1 Introduction

Aging, which results in progressive decline in several functions after adulthood, has a pervasive and complex influence on physiology, cognition, and physical activity, and it has become a global issue in modern society. Age-related decline in these functions not only reduces the quality of life and independence of elderly people, but also impacts the family, society, and national economy. Among the age-related reductions of functionality, the decline in cognitive function among elderly people has received considerable attention. Cognitive function refers to brain functionality related to the formation of knowledge, attention, memory, judgment, language ability, computing power, space–time orientation ability, and reasoning ability [1]. The age-related cognitive decline is noticeable for the elderly and sometimes causes frustration, but it is different from dementia where the brain is damaged by diseases (such as cerebrovascular diseases or Alzheimer’s disease) and with symptoms severe enough to interfere normal daily life.

Cognitive training involving the repetitive practice of cognitive tasks in a static condition is a approach to improve the cognition of elderly people [2]. Because of the association between age-related cognitive decline and cardiovascular diseases (hypertension, diabetes, and obesity), aerobic exercise that improves cardiovascular fitness and metabolic function has been proposed as an intervention to alleviate or even reverse age-related cognitive decline [3]. Nevertheless, interventional studies have provided inconsistent findings regarding the effects of aerobic exercise on the cognitive function of elderly people. Some studies have found that exercise improves the cognitive function of elderly people [4, 5], whereas others have found no differences in cognitive function between control and exercise intervention groups [6]. Meta-analyses have demonstrated that although aerobic exercise increases the cognitive function of healthy senior adults, the increase is not significant when compared with that in the control group [7, 8]. Training that combines physical and cognitive components provides greater benefit for cognition [9]. Exercise type, intensity, frequency, and duration all potentially influence training effects.

Neuroimaging studies have proposed that cognitive training and exercise improve the cognitive functions through neuroplasticity not only in patients with mild cognitive impairment but also in healthy sensor adults [10, 11]. Moderate-intensity aerobic exercise modulates cognitively and clinically relevant functional brain networks in older adults, especially for increasing the integration of attention and executive control network (ECN) and the functional connectivity of default mode network (DMN) [12], and decreasing the brain atrophy in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and medial temporal lobe [13]. Not only functional connectivity but also structural connectivity derived from diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) showed that physical exercise significantly increases the integrity of motor functioning related white matter fiber tracts [14]. Nevertheless, it is still unclear the effects of exercise with different cognitive load on neuroplasticity and cognitive function. The aim of this study was to explore the effects of exercise with either high cognitive load or low cognitive load on cognitive performance and neuroplasticity in healthy elderly. According to the published findings, we targeted the exercise-related neuroplasticity on the four specific networks: DMN, ECN, sensorimotor (SMN) and salience network (SN) encompassing ACC.

2 Methods

2.1 Participants

Twenty-eight sedentary community-dwelling senior adults (age ≥ 65 years old) without known cognitive impairment were recruited. Volunteers were included if they were sedentary (exercised less than 90 min per week in the past year) and could walk at least 5 m. Volunteers were excluded if they had diagnosed neurological and psychiatric disorders, had score of Mini Mental State Examination lower than 26, had heart failure or met the exclusion criteria specific to magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanning including claustrophobia and having metallic foreign body in the body. Participants were randomly assigned to either the high-cognitive load exercise group (HE, n = 10) or the low-cognitive load exercise group (LE, n = 9). For the allocation, lots were drawn by a person who did not involve in this study. Participants who registered after the start of the exercise program were allocated to the control group (C, n = 9). All participants were assessed at baseline and after 4 months of intervention. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Chang Gung Medical Foundation and all methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations. Written informed consents were obtained from all the participants of this study. Information that could lead to identification of a participant (e.g. clinical images and videos) was not collected in this study.

2.2 Exercise Pogram

Participants in the HE and LE groups engaged in moderate-intensity aerobic exercise training for 4 months; exercise frequency was three times a week, and each session lasted for 50 min. The exercise mode of the LE group was walking on a treadmill supervised by a physical therapist. Walking was chosen because it is a widely accepted exercise type among elderly people. The exercise mode of the HE group was aerobic dance led by an aerobic dance instructor. The dance program included common aerobic movements, such as V step, marching, kicks, and lunges in different directions. Participants followed the movement of the instructor, learnt new dance movements, memorized the order of different movements, and switched between movements throughout the exercise. In both groups, each 50-min exercise session comprised 10 min of warm-up, 30 min of moderate-intensity exercise (55% to 69% of the age-predicted maximal heart rate), and 10 min of cool-down. To monitor equal exercise intensity between LE and HE groups, all participants wore a heart rate monitor (Polar RS400; Polar Electro Inc.) during exercise. The average heart rate during main exercise was similar between the LE and HE groups (Supplementary Table).

2.3 Measurement

2.3.1 Cognitive Performance

Cognitive performance of participants was assessed using the Chinese version of Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument 2.0 (CASI 2.0). CASI is a 100-point scale that assesses nine domains of cognitive function, including attention, orientation, short-term memory, long-term memory, language, drawing, abstract thinking and judgment, mental manipulation, and animal name fluency. Higher scores indicate higher cognitive function [1, 15]. The total score and the domain scores of CASI was used to evaluate the cognitive ability of individuals. The total score reflects the overall cognitive ability of individuals and the nine domain scores reflect the ability of designated individual cognitive abilities. The validity of the overall score and domain scores of CASI has been confirmed previously [16, 17]. We also recorded the time taken to finish the cognitive test because time it takes to respond to questions reflects the attention, processing speed and executive function of individuals [18].

2.3.2 MRI Acquisition

MRI data were acquired on a 3 T scanner (Prisma, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with a 20-channel head coil at National Taiwan University. High-resolution longitudinal relaxation time weighted imaging (T1-weighted anatomical images) were collected using a magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo (MP-RAGE) sequence: inversion time (TI) = 900 ms, repetition time (TR) = 2000 ms, echo time (TE) = 2.28 ms, flip angle (FA) = 8°, field-of-view (FOV) = 256 × 256 mm2, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm3, slice number = 192, and acquisition time = 277 s. Resting-state functional MRI (RS-fMRI) data were acquired using a gradient-echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence with whole-brain coverage: FOV = 192 × 192 mm2, slice number = 32, voxel size = 3 × 3 × 4 mm3, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 32 ms, FA = 84° and total scan time = 286 s. In RS-fMRI, participants were instructed to immobilize head position, to visually fixate on a cross, not to fall asleep, and not to think of anything in particular. Diffusion tensor images (DTI) were acquired using a dual-spin-echo EPI sequence: slice number = 55 with thickness = 2.5 mm without gap, FOV = 240 × 240 mm2, voxel size = 2.5 mm isotropic, TR/TE = 7000/87 ms, and b-value = 1000 s/mm2 with 30 noncollinear diffusion gradient directions and 10 null images (b = 0). Total scan time for DTI acquisition was 280 s.

2.3.3 MRI Preprocessing

In network analyses, we parcellated both fMRI and DTI data into 90 brain regions using Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) [19]. First, for functional network construction, the RS-fMRI data were preprocessed using the default preprocessing pipeline of CONN functional connectivity toolbox (ver.15 h) [20]. After preprocessing, the average fMRI signal of the white matter and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) was used for nuissance regression and underwent the band pass filter (0.01–0.08 Hz). Finally, the correlation between the time courses each pair of brain parcellations was computed for each volunteer, generating the weighted connectivity matrix. Second, for structural network construction, the DTI data were processed using pipeline for analyzing brain diffusion images (PANDA) (ver.1.3.1) [21]. Based on matrix laboratory (MATLAB) and FSL (Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Brain Software Library). For fiber tracking, fiber assignment was performed using a fiber assignment by continuous tracking (FACT) algorithm, with an FA threshold of 0.2 and an angle threshold of 45. For network construction, the 90 AAL regions were taken as network nodes. Finally, two weighted and undirected connectivity matrices of fiber number (FN) and FA values were generated for each participant.

2.3.4 Functional and Structural Connectivity

The network measures were computed using Brain Connectivity Toolbox (BCT) [22]. In BCT, we estimated the network measures using the weighted matrices over a range of thresholds (FN in structural connectivity: 0–10, 1 increments; matrix density in functional connectivity: 0.37–0.5, 0.001 increments) [23,24,25]. After thresholding, the weighted FN matrix became a binary matrix. New weighted matrix was then generated by multiplying the FA-based connectivity matrix by the binary matrix. The generated binary and weighted matrices (for both structural and functional connectivity) were calculated for further network analyses. For these analyses, three types of local node measures were selected: (1) degree: the number of node connections; (2) betweenness: the fraction of all shortest paths in the network that contains a given node; (3) local efficiency: the global efficiency computed from the reciprocal of path lengths to the neighbors of a given node [22].

2.4 Statistical Analysis

All data are presented as mean ± SEM or number (percentage). SPSS version 24 (SPSS, Chicago, Illinois, USA) and GraphPad Prism 5.0 (GraphPad software Inc., La Jolla, CA, USA) were used to analyze data and draw the plots. Baseline differences among the three groups were tested using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and chi-squared tests. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with post-test values as the dependent variable, and baseline, age and the education year (note: age and education are two predictors of the CASI score [15]) as covariates was used to determine the effects of intervention and the Fisher's Least Significant Difference was used as the post-hoc test. Pearson’s correlation coefficient was used to examine the association between outcome variables. P < 0.05 was considered significant for all variables except outcomes of brain plasticity where the significance level was set at 0.013 for Bonferroni correction (two-tailed) for multiple comparisons regarding four targeting brain networks were explored.

3 Results

3.1 Baseline Characteristics

The study population was characterized by a mean age of 69.2 ± 0.8 years (range 63 to 79 years), years of education of 10.5 ± 0.8, BMI of 25.0 ± 0.5 kg/m2, and waist circumference of 88.6 ± 1.6 cm for men and of 85.3 ± 1.3 cm for women. Twenty-one percent of participants had hypertension, and 7% had diabetes. Regarding cognitive performance, the study population had a total CASI score of 91.0 ± 0.8, long-term memory domain score of 9.96 ± 0.04, short-term memory domain score of 10.57 ± 0.21, attention domain score of 7.64 ± 0.21, mental manipulation domain score of 7.93 ± 0.30, orientation domain score of 17.61 ± 0.15, abstract thinking and judgment domain score of 10.11 ± 0.28, language domain score of 9.25 ± 0.18, drawing domain score of 10.00 ± 0.00, and animal name fluency domain score of 7.93 ± 0.34. The baseline characteristics of participants in the C, LE, and HE groups were all similar, except for years of education. Participants in the LE and HE groups had more years of education than those in the C group, but no significant difference was observed between the LE and HE groups (Table 1).

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of participants

3.2 Exercise Compliance

Two participants in the HE group and one participant in the LE group withdrew from the study before the post-test. Specifically, one participant in the HE group withdrew in the first month of training due to kidney stone surgery, and the other participant withdrew due to frequent colds. Contact with one participant in the LE group was lost after baseline assessment. The average exercise attendance rate of participants who completed both pre- and post-tests was 90.0% for the HE group and 84.1% for the LE group. No significant difference was observed in the attendance rate of exercise training between the HE and LE groups (P = 0.21).

3.3 Effects of Exercise Intervention

3.3.1 Cognitive Performance

The total CASI score of HE group at post-test was significantly higher than that of the C group after adjusting the baseline values, age and the education (P < 0.05). There were no significant differences in the total CASI score at post-test between C and LE groups and between LE and HE groups. The HE group took shorter time to finish the cognitive battery at post-test than the C and LE groups (Table 2).

Table 2 CASI scores of each group before and after exercise intervention

3.3.2 Functional and Structural Connectivity

Network analysis of functional connectivity (based on RS-fMRI data) showed group-dependent changes from pretest to post-test (Fig. 1a). Specifically, HE group had increased degrees in the left superior frontal gyrus (SFG, AAL region 3) from ECN and left anterior cingulate cortex (ACC, AAL region 31) from SN, and had decreased degrees in the left middle occipital gyrus (MOG, AAL region 51) from the visual network, and left postcentral gyrus (PCG, AAL region 57) from SMN after the 4-month intervention (Fig. 1b–e). Moreover, the local efficiency of the left angular gyrus (AG, AAL region 65) from DMN was significantly increased in the C group, but not in the LE and HE groups. The local efficiency of left inferior temporal gyrus (ITG, AAL region 89), sub-region of DMN, significantly decreased in the HE group, but not in the C and LE groups (Fig. 1f–g). Network analysis of structural connectivity demonstrated an alternative scenario (Fig. 2a). Specifically, the betweenness measure of the left middle temporal gyrus (MTG, AAL region 85) from DMN derived from binary connectivity matrices alone and the left ITG (AAL region 89) derived from weighted matrices showed significant increase in the C group after the 4 months (Fig. 2b–c).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Brain regions showing group-dependent neuroplasticity in functional connectivity. a The summary figure of brain regions showing group-dependent neuroplasticity in functional connectivity (red nodes indicate degree, and yellow nodes indicate local efficiency). bg Network measures of brain regions showing group-dependent neuroplasticity in functional connectivity. Each line links the pre- and post-test data of each individual. ACC anterior cingulate cortex; AG angular gyrus; C control group; HE high-cognitive load exercise group; ITG inferior temporal gyrus; LE low-cognitive load exercise group; MOG middle occipital gyrus; SFG superior frontal gyrus; PCG postcentral gyrus; b binary matrix (n = 6 in the C group; n = 7 in the LE group; n = 7 in the HE group)

Fig. 2
figure 2

Brain regions showing group-dependent neuroplasticity in structural connectivity. a The summary figure of brain regions showing group-dependent neuroplasticity in structural connectivity (blue nodes indicate betweenness). bc Network measures of brain regions showing group-dependent neuroplasticity in structural connectivity. Each line links the pre- and post-test data of each individual. C control group; HE high cognitive load exercise group; ITG inferior temporal gyrus; LE low cognitive load exercise group; MTG middle temporal gyrus; b binary matrix; w weighted matrix (n = 6 in the C group; n = 7 in the LE group; n = 7 in the HE group)

3.3.3 Relationship Between Changes in Cognitive Performance and Brain Plasticity

The changes in the time to complete the cognitive performance test were negatively correlated with the changes in ACC degree in functional connectivity and were positively correlated with the changes in MOG degree in functional connectivity and the changes in MTG betweenness in structural connectivity. The changes in the total CASI score were negatively correlated with the changes in AG local efficiency in functional connectivity and the changes in MTG betweenness in structural connectivity. Changes in the abstract thinking and judgment domain score were negatively correlated with the changes in PCG local efficiency in functional connectivity. Changes in the animal name fluency domain score were negatively correlated with the changes in ITG local efficiency in functional connectivity (Table 3).

Table 3 The relationship between changes of cognitive performances and brain plasticity

4 Discussion

The main finding of this study was that 4 months of exercise with high-cognitive load, but not exercise with low-cognitive load, improved the overall cognitive function of healthy elderly. MRI indices provided neuroscientific evidence for differential training effects between exercise with high and low cognitive loads. Exercise-related changes in functional connectivity were only prominent in the HE group. To the network specificity, the degrees of ECN and SN changed in the HE group after intervention. In addition, exercise prevented time-related changes in structural betweenness in the C group.

The novel finding of this study is that at the same exercise intensity, duration, and frequency, exercise involving high cognitive loads (dancing) had a more positive influence on cognitive function than exercise involving low cognitive loads (treadmill walking). Dancing is a cognitively taxing activity because this sensorimotor activity involves acoustic stimulation and various cognitive function such as attention, language, memory, visuo-spatial ability and visuomotor integration [26, 27]. Greater improvement in the executive function of healthy elderly people has been shown after one session of aerobic dance requiring higher cognitive engagement than after one session of aerobic dance requiring lower cognitive engagement [28]. Anderson-Hanley et al. found greater improvements in the executive function of elderly people with cyber cycling, where participants received more cognitive stimuli, than elderly people who rode traditional stationary bikes [29]. Our findings together with those of previous studies suggest that to improve the cognitive function through exercise, older individuals should choose an exercise type with at least moderate intensity and with high cognitive loads.

By quantitative MRI technologies, we found the beneficial effects of high-cognitive load exercise associated with subregions of ECN, DMN, SN and SMN in well agreement with previous findings. However, because these regions may involve cross-network functionality following the exercise intervention, the functional roles of each subregion are discussed below. We found that high-cognitive load exercise increased resting-state functional connectivity in SFG and ACC and decreased functional connectivity in sensory-associated brain regions, MOG and PCG. The SFG and ACC are associated with focusing attention and working memory and are highly susceptible to age-related alterations in cognitive engagement and functional connectivity [30, 31]. The role of SFG in working memory was evidenced by a lesion study, in which patients with brain lesions restricted to the left SFG had significant deficits in working memory compared with healthy controls and patients with prefrontal lesions but intact SFG, particularly when the complexity of the tasks increased [32]. The ACC is responsible for the selection of salient stimuli and the reactivity of individuals to affective inputs [33]. Greater ACC activity predicts higher focusing attention, particularly during distracting events [34]. MOG and PCG are perception-related brain regions for vision and limb sensation, respectively. The decreased functional connectivity of MOG and PCG in the HE group implied that training involving high cognitive engagement decreased the interference originating from visual and physical perceptions. Taken together, the findings indicated that the increased functional connectivity of SFG and ACC and the decreased functional connectivity of MOG and PCG in the HE group potentially promoted the ability to focus in elderly participants.

Our findings for the exercise-induced brain plasticity of SFG and ACC are similar to those of previous studies. Colcombe et al. showed that 6 months of moderate-to-high intensity exercise training increased the brain volume of the ACC, supplementary motor area, and middle frontal gyrus [35]. Voss et al. showed that a 1-year walking intervention program increased the functional connectivity of the frontal executive network in elderly people. However, the exercise-induced brain plasticity was not significant within 6 months of intervention [36]. In the current study, 4 months of moderate-intensity exercise involving high cognitive loads resulted in favorable changes in functional connectivity among elderly participants. Therefore, the characteristics of exercise affect the length of training required to observe significant brain plasticity. Exercise involving high cognitive load may result in rapid neuroplasticity effects, leading to subsequent cognitive improvements in senior adults.

We found that exercise, particularly high-cognitive load exercise protected the brain from the age-related increase in local efficiency in AG and ITG. In addition, the changes in AG local efficiency were negatively correlated with the changes in the total CASI score. Both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have shown that aging leads to increases in brain local efficiency [37, 38]. The beneficial effects of exercise on local efficiency have been reported previously; that is, the functional connectivity of AG did not change in the control group but decrease in elderly people who engaged 6 months of multimodal activities, including cognitive, aerobic, and recreational activities [39].

Regarding structural connectivity, the betweenness of MTG and ITG increased in the control group, but not in the high-cognitive load exercise group. Changes in the betweenness of MTG were negatively associated with the changes in cognitive performance. The betweenness of a brain area is the fraction of the number of shortest paths connecting neighboring brain areas that pass through the specific brain area. Greater betweenness of a brain area suggests that certain brain areas participate in a greater number of shortest paths. The betweenness of a brain region increases when the brain region has more linkage with other brain regions or when its neighboring brain region is lost [40]. Regarding the inverse correlation between the exercise-related changes in betweenness and cognition of our elderly population, our finding of the increased betweenness of MTG and ITG in the control group was likely due to the loss of gray matter volume. This finding was supported by additional analytic results that showed gray matter volumes of MTG and ITG were decreased in the control group, but no significant changes in the gray matter volume were found in the LE and HE groups (data not shown). The gray matter volume of the ITG has been reported to decrease with aging [41]. In brief, our findings suggest that high-cognitive load exercise improves the cognitive function of elderly people by preventing the volumetric loss of the temporal gyrus and its corresponding maintenance in betweenness.

This study had some limitations. First, this study had small sample size with 9 ~ 10 subjects in each group. Further studies with larger sample sizes are needed. Second, this study targeted on specific networks and their subregions associated with exercise, which involves multiple comparison issue in statistics. We took the network number into account for the multiple comparison for disclosing the plausible network changes. Given sufficient sample size, future studies are warranted to validate our findings. Third, while subjects in two exercise groups were allocated randomly, the control group was not allocated in a randomized manner. Although the recruitment strategies of the control group were not different from the other two groups, the education year was fewer in the control group than the other two groups. The difference in the education year among groups was likely due to the small sample size of the exploratory study. Thus, our finding could be influenced by the difference in the education between the control group and exercise groups even though education has been adjusted in ANCOVA as a covariate in this study. Last, this study did not have a group with only cognitive training, thus limiting the interpretation of the results.

In conclusion, this study might suggest that exercise with greater cognitive load likely results in greater training effects on cognition and brain connectivity than exercise requiring lower cognitive loads for healthy elderly.