Religious and sacred imperatives in human conflict. Science, 336, 855–857.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, H. C. (2012). A hierarchical model of the evolution of human brain specializations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 109, 10733–10740.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basalla, G. (1988). The evolution of technology. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baugh, T. G., & Ericson, J. E. (1994). Prehistoric exchange systems in North America. New York, NY: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, A. V., Richerson, P. J., & McElreath, R. (2009). Culture rather than genes provides greater scope for the evolution of large-scale human prosociality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 106, 17671–17674.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boakes, R. (1984). From Darwin to behaviourism: Psychology and the minds of animals. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolhuis, J. J., Brown, G. R., Richardson, R. C., & Laland, K. N. (2011). Darwin in mind: New opportunities for evolutionary psychology. PLoS Biology, 9, e1001109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borgerhoff Mulder, M. (1991). Human behavioural ecology. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borgerhoff Mulder, M. (1998). The demographic transition: Are we any closer to an evolutionary explanation? Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 44, 266–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borgerhoff Mulder, M., & Beheim, B. A. (2011). Understanding the nature of wealth and its effects on human fitness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 366, 344–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borgerhoff Mulder, M., Bowles, S., Hertz, T., Bell, A., Beise, J., Clark, G., Fazzio, I., Gurven, M., Hill, K., Hooper, P.L., Irons, W., Kaplan, H., Leonetti, D., Low, B., Marlowe, F., McElreath, R., Naidu, S., Nolin, D., Piraino, P., Quinlan, R., Schniter, E., Sear, R., Shenk, M., Smith, E.A., von Rueden, C., & Wiessner, P. (2009). The intergenerational transmission of wealth and the dynamics of inequality in pre-modern societies. Science, 326, 682–688.

  • Borgerhoff Mulder, M., Richerson, P. J., Thornhill, N. W., & Voland, E. (1997). The place of behavioral ecological anthropology in evolutionary social science. In P. Weingart, S. D. Mitchell, P. J. Richerson, & S. Maasen (Eds.), Human by nature: Between biology and the social sciences (pp. 253–282). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borgerhoff Mulder, M., & Schacht, R. (2012). Human behavioural ecology. In Encyclopedia of life sciences (pp. 1–10). Chichester: Wiley. doi:10.1002/9780470015902.a0003671.pub2.

  • Bowden, M. (1991). Pitt Rivers: The life and archaeological work of lieutenant-general Augustus Henry Lane Fox Pitt Rivers. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (2011). A cooperative species: Human reciprocity and its evolution. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2009). Voting with your feet: Payoff biased migration and the evolution of group beneficial behavior. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 257, 331–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011a). The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 108, 10918–10925.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011b). Rapid cultural adaptation can facilitate the evolution of large-scale cooperation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 65, 431–444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broughton, J. M., & O’Connell, J. F. (1999). On evolutionary ecology, selectionist archaeology, and behavioral archaeology. American Antiquity, 64, 153–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. R. (2013). Why mechanisms shouldn’t be ignored. Behavioral Ecology, 24, 1041–1042.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. R., Dickins, T., Sear, R., & Laland, K. N. (2011). Evolutionary accounts of human behavioural diversity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 366, 313–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buchsbaum, D., Gopnik, A., Griffiths, T. L., & Shafto, P. (2011). Children’s imitation of causal action sequences is influenced by statistical and pedagogical evidence. Cognition, 120, 331–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (Ed.). (2005). The handbook of evolutionary psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chagnon, N. A., & Irons, W. (1979). Evolutionary biology and human social behavior: An anthropological perspective. North Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 489–558.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chudek, M., Heller, S., Birch, S., & Henrich, J. (2012). Prestige-biased cultural learning: Bystander’s differential attention to potential models influence children’s learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 46–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chudek, M., & Henrich, J. (2011). Culture-gene coevolution, norm-psychology and the emergence of human prosociality. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 218–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chudek, M., Zhao, W., & Henrich, J. (2013). Culture-gene coevolution, large-scale cooperation and the shaping of human social psychology. In B. Calcott, R. Joyce, & K. Sterelny (Eds.), Signaling, commitment, and emotion. Boston, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1987). From evolution to behavior: Evolutionary psychology as the missing link. In J. Dupré (Ed.), The latest on the best: Essays on evolution and optimality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31, 187–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides & J. (Eds.), The adapted mind. Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 163–228). New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1997). Evolutionary psychology: A primer. Center for Evolutionary Psychology, University of California Santa Barbara. Retrieved from http://www.psych.ucsb.edu/research/cep/primer.html. Accessed 30 Aug 2013.

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2001). Unravelling the enigma of human intelligence: Evolutionary psychology and the multimodular mind. In R. J. Sternberg & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), The evolution of intelligence (pp. 145–199). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Courtiol, A., Pettay, J. E., Jokela, M., Rotkirch, A., & Lummaa, V. (2012). Natural and sexual selection in a monogamous historical human population. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 109, 8044–8049.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2011). Natural pedagogy as an evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 1149–1157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Currie, T. E., Greenhill, S. J., Gray, R. D., Hasegawa, T., & Mace, R. (2010). Rise and fall of political complexity in island South-East Asia and the Pacific. Nature, 467, 801–804.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danchin, É., Giraldeau, L.-A., & Cézilly, F. (Eds.). (2008). Behavioural ecology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delton, A. W., Krasnow, M. M., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2011). Evolution of direct reciprocity under uncertainty can explain human generosity in one-shot encounters. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 108, 13335–13340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Efferson, C., & Richerson, P. J. (2007). A prolegomenon to non-linear empiricism in the human sciences. Biology and Philosophy, 22, 1–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eldakar, O. T., & Wilson, D. S. (2011). Eight criticisms not to make about group selection. Evolution, 65, 1523–1526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 429–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Gachter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415, 137–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425, 785–791.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grafen, A. (1984). Natural selection, kin selection, and group selection. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach (pp. 62–84). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, R. D., Atkinson, Q. D., & Greenhill, S. J. (2011). Language evolution and human history: What a difference a date makes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 1090–1100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagen, E. H., & Hammerstein, P. (2006). Game theory and human evolution: A critique of some recent interpretations of experimental games. Theoretical Population Biology, 69, 339–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L. (2012). Trusting what you’re told: How children learn from others. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haslam, S. A. (2001). Psychology in organizations: The social identity approach. London: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298, 1569–1579.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2009). The evolution of costly displays, cooperation and religion: Credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 244–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Gintis, H., Fehr, E., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., McElreath, R., Alvard, M., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Henrich, N. S., Hill, K., Gil-White, F., Gurven, M., Marlowe, F. W., Patton, J.Q., & Tracer, D. (2005). ‘Economic Man’ in cross-cultural perspective: Ethnography and experiments from 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 795–855.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Broesch, J. (2011). On the nature of cultural transmission networks: Evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 1139–1148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Ensimger, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J. C., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N., Lesorogol, C., Marlowe, F., Tracer, D., & Ziker, J. (2010). Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science, 327, 1480–1484.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige—freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 165–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2002). Are peasants risk averse decision-makers. Current Anthropology, 43, 172–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. M., & Hull, D. L. (2001). Selection theory and social construction: The evolutionary naturalistic epistemology of Donald T. Campbell. New York, NY: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K., Barton, M., & Hurtado, A. M. (2009). The emergence of human uniqueness: Characters underlying behavioral modernity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 18, 174–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K. R., Walker, R. S., Božičević, M., Eder, J., Headland, T., Hewlett, B., Hurtado, A. M., Marlowe, F., Wiessner, P., & Wood, B. (2011). Co-residence patterns in hunter-gatherer societies show unique human social structure. Science, 331, 1286–1286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson, G. M., & Knudsen, T. (2008). In search of general evolutionary principles: Why Darwinism is too important to be left to the biologists. Journal of Bioeconomics, 10, 51–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurford, J. R. (2011). The origins of grammar: Language in the light of evolution II. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology. New York, NY: Henry Holt & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H. (1994). Evolutionary and wealth flows theories of fertility: Empirical tests and new models. Population and Development Review, 20, 753–791.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H. S., & Gangestad, S. W. (2005). Life history theory and evolutionary psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology (pp. 68–95). New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendal, J., Tehrani, J. J., & Odling-Smee, J. (2011). Human niche construction in interdisciplinary focus. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 785–792.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennett, D. J. (2005). The Island Chumash: Behavioral ecology of a maritime society. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. G. (2009). The human career: Human biological and cultural origins (3rd ed.). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krasnow, M. M., Cosmides, L., Pedersen, E. J., & Tooby, J. (2012). What are punishment and reputation for? PLoS ONE, 7, e45662.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., & Brown, G. R. (2006). Niche construction, human behaviour and the adaptive-lag hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 15, 95–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., & Brown, G. R. (2011). Sense and nonsense: Evolutionary perspectives on human behaviour (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., & Feldman, M. W. (2000). Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 131–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, F. J., & Feldman, M. W. (2001). Cultural niche construction and human evolution. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 14, 22–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, F. J., Hoppitt, W., & Uller, T. (2012). More on how and why: Cause and effect in biology revisited. Biology and Philosophy, 28, 719–745.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, F. J., & Myles, S. (2010). How culture shaped the human genome: Bringing genetics and the human sciences together. Nature Reviews Genetics, 11, 137–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Sterelny, K., Odling-Smee, J., Hoppitt, W., & Uller, T. (2011). Cause and effect in biology revisited: Is Mayr’s proximate-ultimate dichotomy still useful? Science, 334, 1512–1516.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loulergue, L., Schilt, A., Spahni, R., Masson-Delmotte, V., Blunier, T., Lemieux, B., Barnola, J. M., Raynaud D., Stocker, T. F., & Chappellaz, J. (2008). Orbital and millennial-scale features of atmospheric CH4 over the past 800,000 years. Nature, 453, 383–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawson, D. W., Alvergne, A., & Gibson, M. A. (2012). The life-history trade-off between fertility and child survival. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 279(1748), 4755–4764.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawson, D. W., & Mace, R. (2011). Parental investment and the optimization of human family size. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 333–343.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lumsden, C. J., & Wilson, E. O. (1981). Genes, mind, and culture: The coevolutionary process. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mace, R., & Jordan, F. M. (2011). Macro-evolutionary studies of cultural diversity: A review of empirical studies of cultural transmission and cultural adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 402–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathew, S., & Boyd, R. (2011). Punishment sustains large-scale cooperation in prestate warfare. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 108, 11375–11380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathew, S., Boyd, R., & Van Veelen, M. (In press). Human cooperation among kin and close associates. In P. J. Richerson & M. Christiansen, (eds). Cultural Evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • McElreath, R., Lubell, M., Richerson, P. J., Waring, T. M., Baum, W., Edsten, F., Efferson, C., & Paciotti, B. (2005). Applying evolutionary models to the laboratory study of social learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 483–508.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A. (2011). Cultural evolution: How Darwinian theory can explain human culture and synthesize the social sciences. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., & O’Brien, M. J. (2008). The cultural transmission of Great Basin projectile-point technology I: An experimental simulation. American Antiquity, 73, 3–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, T. J. H., Rendell, L. E., Ehn, M., Hoppitt, W. J. E., & Laland, K. N. (2012). The evolutionary basis of human social learning. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 1729, 653–662.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nakahashi, W., Wakano, J. Y., & Henrich, J. (2012). Adaptive social learning strategies in temporally and spatially varying environments: How temporal vs spatial variation, number of cultural traits, and costs of learning influence the evolution of conformist-biased transmission, payoff-biased transmission, and individual learning. Human Nature, 23, 386–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Research Council, (2002). Abrupt climate change: Inevitable surprises. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, R. R., & Winter, S. G. (1982). An evolutionary theory of economic change. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nettle, D., Gibson, M. A., Lawson, D. W., & Sear, R. (2013). Human behavioral ecology: Current research and future prospects. Behavioral Ecology, 24, 1031–1040.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newmeyer, F. J. (2004). Against a parameter-setting approach to typological variation. Linguistic Variation Yearbook, 4, 181–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newson, L., Postmes, T., Lea, S. E. G., Webley, P. M., Richerson, P. J., & McElreath, R. (2007). Influences on communication about reproduction: The cultural evolution of low fertility. Evolution & Human Behavior, 28(3), 199–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newson, L., & Richerson, P. J. (2009). Why do people become modern: A Darwinian mechanism. Population and Development Review, 35, 117–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norenzayan, A., & Gervais, W. (2012). The cultural evolution of religion. In E. Slingerland & M. Collard (Eds.), Creating consilience: Integrating science and the humanities (pp. 243–265). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K. N., & Feldman, M. W. (2003). Niche construction: The neglected process in evolution. Monographs in Population Biology 37. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perreault, C. (2012). The pace of cultural evolution. PLoS ONE, 7, e45150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perreault, C., Moya, C., & Boyd, R. (2012). A Bayesian approach to the evolution of social learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 449–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, G. H., Dominy, N. J., Claw, K. G., Lee, A. S., Fiegler, H., Redon, R., Werner, J., Villanea, F. A., Mountain, J. L., Misra, R., Carter, N. P., Lee, C. & Stone, A. C. (2007). Diet and the evolution of human amylase gene copy number variation. Nature Genetics, 39, 1256–1260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petroski, H. (1992). The evolution of useful things. New York, NY: Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York, NY: W. Morrow & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (2010). The cognitive niche: Coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107, 8993–8999.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., & Bloom, P. (1990). Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 707–784.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plotkin, H. (1994). Darwin machines and the nature of knowledge. New York, NY: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, A., Shennan, S., & Thomas, M. G. (2009). Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science, 324, 1298–1301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., Bettinger, R. L., & Boyd, R. (2005). Evolution on a restless planet: Were environmental variability and environmental change major drivers of human evolution? In F. M. Wuketits & F. J. Ayala (Eds.), Handbook of evolution: Evolution of living systems (including hominids) (pp. 223–242). Weinheim: Wiley-VCH.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2010a). Gene–culture coevolution in the age of genomics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107, 8985–8992.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2010b). Why possibly language evolved. Biolinguistics, 4, 289–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P., & Henrich, J. (2012). Tribal social instincts and the cultural evolution of institutions to solve collective action problems. Cliodynamics, 3, 38–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, S., Lewontin, R. C., & Kamin, L. J. (1984). Not in our genes: Biology, ideology, and human nature. London: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segestråle, U. (2000). Defenders of the truth: The sociobiology debate. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A. (2000). Three styles in the evolutionary analysis of human behavior. In L. Cronk, N. Chagnon, & W. Irons (Eds.), Adaptation and human behavior: An anthropological perspective (pp. 27–46). New York, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A. (2010). Communication and collective action: Language and the evolution of human cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 231–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A., Borgerhoff Mulder, M., & Hill, K. (2001). Controversies in the evolutionary social sciences: A guide for the perplexed. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 16, 128–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, K., & Kirby, S. (2008). Cultural evolution: Implications for the human language faculty and its evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 3591–3603.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, J. P., Blumberg, M. S., McMurray, B., Robinson, S. R., Samuelson, L. K., & Tomblin, J. B. (2009). Short arms and talking eggs: Why we should no longer abide the nativist-empiricist debate. Child Development Perspectives, 3, 79–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1984). Anthropology and psychology: Towards an epidemiology of representations. Man, 20, 73–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stearns, S. C., Byars, S. G., Govindaraju, D. R., & Ewbank, D. (2010). Measuring selection in contemporary human populations. Nature Reviews Genetics, 11, 611–622.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (2012). The evolved apprentice: How evolution made humans unique. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Symons, D. (1989). A critique of Darwinian anthropology. Ethology and Sociobiology, 10, 131–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 2405–2415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1996). Do apes ape? In B. G. Galef Jr (Ed.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 319–346). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind. Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 137–159). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & DeVore, I. (1987). The reconstruction of hominid behavioral evolution through strategic modeling. In W. G. Kinzey (Ed.), The evolution of human behavior: Primate models (pp. 183–237). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turchin, P. (2009). A theory for the formation of large empires. Journal of Global History, 4, 191–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 2417–2428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S., van Vugt, M., & O’Goram, R. (2008). Multilevel selection theory and major evolutionary transitions: Implications for psychological science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 6–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The new synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. O. (1978). On human nature. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. O. (1998). Consilience: The unity of knowledge. London: Abacus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witt, U. (1999). Bioeconomics as economics from a Darwinian perspective. Journal of Bioeconomics, 1, 19–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zachos, J., Pagani, M., Sloan, L., Thomas, E., & Billups, K. (2001). Trends, rhythms, and aberrations in global climate 65 Ma to present. Science, 292, 686–693.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This article stems from the authors’ attendance at a Max Planck symposium on ‘Biological determinants and contingencies of economic behavior’ at the Ringberg Castle, Munich. We thank the organiser, Prof. Ulrich Witt for the invitation to participate in the symposium, and we are grateful to the other participants for many stimulating discussions. We also grateful for comments on the manuscript from Curtis Atkisson, Clark Barrett, Rob Boyd, Joe Henrich, Robert Kurzban, Monique Borgerhoff Mulder, Kevin Laland, Lesley Newson, Ryan Schacht and three anonymous reviewers.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gillian R. Brown.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Brown, G.R., Richerson, P.J. Applying evolutionary theory to human behaviour: past differences and current debates. J Bioecon 16, 105–128 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10818-013-9166-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10818-013-9166-4

Keywords

Navigation