Skip to main content
Log in

Executive Function Differences Between Bilingual Arabic–English and Monolingual Arabic Children

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study aimed to explore the differences between Arabic–English bilingual and monolingual Arabic children on a battery of executive functions. Prior research on the influence of bilingualism on cognitive abilities and executive functions has shown mixed results. Some results suggested that bilinguals perform significantly better than monolinguals, while others showed that monolinguals perform significantly better. Other studies showed no significant differences between both groups, findings which were argued to be due to methodological issues. A total of 50 Arabic monolingual and Arabic–English bilingual children ranging 7–10 years of age participated in the current study. Six executive function tasks, divided into two categories (inhibition of improper response tasks, and behavioral operational control tasks), were administered. Results did not show significant differences for most executive functions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

(*) are the transliteration of Arabic reference; (**) are the translation of the Arabic references.

  • *Abdelgafar, G. (2004). Alwazaef Altanfeziyah Lada Aienah min Alatfal thawe Edetirab Aldeslexia. Majalat Kuliyat Aladab Jameat Bani Suaif, 7, 142–175.

  • **Abdelgafar, G. (2004). Executive functions among a sample of dyslexic children. Journal of Arts, Bani-Suaif University, 7, 142–175.

  • *Abdelgafar, G. (2008). Edetirab Alqiraeh Alertiqaei min Manzur Nafsi Maerifi Eiyadi. Alqaherah: Dar Etrac Lilnasher.

  • **Abdelgafar, G. (2008). Developmental Dyslexia in Perspective of Clinical Cognitive Psychology. Cairo: Etrac Publisher Institute.

  • *Abdelkawi, S. (2011). Elim Alnafs Alasabi. Alqaherah: Maktabat Alanglo Almasreya.

  • **Abdelkawi, S. (2011). Neuropsychology. Cairo: Anglo EgyptianBookshop.

  • *Abdeltawab, N. (2007). Alosus Alasabeya wa Alnafsiya lilwazaeif Altanfiziyah: Tatbiqat ala Kibar Alsin. Alqaherah: Dar Etrac Lilnasher.

  • **Abdeltawab, N. (2007). Neuropsychological Fundamentals of Executive Functions: Implications of Elders. Cairo: Etrac Publisher Institute.

  • Allman, B. (2005). Vocabulary size and accuracy of monolingual and bilingual preschool children. In J. Cohen, K. McAlister, K. Rolstad, & J. MacSwan (Eds.), Proceedings of the 4th international symposium on bilingualism (pp. 58–77). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. (1995). Cognitive psychology and its implications. New York: W.H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 71–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, C. (2011). Foundations of bilingual education and bilingualism (5th ed.). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70, 636–644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. In E. Bialystok (Ed.), Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition (pp. 1–300). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2005). Consequences of bilingualism for cognitive development. In J. Kroll & A. Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 417–432). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., & Codd, J. (1997). Cardinal limits: Evidence from language awareness and bilingualism for developing concepts of number. Cognitive Development, 12, 85–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychol Aging, 19, 290–303.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., & Feng, X. (2009). Language proficiency and executive control in proactive interference: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual children and adults. Brain and Language, 109, 93–100.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Luk, G., & Kwan, E. (2005). Bilingualism, biliteracy, and learning to read: interactions among languages and writing systems. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(1), 43–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., & Majumder, S. (1998). The relationship between bilingualism and the development of cognitive processes in problem-solving. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 69–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., & Viswanathan, M. (2009). Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition, 112, 494–500.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C., Zelazo, P., & Greenberg, M. (2005). The measurement of executive function in young children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 561–571.

  • Bloomfield, L. (1933). Language. New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bondi, M., Serody, B., Chan, A., Eberson-Shumate, S., Delis, D., Lawrence, H., et al. (2002). Cognitive and neuropathologic correlates of stroop color-word test performance in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 16(3), 335–343.

  • Carlson, S., & Meltzoff, A. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11, 282–298.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Catale, C., Willems, S., Lejeune, C., & Meulemans, T. (2012). Parental educational level influence on memory and executive performance in children. Revue européenne de psychologie appliqué, 62, 161–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, A. (2005). Lexical access in bilingual production. In J. Kroll & A. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 308–328). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deuchar, M., & Quay, S. (1998). One vs. two systems in early bilingual syntax: Two versions of the question. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(3), 231–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (2011). Biological and social influences on cognitive control processes dependent on prefrontal cortex. Progress in Brain Research, 189, 319–339.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diaz, R., & Klingler, C. (2000). Towards an explanatory model of the interaction between bilingualism and cognitive development. In E. Bialystok (Ed.), Language processing in bilingual children. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fama, R., Sullivan, E., Shear, P., Cahn-Weiner, D., Marsh, L., Lim, K., et al. (2000). Structural brain correlates of verbal and non verbal fluency measures in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 14, 29–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, C., & Shen, M. (1971). Some language-related cognitive advantages of bilingual 5-year-olds. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 118, 235–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, C. (1959). Diglossia. Word, 15, 325–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Festman, J., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Münte, T. (2010). Individual differences in control of language interference in late bilinguals are mainly related to general executive abilities. Behavioral and Brain Function 6(5) doi:10.1186/1744-9081-6-5.

  • Gathercole, V. C., Thomas, E., Jones, L., Guasch, N., Young, N., & Hughes, E. (2010). Cognitive effects of bilingualism: Digging deeper for the contributions of language dominance, linguistic knowledge, socio-economic status and cognitive abilities. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13(5), 617–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grosjean, F. (1989). Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Brain Language, 36, 3–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hakuta, K. (1986). Mirror of language: The debate on bilingualism. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hakuta, K., & Diaz, R. (1985). The relationship between degree of bilingualism and cognitive ability: A critical discussion and some new longitudinal data. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Children’s language (Vol. 5). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hernandez, M., Costa, A., Fuentes, L., Vivas, A., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2010). The impact of bilingualism on the executive control and orienting networks of attention. Bilingualism Language and Cognition, 13(3), 315–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hinkin, C., Castellon, S., Hardy, D., Granholm, E., & Siegle, G. (1999). Computerized and traditional stroop task dysfunction in HIV-1 infection. Neuropsychology, 13(2), 306–316. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.13.2.306.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoover-Dempsey, K., Walker, J., Jones, K., & Reed, R. (2002). Teachers involving parents (TIP): An in-service teacher education program for enhancing parental involvement. Teaching and Teacher Education, 18(7), 843–867.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huizinga, M., Dolan, C., & Molen, M. (2006). Age-related change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2017–2036.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ivanova, I., & Costa, A. (2008). Does bilingualism hamper lexical access in speech production? Acta Psychologica, 127, 277–288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jurado, M., & Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: A review of our current understanding. Neuropsychological Revue, 17, 213–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. (2007). Is speech learning ‘gated’ by the social brain? Developmental Science, 10, 110–120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lemmon, C., & Goggin, J. (1989). The measurement of bilingualism and its relationship to cognitive ability. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 133–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition, 114, 29–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Mileka, L. (1998). Miqyas Wikslar Lilatfal. Alqaherah: Alnuskah Alarabiyah. Maktabat Alanglo almasriyah.

  • **Mileka, L. (1998). Wechsler Scale for Children, Arabic Version. Cairo: Anglo EgyptianBookshop.

  • Millett, R. (2010). The cognitive effects of bilingualism: Does knowing two languages impact children’s ability to reason about mental states? Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota.

  • Mishra, R., Hilchey, M., Singh, N., & Klein, R. (2012). On the time course of exogenous cueing effects in bilinguals: Higher proficiency in a second language is associated with more rapid endogenous disengagement. The Quartely Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(8), 1502–1510.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moawad, R. (2006). The acquisition of the arabic gender and number systems. Unpublished thesis, University of Wales, Bangor.

  • Oller, D., & Pearson, B. (2002). Assessing the effects of bilingualism: A background. In D. K. Oller & R. E. Eilers (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 3–21). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peal, E., & Lambert, W. (1962). The relation of bilingualism to intelligence. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 76(546), 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perlstein, M., Carter, S., Barch, M., & Baird, W. (1998). The stroop task and attention deficits in schizophrenia: A critical evaluation of card and single-trial stroop methodologies. Neuropsychology, 12, 414–425.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M., & Rothbart, M. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Developement and Psychopathology, 12, 427–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poulin-Dubois, D., Blaye, A., Coutya, J., & Bialystok, E. (2011). The effects of bilingualism on toddlers’ executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 567–579.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reiter, A., Tucha, O., & Lang, K. (2004). Executive functions in children with dyslexia. Dyslexia, 11(2), 116–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Balaguer, R., & Münte, T. (2006). Executive control in bilingual language processing. Language Learning, 56, 133–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Santisi, M., Arecco, M., Salvatierra, J., Conde, A., et al. (2002). Stroop effect in Spanish–English bilinguals. Journal of th International Neuropsychological Socety, 8, 819–827.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sabbagh, M., Xu, F., Carlson, S., Moses, L., & Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind: A comparison of Chinese and U.S. preschoolers. Psychol Sci, 17(1), 74–81.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, G. (1988). Linking language with mathematics achievement: problems and prospects. In R. R. Cocking & J. P. Mestre (Eds.), Linguistic and cultural influences on learning mathematics (pp. 47–62). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Saleh, A. (1971). Extibar Thurston Lilqudurat Alawaliyah. Alqaherah: Dar Alnahdah Almasriyah.

  • **Saleh, Z. (1971). Thurstone’s primary mental abilities test. Dar Alnahda Almasriya: Cairo.

  • Wilbourn, M., Kurtz, L., & Kalia, V. (2011). The Lexical Stroop Sort (LSS) picture-word task: A computerized task for assessing the relationship between language and executive functioning in school-aged children. Behavioral Research Methods?, 44(1), 270–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xue, G., Dong, O., Jin, Z., & Chen, C. (2004). Mapping of verbal working memory in nonfluent Chinese–English bilinguals with functional MRI. NeuroImage, 22, 1–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ruba AbdelMatloub Moawad.

Additional information

This research project was supported by a Grant from the Research Center of University for Girls at Olaisha, Deanship of scientific Research, King Saud University.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Abdelgafar, G.M., Moawad, R.A. Executive Function Differences Between Bilingual Arabic–English and Monolingual Arabic Children. J Psycholinguist Res 44, 651–667 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-014-9309-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-014-9309-3

Keywords

Navigation