Skip to main content
Log in

Specificity of Information Processing Styles to Depressive Symptoms in Youth Psychiatric Inpatients

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although information processing has been widely studied with depressed adults, little emphasis has been placed on the specificity of resultant findings to depression, as opposed to other psychological disorders. Analogously, even less effort has been directed toward examining the information processing styles of depressed children and adolescents. The present study investigated the specificity of information processing styles to depression and anxiety among 58 youth psychiatric inpatients. To assess information processing, we used a self-referent encoding task, in which participants were presented with positive and negative adjectives; participants were asked whether these adjectives described them or not, and were then tested on recall of the adjectives. After controlling for age and gender, lower rates of positive adjective endorsement and lower rates of positive adjective recall were found to be associated with depression, but not anxiety. Additionally, negative adjective endorsement was associated with anxiety symptoms. These results suggest specific cognitive features of depressive symptoms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Review, 96, 358–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ammons, R. B., & Ammons, C. H. (1962). The Quick Test (QT): Provisional manual. Psychological Reports, 11, 111–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1992). How might emotions affect learning? In S. A. Christianson (Ed.), Handbook of emotion and memory (pp. 3–31). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brady, E. U., & Kendall, P. C. (1992). Comorbidity of anxiety and depression in children and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 244–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B., Davies, P., & Richman, B. (1971). Word frequency book. New York: American Heritage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chorpita, B. F., Daleiden, E. L., Moffitt, C., Yim, L., & Umemoto, L. A. (2000). Assessment of tripartite factors of emotion in children and adolescents I: Structural validity and normative data of an affect and arousal scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 22, 141–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T., & Stewart, B. (1990). Cognitive specificity and positive-negative affectivity: Complementary or contradictory views on anxiety and depression? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 148–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: Psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 316–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 104, 268–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daleiden, E. L., & Vasey, M. W. (1997). An information-processing perspective on childhood anxiety. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 407–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derry, P. A., & Kuiper, N. A. (1981). Schematic processing and self-reference in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 286–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foa, E. B., & Foa, U. G. (1982). Differentiating depression and anxiety: Is it useful? Is it possible? Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 18, 62–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garber, J., & Robinson, N. S. (1997). Cognitive vulnerability in children at risk for depression. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 619–635.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H. (1981). Self-reinforcement and recall: Differential deficits in depressed and nondepressed psychiatric patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 521–540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammen, C., & Zupan, B. A. (1984). Self-schemas, depression, and the processing of personal information in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, 598–608.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammen, C. L., & Krantz, S. (1976). Effect of success and failure in depressive cognitions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85, 577–586.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E. (1984). Toward an information-processing analysis of depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8, 443–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E., Kendall, P. C., Smith, T. W., Donnell, C., & Ronan, K. (1987). Cognitive specificity in emotional distress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 734–742.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaenicke, C., Hammen, C., Zupan, B., Hiroto, D., Gordon, D., Adrian, C., & Burge, D. (1987). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15, 559–572.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joiner, T. E., Jr. (2000). A test of the hopelessness theory of depression among youth psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29, 167–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joiner, T. E., Jr., Catanzaro, S. J., & Laurent, J. (1996). Tripartite structure of positive and negative affect, depression, and anxiety in child and adolescent psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 401–409.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joiner, T. E., Jr., Katz, J., & Lew, A. (1997). Self verification and depression in youth psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 608–618.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joiner, T. E., Jr., & Wagner, K. D. (1995). Attributional style and depression in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 777–798.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jolly, J. B., & Dykman, R. A. (1994). Using self-report data to differentiate anxious and depressive symptoms in adolescents: Cognitive content specificity and global distress. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 18, 25–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, N. J., Ollendick, T., & Gullone, E. (1991). Negative affectivity in children and adolescents: Relations between anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 441–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs, M. (1980/1981). Rating scales to assess depression in school-aged children. Acta Paedopsychiatrica, 46, 305–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs, M. (1992). Children's Depression Inventory manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuiper, N., & Derry, P. (1982). Depressed and nondepressed content in self-reference in mild depressives. Journal of Personality, 50, 67–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuiper, N. A., & MacDonald, M. R. (1983). Schematic processing in depression: The self-based consensus bias. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 7, 469–484.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laurent, J., & Stark, K. D. (1993). Testing the cognitive content-specificity hypothesis with anxious and depressed youngsters. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 226–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, M. R., & Kuiper, N. A. (1984). Self-schema decision consistency in clinical depressives. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 2, 264–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDowall, J. (1984). Recall of pleasant and unpleasant words in depressed subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 401–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S., Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1998). Comorbidity of anxiety and unipolar mood disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 377–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nathan, P. E., & Langenbucher, J. W. (1999). Psychopathology: Description and classification. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 79–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, R. E., & Craighead, W. F. (1977). Selective recall of positive and negative feedback, self-control behaviors, and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86, 379–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paget, K. D., & Reynolds, C. R. (1984). Dimensions, levels, and reliabilities of the Revised Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale with learning disabled children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17, 137–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pela, O. A., & Reynolds, C. R. (1982). Cross-cultural application of the Revised Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale: Normative and reliability data for Nigerian primary school children. Psychological Reports, 51, 1135–1138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rehm, L. P. (1977). A self-control model of depression. Behavior Therapy, 8, 787–804.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, C. R. (1982). Convergent and divergent validity of the Revised Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 42, 1205–1212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, C. R., & Richmond, B. O. (1985). Revised Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale (RCMAS) manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, D., & Rehm, L. P. (1980). Relationship among self-monitoring processes, memory, and depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4, 149–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, Z. V. (1988). Appraisal of the self-schema construct in cognitive models of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 147–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, L., & Ingram, R. E. (1999). Cognitive reactivity and depressotypic information processing in children of depressed mothers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 202–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 247–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wener, A. E., & Rehm, L. P. (1975). Depressive affect: A test for behavioral hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84, 221–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zupan, B. A., Hammen, C., & Jaenicke, C. (1987). The effects of current mood and prior depressive history on self-schematic processing in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 43, 149–158.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gençöz, T., Voelz, Z.R., Gençöz, F. et al. Specificity of Information Processing Styles to Depressive Symptoms in Youth Psychiatric Inpatients. J Abnorm Child Psychol 29, 255–262 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010385832566

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010385832566

Navigation