INTRODUCTION

Recently, several reviews have noted the critical need for examining the potential benefit of combination therapies, involving either multiple medications or the combination of medication with specific psychotherapies in the treatment of alcoholism (Kranzler, 2000). Two medications, naltrexone and acamprosate (calcium-acetyl homotaurinate), would appear to be ideal candidates for investigating the potential benefit of combined treatments, given that these pharmacotherapies have been used extensively in the treatment of alcoholism (see Garbutt et al, 1996; Litten et al, 1996; Mason and Ownby, 2000 for reviews of the clinical literature). An investigation of the combination of these two compounds is particularly intriguing given that these drugs have distinctly different neurochemical actions and, therefore, the combination may be more efficacious in treating alcoholism than either drug alone.

It is clear that endogenous opioid peptides play a key role in the reward pathways of ethanol as well as other drugs of abuse (Herz, 1997; Koob et al, 1998). Acute ethanol consumption or ethanol treatment in rodents has been reported to increase endorphin and enkephalin gene expression and increase opioid peptide release in the brain and pituitary (Wand, 1989; Angelogianni and Gianoulakis, 1993; Gianoulakis, 1996). Neurochemically, the opioid receptor antagonists naloxone and naltrexone are known to bind to all three opioid receptors (mu, delta, and kappa) as a function of the dose administered (Chang et al, 1979; Corbett et al, 1993). Animal studies have shown that the administration of naloxone and naltrexone decreases ethanol self-administration (see Ulm et al, 1995 for a review). This antagonist-induced decrease in ethanol intake has been observed in a wide variety of settings including home-cage drinking (Marfaing-Jallat et al, 1983; Reid and Hunter, 1984; Hubbell et al, 1986; Sandi et al, 1988; Froehlich et al, 1990) and operant paradigms (Samson and Doyle, 1985; Weiss et al, 1990; Hyytia and Sinclair, 1993; June et al, 1999).

Neurochemically, acamprosate is an analogue of amino acids found in brain, such as taurine and homocysteic acid (Dahchour and De Witte, 2000). Naassila et al (1998) and al Qatari et al (1998) have demonstrated that acamprosate binds to a specific spermidine-sensitive site that modulates the NMDA receptor in a complex way. Their work suggests that acamprosate acts as a ‘partial coagonist’ at the NMDA receptor, such that low concentrations enhance activation when receptor activity is low, and high concentrations inhibit activation when receptor activity is high. This may be particularly relevant to the success of acamprosate as a pharmacotherapy given that chronic exposure to ethanol results in an upregulation of NMDA receptors (Grant, 1990; Liljequist, 1991; Hoffman and Tabakoff, 1994) and an upregulation in the density of voltage-dependent calcium channels (Messing et al, 1986; Dolin et al, 1987; Dolin and Little, 1989; Whittington and Little, 1991). Acamprosate also promotes the release of taurine in the brain (Dahchour and de Witte, 2000). Taurine is a major inhibitory neuromodulator/neurotransmitter and an increase in taurine availability would also contribute to a decrease in hyperexcitability. Thus, each of these changes produced by acamprosate may contribute to the decreased hyperexcitability observed during ethanol withdrawal and abstinence (Dahchour et al, 1998). Therefore, it has been hypothesized that acamprosate may minimize or negate some of the physiological changes produced by chronic ethanol exposure (Popp and Lovinger, 2000).

Acamprosate has been shown to reduce ethanol consumption in rodents that have an extended history of ethanol exposure or are ethanol-dependent (Boismare et al, 1984; Le Magnen et al, 1987; Gewiss et al, 1991; Spanagel et al, 1996a; Hölter et al, 1997). It has also been shown to reduce the increased ethanol consumption associated with abstinence from ethanol (the alcohol deprivation effect) in rats (Hölter et al, 1997; Spanagel et al, 1996a; Heyser et al, 1998). In contrast, acamprosate appears to have less of an effect on baseline alcohol consumption in alcohol naïve and nondependent rats (Le Magnen et al, 1991; Heyser et al, 1998, Stromberg et al, 2001). Acamprosate also has been reported to attenuate some of the behavioral and neurochemical events associated with ethanol withdrawal (Dahchour and de Witte, 1999; Putzke et al, 1996; Cole et al, 2000). For example, acamprosate reduces the hyperactivity and elevated glutamate levels observed during the first 12 h of ethanol withdrawal (Dahchour and De Witte, 1999). However, not all aspects of withdrawal are reduced by acamprosate, such as withdrawal-induced hypothermia (Spanagel et al, 1996b).

The results of these studies show that naltrexone and acamprosate can modify ethanol consumption and that the effectiveness of these compounds may involve different neuropharmacological mechanisms. Therefore, it is possible that the drugs might be complementary (ie more efficacious) when administered in combination. In order to test this hypothesis, the present study was conducted to examine the effects of naltrexone alone or in combination with acamprosate on responding for ethanol after a period of abstinence using an operant model of alcohol-seeking behavior. An increase in ethanol consumption typically is observed after a period of abstinence in rats (Le Magnen, 1960; Sinclair and Senter, 1967, 1968; Wolffgramm and Heyne, 1995; Spanagel et al, 1996a; Heyser et al, 1997, 1998), monkeys (Kornet et al, 1990), and in human social drinkers (Burish et al, 1981). This effect has been termed the ‘alcohol deprivation effect’ (Sinclair and Senter, 1967). This model was selected for several reasons. First, this paradigm mimics the conditions under which these drugs are likely to be administered in human alcoholics, presumably after detoxification and in an alcohol-free state. Second, this is one of the few models that addresses the role of abstinence on potential subsequent drinking behavior. It has been proposed that a deprivation-induced increase in drinking may be related to ‘loss of control’, which may have implications for relapse (Wolffgramm and Heyne, 1995). Third, it has been reported previously that administration of acamprosate blocked the increased ethanol consumption typically observed in rats after an imposed abstinence period (Hölter et al, 1997; Spanagel et al, 1996a; Heyser et al, 1998). And fourth, intermittent treatment with naltrexone has been shown to attenuate moderately the alcohol deprivation effect (Hölter and Spanagel, 1999).

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects

Subjects were 117 male Wistar rats (Charles River Laboratories, Hollister, CA). Rats were housed in groups of two per cage in a temperature- and humidity-controlled environment with a 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 10:00 pm). Food and water were available ad libitum, except as noted in the procedure. All procedures were conducted in accordance with the guidelines established by the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

Apparatus

Ethanol self-administration training was conducted in standard operant chambers (Coulbourn Instruments, Allentown, PA) that were located in sound-attenuated, ventilated environmental cubicles. Two 35-ml syringes dispensed either ethanol or water through plastic tubing into two stainless-steel drinking cups mounted 4 cm above the grid floor and centered on the front panel of each chamber. Each drinking cup could hold approximately two reinforcer deliveries (0.2 ml). Two retractable levers were located 4.5 cm to either side of the drinking cups. Fluid delivery and recording of operant responses were controlled by microcomputers.

Procedure

Operant procedure for oral ethanol self-administration training

All rats were trained to respond for ethanol or water in a two-lever free-choice situation. The procedure for induction of oral ethanol self-administration is a modification of Samson (1986) and has been successfully employed previously in our laboratory (Heyser et al, 1997, 1998). Rats were subjected to a 22-h water deprivation schedule only during the first 3 days of training (no ethanol was available during this period of training). Self-administration training was initiated in operant chambers by allowing rats to press either of two levers for a 0.2% (w/v) saccharin solution on a continuous reinforcement schedule (0.1 ml fluid/response) in 30-min daily sessions. After this initial training phase, and throughout the remainder of training and testing, water was freely available in the home cage. Starting on Day 4, rats were trained to alternate responding on the right and left lever (eg left, right, left, right, etc) in order to obtain a 5.0% ethanol+0.2% saccharin (w/v) solution. The alternation procedure was used to reduce positional (lever) biases in responding. Starting on Day 9 both levers were made available in a free-choice situation, with one lever producing an ethanol solution and the other lever producing water. During the next 10 days of training, ethanol concentrations were increased gradually to 8.0% and then to 10.0% ethanol (w/v). At the same time, saccharin concentrations were decreased to 0%. The lever that pro-duced ethanol continued to alternate from left to right on consecutive days throughout all training and testing to prevent establishment of a positional bias in responding. This procedure also maintains stable responding for both alcohol and water and permits a more stringent test of ethanol preference for animals, as the animals must track and respond to the appropriate lever necessary for obtaining ethanol. Rats were allowed to stabilize their intake of 10% ethanol (without saccharin) and water for 40–50 days and meet an established criterion for ethanol intake before being randomly assigned to the experimental groups. The criterion for stable responding was defined as ±20% of the previous day's total number of responses for ethanol for three consecutive sessions. All testing and training were conducted during the animal's dark cycle.

Experiment 1: effects of chronic injections of naltrexone

After the establishment of stable responding for ethanol, a 5-day deprivation period was imposed during which time ethanol was not available. During this period of ethanol deprivation, rats remained in their home cages with food and water available ad libitum. Each animal received chronic (twice daily) subcutaneous (s.c.) injections of saline or naltrexone (dissolved in saline) for 5 days. Injections of saline or naltrexone (0.005, 0.01, 0.06, 0.125, or 0.25 mg/kg) were made at 12-h intervals at approximately 6:00 am and 6:00 pm (n=7–8 per group; total n=45). Animals were tested 30 min after the last injection on Day 5.

Experiment 2: effects of chronic injections of naltrexone and acamprosate

After the establishment of stable responding for ethanol, a 5-day deprivation period was imposed during which time ethanol was not available. Each animal received a s.c. injection of saline or naltrexone (dissolved in saline) followed immediately by an intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection of saline or acamprosate (dissolved in saline) twice daily during the 5 days of ethanol deprivation. The doses for the combined treatment of naltrexone/acamprosate were: 0.005/25, 0.005/100, 0.01/25, 0.01/100, 0.06/25, 0.06/100, 0.125/25, and 0.125/100 mg/kg (the dose of naltrexone is listed first; n=8 per group; total n=72). These doses were selected based on the results of the first experiment (chronic injections of naltrexone) and previous studies (Heyser et al, 1998). This range of doses includes doses that eliminate the ethanol deprivation effect and doses that are ineffective in order to assess additivity and to test for possible synergistic effects of these compounds. Injections were made at 12-h intervals at approximately 6:00 am and 6:00 pm. Animals were tested 30 min after the last injection on Day 5.

Data Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed with the SPSS package for Windows version 8.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). Results of the operant procedure are reported as the mean total number of bar presses for ethanol and water. Data for the number of bar presses for ethanol and water were analyzed by a mixed factor analysis of variance (ANOVA), with Dose as a between-subjects factor and Day of testing as a within-subjects factor. Tukey's honestly significant difference tests and simple main effects analysis (Kirk, 1982) were used to determine the locus of significant main effects and interactions. A significance level of p<0.05 was used for all statistical analyses.

RESULTS

Experiment 1

The ANOVA conducted on the total number of bar presses for ethanol revealed a significant main effect of Day F(2,78)=10.24, p<0.05, along with a significant Day × Treatment interaction F(10,78)=8.91, p<0.05. As can be seen in Figure 1 (top panel), animals treated with saline or 0.005 and 0.01 mg/kg naltrexone and deprived of ethanol for 5 days significantly increased their responding for ethanol compared to baseline levels of responding on the first day post-deprivation (for saline animals: mean baseline intake=0.67 g/kg and mean post-deprivation Day 1=1.13 g/kg). In contrast, no increase in responding for ethanol was observed in animals chronically treated with 0.06, 0.125, or 0.25 mg/kg naltrexone. Furthermore, responding for ethanol was significantly decreased in animals treated with 0.125 and 0.25 mg/kg naltrexone on the first day post-deprivation. Responding for ethanol returned to baseline levels on the next day (second day post-deprivation) in these two groups. In contrast, animals treated with 0.06 mg/kg naltrexone significantly increased responding for ethanol on the second day post-deprivation compared to baseline levels. The effects of ethanol deprivation and naltrexone were selective for ethanol, and no significant alterations in responding for water were observed (see Figure 1, bottom panel).

Figure 1
figure 1

Effects of chronic administration of saline or naltrexone on responding for ethanol (top panel) or water (bottom panel) after 5 days of ethanol deprivation. Ethanol responding increased above baseline after 5 days of ethanol deprivation in animals treated with saline. Chronic administration of naltrexone eliminated the ethanol deprivation effect without affecting water responding. Data are presented as mean total responses+SE. *Indicates significantly greater than baseline responding (p<0.05). Indicates significantly lower than baseline responding (p<0.05).

To examine the time course of responding for ethanol, cumulative response records were generated (see Figure 2). As previously observed in our laboratory, animals generally make most of their responses for ethanol in the first 10–15 min of the session. A similar pattern of responding was observed in deprived animals during the first 10 min, and this pattern did not differ from baseline responding (see Figure 2). However, animals treated with saline or 0.005 and 0.01 mg/kg naltrexone during the 5-day period of ethanol deprivation continued to respond for ethanol throughout the 30-min session. This increased responding in the latter stages of the session was eliminated in animals chronically treated with 0.06 mg/kg naltrexone during the deprivation period. Interestingly, these animals significantly increased responding for ethanol on the second day post-deprivation compared to baseline levels. Animals treated with higher doses of naltrexone (0.125 and 0.25 mg/kg) significantly reduced responding for ethanol below baseline levels. Although total responding for ethanol was reduced in animals chronically treated with 0.125 or 0.25 mg/kg naltrexone, initial responding for ethanol was unaffected (see Figure 2). Therefore, in terms of the cessation of responding, administration of either 0.125 or 0.25 mg/kg naltrexone resulted in an earlier termination of responding compared to animals that received saline.

Figure 2
figure 2

Effects of chronic administration of saline or naltrexone on responding for ethanol after 5 days of ethanol deprivation. Mean cumulative responses for ethanol in 5-min intervals during baseline (open circles), 1 day post-deprivation (closed circles), and the second day post-deprivation (open squares). Each panel represents a different treatment. Ethanol responding increased above baseline after 5 days of ethanol deprivation in animals treated chronically with saline. Chronic administration of naltrexone eliminated the deprivation-induced increases in ethanol responding. Data are presented as mean+SE.

Experiment 2

The ANOVA conducted on the number of responses for ethanol revealed a significant main effect of Day F(2,126)=6.71, p<0.05, along with a significant Day × Treatment interaction F(16,126)=5.23, p<0.05. As can be seen in Figure 3 and Figure 4, animals treated with saline, 0.005/25, and 0.01/25 mg/kg (naltrexone+acamprosate) during the 5-day ethanol deprivation period significantly increased their responding for ethanol compared to baseline levels of responding on the first day post-deprivation. In contrast, no increase in responding for ethanol was observed in animals chronically treated with 0.005/100, 0.01/100, 0.06/25, and 0.06/100 mg/kg (naltrexone+acamprosate) on the first day post-deprivation. Furthermore, responding for ethanol was significantly decreased below baseline in animals treated with 0.125/25 and 0.125/100 mg/kg (naltrexone+acamprosate). The deprivation-induced increase in responding for ethanol was transient as all animals returned to baseline levels of responding on the second day post-deprivation. Further examination of cumulative response curves for ethanol revealed almost identical patterns of responding as that seen after the administration of naltrexone (see Figure 2) or acamprosate (see Heyser et al, 1998) given alone (data not shown). The effects of ethanol deprivation and drug treatment were selective for ethanol, and no significant alterations in responding for water were observed (see Figure 3 and Figure 4).

Figure 3
figure 3

Effects of chronic administration of saline or the combination of naltrexone+25 mg/kg acamprosate on responding for ethanol (top panel) and water (bottom panel) after 5 days of ethanol deprivation. Ethanol responding increased above baseline after 5 days of ethanol deprivation in animals treated with saline, 0.005 mg/kg naltrexone+acamprosate, and 0.01 mg/kg naltrexone+acamprosate. Chronic administration of higher doses of naltrexone and acamprosate eliminated the ethanol deprivation effect without affecting water responding. Data are presented as mean total responses+SE. *Indicates significantly greater than baseline responding (p<0.05). Indicates significantly lower than baseline responding (p<0.05).

Figure 4
figure 4

Effects of chronic administration of saline or the combination of naltrexone+100 mg/kg acamprosate on responding for ethanol (top panel) and water (bottom panel) after 5 days of ethanol deprivation. Ethanol responding increased above baseline after 5 days of ethanol deprivation in animals treated with saline. Chronic administration of naltrexone and acamprosate eliminated the ethanol deprivation effect without affecting water responding. Data are presented as mean total responses+SE. *Indicates significantly greater than baseline responding (p<0.05). Indicates significantly lower than baseline responding (p<0.05).

DISCUSSION

In the present study, increased responding for ethanol was observed after a period of deprivation compared to baseline levels of ethanol intake, confirming previous demonstrations of the alcohol deprivation effect (Le Magnen, 1960; Sinclair and Senter, 1967, 1968; Wolffgramm and Heyne, 1995; Spanagel et al, 1996a; Heyser et al, 1997). Chronic administration of naltrexone alone or in combination with acamprosate eliminated this deprivation-induced increase in drinking. These results confirm previous reports that opiate antagonists and acamprosate reduce the alcohol deprivation effect (Hölter et al, 1997; Spanagel et al, 1996a; Heyser et al, 1998; Hölter and Spanagel, 1999; Hölter et al, 2000). In the present study, the effects of drug administration had no effects on responding for water. Taken together with previous reports, these results provide further support for the effectiveness of each compound in modulating some aspects of alcohol-seeking behavior.

Chronic administration of naltrexone eliminated the ethanol deprivation effect and significantly decreased responding for ethanol at higher doses. These results confirm previous reports that opioid receptor antagonists reduce ethanol intake (Samson and Doyle, 1985; Hubbell et al, 1986; Froehlich et al, 1990; Weiss et al, 1990; Hyytia and Sinclair, 1993; June et al, 1999) and attenuate the ethanol deprivation effect (Hölter and Spanagel, 1999; Hölter et al, 2000). The reduction in responding for ethanol below baseline observed after administration of the higher doses (0.125 and 0.25 mg/kg) of naltrexone was specific to the first day after ethanol deprivation. Responding for ethanol in these treatment groups returned to baseline levels on the following day and remained stable across subsequent test days. Interestingly, the animals receiving 0.06 mg/kg naltrexone did not show an increase in drinking after the period of abstinence on the day ethanol was first made available (ie maintaining baseline levels of responding for ethanol). However, an increase in responding above baseline levels was observed the following day (post-deprivation Day 2). This may represent a rebound or delayed ethanol deprivation effect. This rebound may be due to the apparent threshold nature of this dose (still lower doses were without effect) and the fact that naltrexone treatments had been discontinued (ie the last injection of naltrexone was given 24 h prior to the second day post-deprivation).

The decrease in responding for ethanol after the administration of naltrexone was not immediate. All animals responded for ethanol within 30 s of the start of each session, regardless of drug administration. In addition, all groups initiated responding for ethanol and showed similar rates of responding during the first minutes of the operant session. Therefore, the blockade of opioid receptors does not seem to affect the onset of responding for ethanol, but results in the early cessation of responding in this limited-access setting. These data confirm several previous reports in animals that showed a delayed effect of opioid antagonists to affect ethanol intake (Hubbell et al, 1986; Hyytia and Sinclair, 1993; Slawecki et al, 1997; Heyser et al, 1999).

Chronic administration of naltrexone and acamprosate in combination resulted in an elimination of the alcohol deprivation effect. However, there was little evidence of any additive or synergistic effects of this combination on responding for ethanol on post-deprivation Day 1. More specifically, the administration of combinations including either 0.06 mg/kg naltrexone or 100 mg/kg acamprosate reduced the ethanol deprivation effect, and combinations including the higher dose of naltrexone (0.125 mg/kg) further reduced responding for ethanol below baseline levels. These data replicate Experiment 1 in the present study (effects of chronic injections of naltrexone) and confirm our previous report showing the elimination of the ethanol deprivation effect after the administration of 100 mg/kg acamprosate, but not after the administration of 25 mg/kg acamprosate (Heyser et al, 1998). Thus, the effects of these combined administrations on responding for ethanol are similar to those observed after the administration of either drug alone. Although acamprosate alone did not affect baseline responding for ethanol in this limited access paradigm (Heyser et al, 1998), there are reports showing a reduced consumption of ethanol after the administration of acamprosate (Boismare et al, 1984; Le Magnen et al, 1987; Spanagel et al, 1996a). However, rats in these studies were dependent on ethanol (Le Magnen et al, 1987) or selected for high ethanol intake (Boismare et al, 1984) or had an extensive history of ethanol consumption (24-h access to bottles of ethanol for 8 months) (Spanagel et al, 1996a). In contrast, animals in the present study were only trained to stable baseline (about 3–4 months) and in a limited access paradigm (30 min/day). Therefore, the effectiveness of acamprosate in reducing ethanol consumption appears to be related to ethanol preference and/or history (see Heyser et al, 1998 for discussion). This hypothesis is supported by a recent study by Stromberg et al (2001) that showed relatively little effect of acamprosate on baseline responding for ethanol. These authors also reported no synergistic or additive effects of the coadministration of acamprosate and naltrexone on baseline responding for ethanol.

The guiding hypothesis being tested in the current study was that the administration of naltrexone in combination with acamprosate would be more effective in modulating responding for ethanol than either drug given alone. This hypothesis was based on a number of empirical findings. First, these drugs have been shown to significantly reduce the rate of relapse in alcohol-dependent humans (see Garbutt et al, 1999; Litten et al, 1996; Mason and Ownby, 2000; for reviews of the clinic literature), despite the fact that these drugs work through different neurochemical mechanisms. Second, it has been hypothesized that naltrexone has a more direct effect on the positive reinforcing action of ethanol given that naltrexone decreases ethanol consumption in a variety of animal models (Samson and Doyle, 1985; Hubbell et al, 1986; Froehlich et al, 1990; Weiss et al, 1990; Hyytia and Sinclair, 1993; June et al, 1999). In contrast, the effectiveness of acamprosate on ethanol consumption appears to be dependent on the history of ethanol exposure. Acamprosate, unlike naltrexone, has been shown to reduce some of the symptoms of ethanol withdrawal (Dahchour and de Witte, 1999; Putzke et al, 1996; Cole et al, 2000). For example, in a recent report, Cole et al (2000) showed that acamprosate, but not naltrexone, inhibited conditioned abstinence behaviors. Taken together, this combination of drug therapies would seem ideal in the sense that acamprosate and naltrexone target aspects of both positive and negative reinforcement. However, there was little evidence of any additive or synergistic effects of this combination on responding for ethanol on post-deprivation Day 1.

Given this lack of additivity, it would appear from these data that acamprosate and naltrexone are working on parallel neuronal systems. Unfortunately, it is not possible at this time to fully describe the neuronal mechanisms responsible for the alcohol deprivation effect. The neuronal mechanisms are likely to be complex given the multiple actions of ethanol in the central nervous system and the fact that these systems may be (and often are) altered by the history of ethanol exposure. For example, chronic exposure to ethanol results in an upregulation of NMDA receptors (Grant, 1990; Liljequist, 1991; Hoffman and Tabakoff, 1994) and an upregulation in the density of voltage-dependent calcium channels (Messing et al, 1986; Dolin et al, 1987; Dolin and Little, 1989; Whittington and Little, 1991). More recently, Rasmussen et al (2002) has reported that mediobasal hypothalamus pro-opiomelanocortin (POMC) mRNA concentrations were decreased after chronic ethanol consumption. In contrast, an increase in forebrain POMC mRNA was observed after a period of ethanol deprivation (abstinence). Therefore, changes in the endogenous opioid systems are observed in response to the presence of ethanol and its subsequent removal. It is clear from other reports that these neuroadaptations are not limited to these systems and include alterations in serotonergic (McBride, Le, and Noronha, 2002) and dopaminergic (Bailey et al, 2001) function. These results illustrate the need for further research into these neuronal mechanisms. Furthermore, although the data from the present study failed to show a benefit from a combined treatment strategy on the acute alcohol deprivation effect using a limited access operant paradigm, caution should be taken in terms of generalizing these results to other experimental situations that include aspects of ethanol dependence and protracted abstinence (see Roberts et al, 2000).