Skip to main content
Log in

Individual Differences in Social Comparison and its Consequences for Life Satisfaction: Introducing a Short Scale of the Iowa–Netherlands Comparison Orientation Measure

  • Published:
Social Indicators Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research in social psychology has shown individual variation in the tendency to compare one’s own opinions and abilities with those of other people, raising the question of whether social comparisons are psychological dispositions. To test the empirical validity of this proposition, Gibbons and Buunk (1999) created an instrument, the Iowa–Netherlands Comparison Orientation Measure (INCOM), that measures the tendency to engage in social comparison and captures central aspects of the self and the other. This paper analyzes the reliability and validity of the INCOM scale for the German population and discusses potentials for shortening the instrument for continued use in large-scale population surveys. Interdependencies between psychological and structural characteristics are investigated and consequences for life satisfaction discussed. The results show evidence that individuals systematically vary in their orientation towards social comparison.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The SOEP pretest modules are representative samples of the German population. The respondents are randomly chosen and do not conflate with respondents of the core sample of the annual SOEP household panel study (see SOEP 2012a, b).

  2. Gibbons and Buuk recommend a shortend version of the INCOM scale by reducing it to the six items of the ability component. We believe, however, that the two dimensional character of SCO needs to be preserved, since comparison with abilities and opinions constitute two independent but equally important components of SCO (see Festinger 1954).

  3. SOEP is an annual household panel that has been conducted in Germany since 1984 (Wagner et al. 2007; Siedler et al. 2011).

  4. Please note that item 5 follows a reversed coding structure. A mean value of 3.5 therefore indicates that individuals tend to agree that they are not the type of people who often compare themselves with others.

  5. Note that the first-order model can be easily replaced by a second-order model whenever the first-order dimensions prove to be strongly interrelated (indicated by high correlations between the latent constructs).

  6. Items 3 and 4 showed an equal proportion of shared variance. For the shortened version of the INCOM scale, we selected item 4 to further broaden the scope of the dimension by including a more social ability component.

  7. The χ2 difference test compares the χ2 results of three models: (1) the more restrictive model that limits the factor loadings and the intercepts to be equal for both subgroups (scalar invariance), (2) the less restrictive model that forces only the factor loadings to be equal for both subgroups (metric invariance) and (3) the least restrictive model that forces only the factor structure to be equal for both subgroups (configural invariance) (see Brown 2006). If the χ2 difference test of (1) and (2) is not significant, we observe scalar invariance; if the χ2 difference test of (2) and (3) is not significant we observe metric invariance.

  8. Low education refers to those who have basic education (Hauptschule) or left school without a school-leaving certificate; high education refers to those who left school with a school-leaving certificate (Abitur or Fachhochschulreife) that allows them to attend the university. Those with medium education (Realschule) were not included in the analysis.

  9. Our results deviate from the results presented by Gibbons and Buunk (1999). The authors report a stronger tendency towards SCO for women. This deviation can be explained by differences in (a) the kind of measurement in use and (b) the type of sample. Whereas Gibbons and Buunk (1999) used a total score of SCO, we tested subgroup differences on both dimensions (ability and opinion) separately, since we believe that it is this two-dimensional structure of SCO that interests further research in social psychology. Further, Gibbons and Buunk (1999) base their results on the construct validity on two students’ samples. In contrast, our sample represents the German population and displays results for a more heterogeneous group.

  10. We favor a more relaxed categorization of comparison orientation “subgroups” (above/below the factor mean of ability and opinion comparisons) against a more restrictive one (at least one SD above/below the factor mean) to maintain the sample size. The conservative categorization limits our sample, which varies between 170 and 231 cases for each subgroup. However, choosing a looser categorization offers more analyzable cases, but limits the scope of interpretation to conclusions on general tendencies.

  11. The following variables are used: sex (male = 0; female = 1); age (metric); household financial situation (perceived financial situation of the household; respondents were asked to rate their household on a scale of 0, poor, to 10, wealthy); relative financial situation compared to other households in the neighborhood (difference between perceived financial situation of the household and perceived financial situation of the households in the neighborhood; respondents were asked to rate typical households in their neighborhoods on a scale of 0, poor, to 10, wealthy).

  12. A shortened version of the Rosenberg self-esteem scale is used (Rosenberg 1979). Respondents reported on a seven-point scale whether they agreed with following items: (a) “All in all I am satisfied with myself”; (b) “I am afraid there is not much I am proud of.”

  13. Please note again that item 5 follows a reversed coding structure. A mean value of 4.30 therefore indicates that individuals tend to agree that they are not the type of people who often compare themselves with others.

References

  • Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchanges. In L. Berkowitz & E. Walster (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 267–299). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, M., & Crosby, F. (1980). An empirical examination of relative deprivation theory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16, 442–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biernat, M., & Billings, L. S. (2001). Standards, expectancies and social comparisons. In A. Tesser & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Intraindividual processes (pp. 257–283). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blau, G. (1994). Testing the effect of level and importance of pay referents on pay level satisfaction. Human Relations, 47, 1251–1268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burchell, B., & Yagil, D. (1997). Socioeconomic and political initiators of pay comparison. Work Employment Society, 11, 737–748.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buunk, B. P., Collins, R. L., Taylor, S. E., & VanYperen, N. W. (1990). The affective consequences of social comparison: Either direction has its ups and downs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(6), 1238–1249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buunk, A. P., & Gibbons, F. X. (2006). Social comparison orientation: A new perspective on those who do and those who don’t compare with others. In S. Guimond (Ed.), Social comparison and social psychology. Understanding cognition, intergroup relations and culture (pp. 15–32). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buunk, B. P., & Gibbons, F. X. (2007). Social comparison: The end of a theory and the emergence of a field. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102, 3–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buunk, B. P., Groothof, H. A. K., & Siero, F. W. (2007). Social comparison and satisfaction with one’s social life. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 24, 197–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buunk, B. P., & Mussweiler, T. (2001). New directions in social comparison research. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 467–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bygren, M. (2004). Pay reference standards and pay satisfaction: What do workers evaluate their pay against? Social Science Research, 33, 206–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, T., & Mallery, P. (1999/2000). Social comparison, individualism-collectivism, and self-esteem in China and the United States. Current Psychology: Development, Learning, Personality, Social, 18(4), 340–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., & Oswald, A. J. (1996). Satisfaction and comparison income. Journal of Public Economics, 61, 359–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., & Senik, C. (2010). Who compares to whom? The anatomy of income comparisons in Europe. The Economic Journal, 120, 573–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T., Jr, & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crosby, F. (1976). A model of egoistical relative deprivation. Psychological Review, 83(2), 85–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Ambrosio, C., & Frick, J. R. (2007). Income satisfaction and relative deprivation: An empirical link. Social Indicators Research, 81, 497–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, J. A. (1959). A formal interpretation of the theory of relative deprivation. Sociometry, 22(4), 280–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1997). Social comparisons and subjective well-being. In B. P. Buunk (Ed.), Health, coping, and well-being: Perspectives from social comparison theory (pp. 329–358). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A. (1995). Will raising the incomes of all increase the happiness of all? Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 27, 35–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbons, F. X., & Buunk, B. P. (1999). Individual differences in social comparison: Development of a scale of social comparison orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(1), 129–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goethals, G. R. (1986). Social comparison theory: Psychology from the lost and found. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 261–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, P. S. (1974). An examination of referents used in the evaluation of pay. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 12, 170–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gurr, T. (1970). Why men rebel. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagerty, M. R. (2000). Social comparisons of income in one’s community: Evidence from national surveys of income and happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(4), 764–771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Homans, G. C. (1961). Social behavior: Its elementary forms. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.

    Google Scholar 

  • Homans, G. C. (1976). Commentary. In L. Berkowitz & E. Walster (Eds.), Equity theory: Toward a general theory of social interaction. Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 231–259). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyman, H. H. (1975). Reference individuals and reference idols. In L. A. Coser (Ed.), The Idea of Social Structure. Papers in Honor of Robert K. Merton. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jasso, G. (1990). Methods for the theoretical and empirical analysis of comparison processes. Social Methodology, 20, 369–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jasso, G. (2006). The theory of comparison processes. In P. J. Burke (Ed.), Contemporary social psychology theories (pp. 165–193). Stanford: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Krueger, A., Schkade, D., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. (2006). Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing illusion. Science, 312, 1908–1910.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., & Mayseless, O. (1990). Classic and current social comparison research: Expanding the perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 108(2), 195–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, F. R., Dennis, J., Lüdtke, O., Schupp, J., & Wagner, G. G. (2011). Short assessment of the Big Five: Roboust across survey methods except telephone interviewing. Behavior Research Methods, 43(2), 548–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masters, J. C., & Keil, L. (1987). Generic comparison processes in human judgment and behavior. In J. C. Masters & W. Smith (Eds.), Social comparison, social justice and behavior: Theoretical, empirical, and policy perspectives (pp. 11–54). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride, M. (2001). Relative-income effects on subjective well-being in the cross-section. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 45, 251–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merton, R. K. (Ed.) (1968). Social theory and social structure. 1968 enlarged edition. New York: The Free Press.

  • Merton, R. K., & Rossi, A. S. (1968). Contributions to the theory of reference group behavior. In R. K. Merton (Ed.), Social theory and social structure. 1968 enlarged edition (pp. 279–334). New York: The Free Press.

  • Moore, D. (1990). Discrimination and deprivation: The effects of social comparisons. Social Justice Research, 4(1), 49–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Mplus Users Guide (6th ed.). Los Angeles: Muthen & Muthen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oskamp, S., & Schultz, P. W. (2004). Attitudes and opinions (3rd ed.). New York: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pettigrew, T. F. (1967). Social evaluation theory: Convergences and applications. In D. I. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 241–311). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the Self. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runciman, W. G. (1966). Relative deprivation and social justice. A study of attitudes and social inequality in the twentieth century Britain. Brookfield: Gregg Revivals.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, S. (2010). The selection of pay referents: Potential patterns and impacts on life satisfaction. SOEP Paper, 333, 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, S., & Schupp, J. (2010). Measuring the selection of pay referents. A methodological analysis of the questions on pay referents in the 2008 and 2009 pretest modules. DIW Data Documentation, 48, 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siedler, T., Schupp, J., & Wagner, G. G. (2011). Innovative methods within the context of secondary data analysis: Examples from household panel surveys. In K. H. Trzesniewski, M. B. Donnellan, & R. E. Lucas (Eds.), Secondary data analysis. An introduction to psychologists (pp. 103–118). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Siegrist, J., Starke, D., Chandola, T., Godin, I., Marmot, M., Niedhammer, I., et al. (2004). The measurement of Effort-Reward Imbalance at work: European comparisons. Social Science and Medicine, 58(8), 1483–1499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Socio-Economic Panel Study (SOEP). (2012a). SOEP 2010—Methodenbericht zur Testerhebung 2010 des Sozio-oekonomischen Panels—„Soziale Netzwerke, ökonomische Suchtheorie und weitere Innovationsmodule”. SOEP Survey Papers, 76, 1–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Socio-Economic Panel Study (SOEP). (2012b). SOEP Testerhebung 2011: „Gender Stereotypen, Finanzen und weitere Innovationsmodule”. SOEP Survey Papers, 110, 1–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stouffer, S., Suchman, E. A., DeVinney, L. C., Star, S. A., & Williams, R. M., Jr. (1949). The American Soldier: Adjustment during army life. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suls, J., Martin, R., & Wheeler, L. (2002). Social comparison: Why, with whom, and with what effect? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(5), 159–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, G. G., Frick, J. R., & Schupp, J. (2007). The German Socio-Economic Panel Study (SOEP)—Scope, evolution and enhancements. Schmollers Jahrbuch, 127, 139–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, L. (2000). Individual differences in social comparison. In J. Suls & L. Wheeler (Eds.), Handbook of social comparison. Theory and research (pp. 141–158). New York: Plenum Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Winkler, N., Kroh, M., & Spiess, M. (2006). Entwicklung einer deutschen Kurzskala zur zweidimensionalen Messung von sozialer Erwünschtheit. DIW-Discussion Paper, 579, 1–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, J. V. (1989). Theory and research concerning social comparisons of personal attributes. Psychological Bulletin, 106(2), 231–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Deborah Anne Bowen (DIW) and the anonymous reviewer for their kind support on this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Simone M. Schneider.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Schneider, S.M., Schupp, J. Individual Differences in Social Comparison and its Consequences for Life Satisfaction: Introducing a Short Scale of the Iowa–Netherlands Comparison Orientation Measure. Soc Indic Res 115, 767–789 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-012-0227-1

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-012-0227-1

Keywords

Navigation