The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Objective

Life-threatening danger is assumed to produce, in tandem, increases in both vigilance toward threat and stress-related symptoms, but no data test the validity of this assumption. The authors examined associations, in real time, among imminent life-threatening danger, stress-related symptoms, and vigilance.

Method

Symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), depression, and anxiety were measured in a civilian population (N=131) as a function of war-related stress, operationalized as the time available for seeking cover from rocket attack. A computerized measure of threat-related vigilance using a classic dot-probe attention task was also collected.

Results

PTSD symptoms, depression, and anxiety increased as a function of war-related threat. Acute proximal threat was associated with avoidance of, rather than vigilance toward, negative valence information. For participants within rocket range, the magnitude of threat bias varied with the magnitude of distress symptoms, such that as bias away from threat increased, distress symptoms increased.

Conclusions

These data challenge current thinking about the role of attention in stress responding. Attentional threat avoidance may reduce the acute impact of imminent threat, but this may come at a price in terms of an elevated risk for psychopathology.

The impact of traumatic stress is typically studied in the aftermath of violent incidents (17). However, virtually nothing is known about the acute symptomatic and cognitive correlates of traumatic stress, as manifest in real time, during life-threatening danger. This is due to the practical and ethical challenges researchers face when attempting to collect data in such situations, coupled with difficulties in quantifying the severity of threat in a graded fashion. Here, for the first time, we were able to assess the interplay among gradations of actual, imminent life-threatening danger, individual differences in stress-related symptoms, and attention to threat in humans.

With the outbreak of the Israel-Gaza War on December 27, 2008, civilians on both sides of the conflict experienced a sudden, dramatic increase in life-threatening danger. The immediacy of threat on the Israeli side rose in a graded fashion, as a function of proximity, throughout a 40-km area near the Gaza Strip. This reflected gradations in the physical and temporal proximity of threat (Figure 1). Residents of southern Israel, located within 10 km of the Gaza Strip, encountered proximal threat cues, such as exposure to blasts, property damage, and casualties. Importantly, these residents had only 15 seconds to enter protected space after hearing alert sirens indicating imminent rocket attack and were also exposed to mortar shelling to which no alert signal was provided. Residents within 10–30 km encountered fewer threat cues, were out of mortar fire range, and had a 30- to 45-second warning time to seek shelter from rocket attacks. Residents within 30 to 40 km encountered still fewer such cues and had a 60-second alert signal.

Figure 1. Symptoms of PTSD, Depression, and Anxiety as a Function of War-Related Threata

aWar-related threat was operationalized as the time available to seek shelter from imminent rocket attack.

Using standard self-report instruments, we measured symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), depression, and anxiety as a function of war-related stress, operationalized as the time available for seeking cover from rocket attack. We also collected a behavioral measure of threat-related vigilance using a classic dot-probe attention task (8). It is commonly assumed that life-threatening danger heightens vigilance to threat. However, laboratory-based studies actually suggest more complicated relations among stress exposure, threat vigilance, and individual differences in stress responding. This research suggests that very mild, experimentally induced stress can increase vigilance, but as stress increases in magnitude, it can have opposite effects, leading to avoidance of threat (915). For instance, various studies have shown that socially anxious individuals show an attentional bias away from threat only when observed under conditions of social-evaluative threat (e.g., before giving a speech) (911). Similar suppressions of threat bias were found in snake-fearful participants when exposed to a live snake (13) and in combat veterans with PTSD under conditions involving a future threat of watching a combat video (12). However, all of these investigations used relatively mild stressors. Virtually nothing is known about the interplay among more severe real-life stressors, clinically relevant distress, and threat vigilance.

Based on previous reports (2), we expected to find increased symptoms of PTSD, depression, and anxiety among participants who had shorter periods of time to respond to threat. We also expected that severe life threat would result in suppression of threat-related attention bias indexing threat avoidance and dissociation.

Method

Data Collection and Sample

The present study was approved by the Tel Aviv University Institutional Review Board. Access to participants was obtained through contact with families of students, living in the affected cities, in an introduction to psychology class. These students served as recruitment liaisons and accompanied our research team in data collection at each city. After the procedures had been fully explained, all participants >18 years of age and parents of participants <18 years of age provided written informed consent. Participants <18 years old provided assent. A total of 131 participants were recruited from urban populations (mean age=37.3 years [SD=16.10]; range=15–75 years; men: 33%). Of these, 99 were within the 40-km rocket range: 28 were within 10 km (Sderot), 47 were within 10–30 km (Ashdod and Kiryat-Gat), and 24 were within 30–40 km (Yavne). Thirty-two participants were from outside the rocket range (Tel Aviv). Data were collected over a 6-day period between January 3 and 8, 2009, during the height of the conflict when tens of rockets were fired and siren alerts were frequently sounded. Data were collected in participants' apartments (N=123), public meeting spaces (N=6), and bomb shelters (N=2). Inclusion criteria were >15 years of age and literacy in Hebrew. Although we could not perfectly control the environment in which the experiments were conducted, we did verify that participants were comfortably seated while completing the questionnaires. The dot-probe task was administered to all participants on a standard set of identical laptop computers (15-inch screens). A research assistant was present at all times to assist with the tasks and monitor the integrity of the procedure. Because of the stressful nature of the data collection, some of which was conducted under actual rocket fire, it was not possible to ensure equal numbers of male and female participants. Finally, because a diagnosis of dyslexia or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) might have implications for performance on the dot-probe threat attention task, participants who reported being diagnosed with dyslexia or ADHD were excluded from the analyses associated with this attention task. It was not feasible to conduct a comprehensive psychiatric assessment under the unusual circumstances of the study.

Mental Health Outcomes

Symptoms of PTSD were evaluated using the 17-item National Center for PTSD Checklist of the Department of Veterans Affairs (16, 17). Symptoms were related to any stressful experience (in the wording of the "specific stressor" version of the checklist). A clinical cutoff score of ≥50 was applied to determine the presence of clinically significant PTSD (18). Depression was measured using the 9-item self-report Patient Health Questionnaire (1921). The clinical cutoff for this measure was determined at a score of 10 (22). State anxiety was measured with the 20-item State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (23).

Threat-Bias Assessment: Dot-Probe Task

In the dot-probe task, two words, one threat-related (e.g., DEAD) and one neutral (e.g., DATA), are shown briefly in each trial, and their removal is followed by a small probe in the location previously occupied by one of the stimuli. Participants were required to respond as quickly as possible to the probe without compromising accuracy. Response latencies to the probe provided a snapshot of the distribution of participants' attention, with faster responses evident for the attended location relative to the unattended location. Attention bias toward threat is revealed when participants are faster to respond to probes that replace threat-related stimuli rather than neutral stimuli. The opposite pattern would indicate avoidance of threat stimuli (24).

Threat-related vigilance was evaluated using a Hebrew-adapted version of the classic word-based dot-probe task (8, 25). Stimuli were 38 threat/neutral word pairs. Within each pair, word length and frequency of usage in the Hebrew language were matched. Ratings of emotional valence by 18 independent judges were used to confirm that threat words were consistently rated as negative and neutral words as neutral. The task consisted of 152 trials in which threat/neutral word pairs were presented in a randomized order. Each trial began with a centrally presented fixation display ("+++" [display time: 500 msec]), immediately followed by a vertically aligned word pair written in 1-cm high white block text (display time: 1,000 msec). One word appeared directly above the location vacated by the preceding fixation signal, while the other appeared directly below. A distance of 3 cm separated the two words. The word pair was then replaced by a target probe that appeared in either of the two locations vacated by the words. Probe type was either a pair of dots or a single dot, determined randomly in each trial. Participants were required to identify which of the two probe types appeared by pressing the corresponding key as quickly as possible without compromising accuracy. The participant's response cleared the screen, and the next trial began 500 msec later.

Trials with incorrect response and trials in which the response time was two standard deviations of the participant's mean for a particular condition were excluded from subsequent analysis (<2% of all trials). Threat bias was calculated as the difference between the average response time to targets in neutral word locations and those in threat word locations. Positive bias values represent attention bias toward threat, whereas negative bias values reflect an attentional bias away from threat.

Threat-related attentional biases in anxious and nonanxious populations have been rigorously studied using the dot-probe task. There is broad consensus that performance on this task provides a reliable and valid measure of a participant's attention bias. Specifically, it has been demonstrated through narrative and quantitative meta-analytic reviews (24, 26, 27) that the dot-probe task generates consistent associations with individual differences in anxiety, rendering significant medium effect sizes of threat bias in anxious individuals. Dot-probe studies have been conducted with diverse populations, including individuals with clinical and subclinical anxiety, patient and normative samples, and children and adults (24). And, most relevant to the present study, the dot-probe task has been used to demonstrate threat-related bias in participants with PTSD (2830) and has proven to be sensitive to situational stress in laboratory-based studies (9, 15). Split-half reliability of the dot-probe task in the present sample was highly significant (reaction times for neutral and negative trials: all r values=0.99, df=111, all p values <0.0001; bias score: r=0.45, df=111, p<0.0001).

Statistical Analyses

Outcome variable scores (PTSD Checklist, Patient Health Questionnaire–9, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory) were separately subjected to univariate analyses of variance (ANOVAs), with situational stress (15-second alert, 30- to 45-second alert, 60-second alert, and no threat) as a between-subjects factor. Follow-up Scheffe contrasts were computed to clarify the nature of main effects. Reaction time data from the dot-probe task were subjected to repeated-measures ANOVA, with situational stress as a between-subjects factor and target location (threat, neutral) as a repeated within-subject measure. Threat bias scores were calculated as mean reaction time for targets in neutral word locations minus mean reaction time for targets in threat word locations to simplify presentation of a significant situational stress-by-target location interaction effect (Figure 1). Once again, follow-up Scheffe contrasts were computed to clarify the nature of main effects. In addition, we computed a correlation between threat bias and self-reported negative emotional symptoms. Negative emotional symptoms were indexed by a combined symptom measure, created by averaging the z scores from the PTSD Checklist, Patient Health Questionnaire–9, and State-Trait Anxiety Inventory–State Scale.

Results

PTSD and depression symptoms increased as a function of threat (i.e., shorter warning to seek shelter) (PTSD: F=6.77, df=3, 129, p<0.0001; depression: F=5.80, df=3, 129, p=0.001) (Figure 1). Follow-up contrasts indicated that PTSD and depression symptoms were equally low in participants outside of rocket range and participants who had 60 seconds to seek shelter. However, each additional reduction in alert time was mirrored by significant increases in symptoms. This pattern was mirrored by percentages of participants falling above clinical cutoff scores for PTSD (21.4%, 10.9%, 0%, and 3.1% for the 15-second, 30- to 45-second, 60-second, and no threat zones, respectively; χ2=8.98, df=3, p=0.03) and depression (51.9%, 37.0%, 12.5%, and 15.6% for the 15-second, 30- to 45-second, 60-second, and no threat zones, respectively; χ2=13.78, df=3, p=0.003). State anxiety was highest in participants who had only a 15-second warning to seek shelter, whereas having at least 30 to 45 seconds for doing so resulted in significantly lower levels of state anxiety (F=4.14, df=3, 129, p<0.008), which was not different from being outside of rocket range altogether (Figure 1).

As seen in Table 1, analyses of the attention task data revealed 1) a main effect for target location, such that reaction times to targets appearing in threat locations (mean reaction time=621 msec [SD=168]) were slower than reaction times to targets in neutral locations (mean reaction time=617 msec [SD=157]) (F=4.04, df=1, 110, p<0.05), and 2) a main effect of situational stress (i.e., time to seek shelter) (F=10.03, df=3, 110, p<0.0001). These effects were subsumed under a significant target location-by-situational stress interaction effect (F=2.86, df=3, 110, p<0.04). Specifically, participants living in cities under attack displayed a 5- to 12-msec attention bias away from threat, whereas participants living outside of rocket range displayed a 5-msec bias toward threat (Figure 1). To further explicate the main effect of situational stress on raw reaction time patterns, post hoc contrasts were computed. These analyses revealed that the raw reaction times of the participants who had 15 seconds to seek shelter and the raw reaction times of the participants who had 30–45 seconds of alert time were of similar magnitude (threat trials: p=0.61; neutral trials: p=0.69). In contrast, the reaction times of these two groups of participants (i.e., those who had <45 seconds to seek shelter) were significantly slower than those who had a 60-second alert time or who were outside of rocket range all together (p<0.003).

Table 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Civilians Within and Outside Rocket Range in the Israel-Gaza War
Table 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Civilians Within and Outside Rocket Range in the Israel-Gaza War
Enlarge table

Finally, for participants within rocket range, threat bias inversely correlated with a combined distress index (computed as the averaged z scores for PTSD, depression, and anxiety scales) (r=–0.22, df=81, p<0.05), such that as bias away from threat increased (became more negative) distress symptoms became greater. No correlation between threat bias and the combined distress index emerged for participants living outside of rocket range (r=0.11, df=30, p=0.56).

Discussion

Research on the consequences of extreme threat typically involves data collection in the aftermath of a violent incident as opposed to data acquired in real time in the context of life-threatening circumstances (14). In this study, we present novel data on the impact of war-related stress gathered during an ongoing rocket attack. Our data indicate a stepwise decline in PTSD and depression symptoms as a function of decreasing threat imminence, as roughly indexed by the amount of time available to seek shelter during a rocket attack. These results are akin to reports of increased PTSD and depression as a function of physical distance and level of exposure to the 9/11 attacks (2). The reaction time data add to a growing literature on the plasticity of attention bias to threat in highly anxious adults and children when studied under situations that provoke increases in state anxiety (15, 31, 32). We also found that state anxiety was specifically elevated in participants who had a very short time to seek safety but not in participants who had slightly longer intervals to prepare for an imminent attack. Taken together, these findings relate variations in objective threat level to variations in sense of control and available coping resources (33).

Our data indicate that individuals exposed to severe life-threatening risks manifest avoidance of, rather than vigilance toward, negative valence information. This pattern is in accord with previous laboratory reports using mild threat induction protocols (e.g., 12, 15). However, there appears to be a price for such avoidance in increased symptoms of psychopathology. Indeed, considerable research on the stress response suggests that extreme reactions may be adaptive in the short-term but associated with deleterious outcomes in the long-term (12, 34).

We also found that raw reaction times of participants who had <45 seconds to seek shelter were generally slower than raw reaction times of participants who had 60 seconds to do so or who were outside of rocket range. These results are congruent with established reports of a general psychomotor slowing in depressed and PTSD patients (3537) and with the fact that higher depression and PTSD symptoms were found among participants who had less time to respond to threat in the present study.

The interpretation of our findings should be considered in light of limitations to the study. First, despite our effort to implement standardized experimental conditions, we could not perfectly control the physical environments in which the assessments were conducted because of the special circumstances of the study. Nevertheless, secondary analyses suggested that this was unlikely to account for the associations we observed. Second, although the entire situation of being in a war zone and under repeated rocket attacks was considered stress exposure (with graded severity as a function of time to seek shelter), the interval between actual data collection and the exact timing of a rocket attack varied across participants. Third, our results focus on the associations among stress, attention to threat, and symptoms collected at one point in time. It will be important for future studies to track such associations prospectively to clarify the degree to which the direction of associations among attention and various distress measures remains stable or shows signs of change. Finally, to maintain consistency with prior research relating between-group differences in stress to differences in attention, we used the dot-probe task to assess attention bias toward threat. Future investigations might consider the degree to which other attention tasks generate similar or different findings.

In summary, findings from the present study challenge current thinking about the role of attention in response to acute threat. These data may be useful for those interested in treating clinically significant posttraumatic reactions by facilitating adaptive changes in attention.

Received July 9, 2009; revision received Oct. 1, 2009; accepted Oct. 16, 2009. From the Department of Psychology, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel; the Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York; the Mood and Anxiety Disorders Program, National Institute of Mental Health-National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md.; and the Department of Human Development, University of Maryland, College Park, Md. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Bar-Haim, Department of Psychology, Tel Aviv University, Tel-Aviv 69978, Israel; (e-mail).

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Supported by the Israeli Science Foundation (grant no. 964/08 [Dr. Bar-Haim]).

References

1 Bleich A , Gelkopf M , Solomon Z : Exposure to terrorism, stress-related mental health symptoms, and coping behaviors among a nationally representative sample in Israel. JAMA 2003; 290:612–620 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Galea S , Ahern J , Resnick H , Kilpatrick D , Bucuvalas M , Gold J , Vlahov D : Psychological sequelae of the September 11 terrorist attacks in New York City. N Engl J Med 2002; 346:982–987 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Hobfoll SE , Bridges A , Lomranz J , Eyal N , Tzemach M : Pulse of a nation: depressive mood reactions of Israelis to the Israel-Lebanon war. J Pers Soc Psychol 1989; 56:1002–1012 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Resnick HS , Kilpatrick DG , Dansky BS , Saunders BE , Best CL : Prevalence of civilian trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in a representative national sample of women. J Consult Clin Psychol 1993; 61:984–991 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Breslau N , Davis GC , Andreski P , Peterson E : Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress disorder in an urban population of young adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:216–222 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Kessler RC , Sonnega A , Bromet E , Hughes M , Nelson CB : Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52:1048–1060 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Norris FH : Epidemiology of trauma: frequency and impact of different potentially traumatic events on different demographic groups. J Consult Clin Psychol 1992; 60:409–418 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 MacLeod C , Mathews A , Tata P : Attentional bias in emotional disorders. J Abnorm Psychol 1986; 95:15–20 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Mansell W , Clark DM , Ehlers A , Chen YP : Social anxiety and attention away from emotional faces. Cogn Emot 1999; 13:673–690 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

10 Garner M , Mogg K , Bradley BP : Orienting and maintenance of gaze to facial expressions in social anxiety. J Abnorm Psychol 2006; 115:760–770 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Amir N , McNally RJ , Riemann BC , Burns J , Lorenz M , Mullen JT : Suppression of the emotional Stroop effect by increased anxiety in patients with social phobia. Behav Res Ther 1996; 34:945–948 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Constans JI , McCloskey MS , Vasterling JJ , Brailey K , Mathews A : Suppression of attentional bias in PTSD. J Abnorm Psychol 2004; 113:315–323 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Mathews A , Sebastian S : Suppression of emotional Stroop effects by fear-arousal. Cogn Emot 1993; 7:517–530 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

14 Mogg K , Kentish J , Bradley BP : Effects of anxiety and awareness on color-identification latencies for emotional words. Behav Res Ther 1993; 31:559–567 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Helfinstein SM , White LK , Bar-Haim Y , Fox NA : Affective primes suppress attention bias to threat in socially anxious individuals. Behav Res Ther 2008; 46:799–810 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Blanchard EB , Jones-Alexander J , Buckley TC , Forneris CA : Psychometric properties of the PTSD Checklist (PCL). Behav Res Ther 1996; 34:669–673 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Weathers FW , Litz BT , Herman DS , Huska JA , Keane TM : The PTSD Checklist (PCL): reliability, validity, and diagnostic utility, in the Proceedings of the 1993 Annual Meeting of the International Society of Traumatic Stress Studies. Deerfield, Ill, International Society of Traumatic Stress Studies, 1993. http://www.pdhealth.mil/library/downloads/PCL_sychometrics.doc Google Scholar

18 Hoge CW , Castro CA , Messer SC , McGurk D , Cotting DI , Koffman RL : Combat duty in Iraq and Afghanistan, mental health problems, and barriers to care. N Engl J Med 2004; 351:13–22 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Spitzer R , Kroenke K , Williams J : Validation and utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD: the PHQ Primary Care Study. JAMA 1999; 282:1737–1744 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Lowe B , Kroenke K , Herzog W , Grafe K : Measuring depression outcome with a brief self-report instrument: sensitivity to change of the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ–9). J Affect Disord 2004; 81:61–66 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Kroenke K , Spitzer RL : The PHQ–9: a new depression and diagnostic severity measure. Psychiatr Ann 2002; 32:509–521 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

22 Kroenke K , Spitzer RL , Williams JB : The PHQ–9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med 2001; 16:606–613 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Spielberger CD , Gorsuch RL , Lushene R , Vagg PR , Jacobs GA : Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, Calif, Consulting Psychologists Press, 1983 Google Scholar

24 Bar-Haim Y , Lamy D , Pergamin L , Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ , van IJzendoorn MH : Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Bull 2007; 133:1–24 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 MacLeod C , Rutherford E , Campbell L , Ebsworthy G , Holker L : Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. J Abnorm Psychol 2002; 111:107–123 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Mogg K , Bradley BP : A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behav Res Ther 1998; 36:809–848 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Pine DS , Helfinstein SM , Bar-Haim Y , Nelson E , Fox NA : Challenges in developing novel treatments for childhood disorders: lessons from research on anxiety. Neuropsychopharmacology 2009; 34:213–228 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Elsesser K , Sartory G , Tackenberg A : Attention, heart rate, and startle response during exposure to trauma-relevant pictures: a comparison of recent trauma victims and patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 2004; 113:289–301 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Dalgleish T , Taghavi R , Neshat-Doost H , Moradi A , Canterbury R , Yule W : Patterns of processing bias for emotional information across clinical disorders: a comparison of attention, memory, and prospective cognition in children and adolescents with depression, generalized anxiety, and posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2003; 32:10–21 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Bryant RA , Harvey AG : Attentional bias in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress 1997; 10:635–644 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Monk CS , Nelson EE , McClure EB , Mogg K , Bradley BP , Leibenluft E , Blair RJR , Chen G , Charney DS , Ernst M , Pine DS : Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation and attentional bias in response to angry faces in adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:1091–1097 LinkGoogle Scholar

32 Pine DS , Mogg K , Bradley BP , Montgomery L , Monk CS , McClure E , Guyer AE , Ernst M , Charney DS , Kaufman J : Attention bias to threat in maltreated children: implications for vulnerability to stress-related psychopathology. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:291–296 LinkGoogle Scholar

33 Hobfoll SE : Conservation of resources: a new attempt at conceptualizing stress. Am Psychol 1989; 44:513–524 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34 Lupien SJ , McEwen BS , Gunnar MR , Heim CM : Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009; 10:434–445 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 McNally RJ : Psychological mechanisms in acute response to trauma. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 53:779–788 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36 Hart RP , Kwentus JA : Psychomotor slowing and subcortical-type dysfunction in depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1987; 50:1263–1266 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 White DA , Myerson J , Hale S : How cognitive is psychomotor slowing in depression? Evidence from a meta-analysis. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 1997; 4:166–174 CrossrefGoogle Scholar