Skip to main content
Log in

Beyond Objectivity and Subjectivity: The Intersubjective Foundations of Psychological Science

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The question of whether psychology can properly be regarded as a science has long been debated (Smedslund in Integrative Psychological & Behavioral Science, 50, 185–195, 2016). Science is typically understood as a method for producing reliable knowledge by testing falsifiable claims against objective evidence. Psychological phenomena, however, are traditionally taken to be “subjective” and hidden from view. To the extent that science relies upon objective observation, is a scientific psychology possible? In this paper, I argue that scientific psychology does not much fail to meet the requirements of objectivity as much as the concept of objectivity fails as a methodological principle for psychological science. The traditional notion of objectivity relies upon the distinction between a public, observable exterior and a private, subjective interior. There are good reasons, however, to reject this dichotomy. Scholarship suggests that psychological knowledge arises neither from the “inside out” (subjectively) nor from the outside-in (objectively), but instead intersubjective processes that occur between people. If this is so, then objectivist methodology may do more to obscure than illuminate our understanding of psychological functioning. From this view, we face a dilemma: Do we, in the name of science, cling to an objective epistemology that cuts us off from the richness of psychological activity? Or do we seek to develop a rigorous intersubjective psychology that exploits the processes through which we gain psychological knowledge in the first place? If such a psychology can produce systematic, reliable and useful knowledge, then the question of whether its practices are “scientific” in the traditional sense would become irrelevant.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Daston, L., & Galison, P. (2007). Objectivity. Brooklyn: Zone Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A., Garvey, A., Hsu, H-C, West-Stroming, D. (2006). Change processes in relationships: A relational-historical research approach. St. Paul, MN: West Publishing Co.

  • Hacker, P. M. S. (1997). Wittgenstein on human nature. London: Pheonix.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M. (2009). The problem of other minds is not a problem: Mirror neurons and intersubjectivity. In J. A. Pineda & J. A. Pineda (Eds.), Mirror neuron systems: The role of mirroring processes in social cognition (pp. 121–133). Totowa: Humana Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M. (2011). Mirroring as a key neural mechanism of sociality. In G. R. Semin, G. Echterhoff, G. R. Semin, & G. Echterhoff (Eds.), Grounding sociality: Neurons, mind, and culture (pp. 13–25). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J., & Sugarman, J. (2009). Does interpretation in psychology differ from interpretation in natural science? Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 39, 19–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J., Sokol, B. W., & Elfers, T. (2008). Taking and coordinating perspectives: from prereflective interactivity, through reflective intersubjectivity, to metareflective sociality. Human Development, 51, 294–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matusov, E. (1996). Intersubjectivity without agreement. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 3, 25–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. J. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509–516.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. (2013). Origins of social cognition: Bidirectional self-other mapping and the ‘like-me’ hypothesis. In M. R. Banaji & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us (pp. 139–144). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Molenaar, P. C. M., & Ram, N. (2010). Dynamic modeling and optimal control of intraindividual variation: A computational paradigm for nonergodic psychological processes. In S. M. Chow, E. Ferrer, & F. Hsieh (Eds.), Statistical methods for modeling human dynamics: An interdisciplinary dialogue. Routledge/Taylor & Francis: New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E. (1979). Transcriptions as theory. In E. Ochs & B. Schieffelin (Eds.), Developmental pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overgaard, S. (2005). Rethinking other minds: Wittgenstein and Levinas on expression. Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy, 48(3), 249–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (2015). Joining intentions in infancy. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 22, 24–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, L. T., Rouhani, P., & Fischer, K. W. (2013). The science of the individual. Mind, Brain, and Education, 7, 152–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, P. J., & Valsiner, J. (2011). The un-making of a method: from rating scales to the study of psychological processes. Theory & Psychology, 21, 47–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schore, J. R., & Schore, A. N. (2008). Modern attachment theory: the central role of affect regulation in development and treatment. Clinical Social Work Journal, 36, 9–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shotter, J. (2000). Wittgenstein and the everyday: from radical hiddenness to “Nothing Is Hidden”; from representation to participation. Journal of Mundane Behavior, 1, 116–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (1987). The perils of averaging data over strategies: an example from children’s addition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116, 250–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (2005). Children’s learning. American Psychologist, 60, 769–778.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R., & Jenkins, E. (1989). How children discover new strategies. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slaney, K. L., & Garcia, D. A. (2015). Constructing psychological objects: the rhetoric of constructs. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 35, 244–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. (2016). Why psychology cannot be an empirical science. Integrative Psychological & Behavioral Science, 50, 185–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sodian, B. (2011). Theory of mind in infancy. Child Development Perspectives, 5, 39–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ter Hark, M. (1990). Beyond the inner and the outer: Wittgenstein’s philosophy of psychology. London: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 3–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Velicer, W. F., Babbin, S. F., & Palumbo, R. (2014). Idiographic applications: Issues of ergodicity and generalizability. In P. M. Molenaar, R. M. Lerner, K. M. Newell, P. M. Molenaar, R. M. Lerner, & K. M. Newell (Eds.), Handbook of developmental systems theory and methodology (pp. 425–441). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. (Anscombe, G.E.M., trans.). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

  • Yanchar, S. C., & Slife, B. D. (2000). Putting it all together: toward a hermeneutic unity of psychology. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 21, 315–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, D. (2005). Subjectivity and selfhood. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zlatev, J., Racine, T. P., Sinha, C., & Itkonen, E. (2008). The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael F. Mascolo.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mascolo, M.F. Beyond Objectivity and Subjectivity: The Intersubjective Foundations of Psychological Science. Integr. psych. behav. 50, 543–554 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-016-9357-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-016-9357-3

Keywords

Navigation