The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Objective:

Twin studies suggest that genetic factors contribute to continuity in mental health problems and that environmental factors are the major contributor to developmental change. The authors investigated the influence of psychiatric risk alleles on early-onset mental health trajectories and whether the trajectories were subsequently modified by exposure to childhood victimization.

Methods:

The sample was a prospective U.K. population-based cohort, the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. The developmental trajectories of emotional problems were estimated in childhood (approximately ages 4–8 years) and adolescence (approximately ages 12–17 years). Psychiatric risk alleles were indexed by polygenic risk scores (PRS) for schizophrenia using genome-wide association study results from the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Chronic peer victimization in late childhood (ages 8.5 and 10.5 years) was assessed as an index of environmental exposure. Individuals with sufficient data on emotional problems, the PRS, and victimization were included in the main analyses (N=3,988).

Results:

Higher schizophrenia PRSs were associated with a trajectory of early-onset increasing emotional problems (odds ratio=1.18, 95% CI=1.02–1.36) compared with a trajectory of low-stable emotional problems. Subsequent exposure to victimization increased the likelihood of transitioning from a trajectory of low-stable emotional problems during childhood (before exposure) to an increasing trajectory in adolescence (after exposure) (odds ratio=2.59, 95% CI=1.48–4.53).

Conclusions:

While the early development of emotional problems was associated with genetic risk (schizophrenia risk alleles), the subsequent course of emotional problems for those who might otherwise have remained on a more favorable trajectory was altered by exposure to peer victimization, which is a potentially modifiable environmental exposure.

Most psychiatric disorders, regardless of when they first manifest, originate in childhood (1). However, early childhood problems are not associated with later mental health outcomes for all; there is change as well as continuity across development (1). Twin studies consistently show that continuity in mental health problems across time is highly heritable (2, 3). This suggests that genetic factors make an important contribution to developmental trajectories. However, twin studies also show that environmental factors are a major contributor to change (3, 4). Identifying potentially malleable environmental factors that may alter the developmental course of heritable mental health problems is an important step toward guiding prevention strategies.

Molecular genetic studies have revealed that although the genetic architecture of psychiatric disorders is complex, it is now possible to assign to individuals a biologically valid indicator of common variant genetic liability for that phenotype (polygenic risk score, or PRS) (5). To date, genetic findings in schizophrenia have led the field of PRS research, largely reflecting the relatively high power of genome-wide association studies of that disorder (6). PRS derived from schizophrenia risk alleles predicts increased liability not only to that disorder but also to other adult psychiatric disorders (e.g., major depression) and to mental health and neurodevelopmental traits in childhood (7, 8). Associations between schizophrenia PRS and anxiety or emotional problems have now been observed across the lifespan—in childhood, adolescence, and adult life (811). These cross-sectional observations suggest that schizophrenia risk alleles may contribute to the trajectories of emotional problems from an early age, although this has not been tested directly. However, genetics alone cannot explain the developmental course of emotional problems (1). Psychosocial stressors also contribute risk; some are particularly prevalent in childhood (12). An example is chronic childhood peer victimization, a common social stressor in childhood that contributes to subsequent emotional problems in childhood and later adult life (1318). Being bullied has been found to be associated with childhood emotional symptoms even when allowing for genetic confounding (15, 16, 18). It is important to investigate genetic and environmental risk factors together to gain understanding of how these factors simultaneously affect the longitudinal course of mental health problems.

In this study, we used a large prospective population-based cohort, the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC), that underwent the same repeated mental health assessments from ages 4 to 17 years, to test specific hypotheses concerning the association of schizophrenia PRS and childhood victimization with emotional problem trajectories. We postulated first that schizophrenia risk alleles (PRS) contribute to early-onset emotional problems that remain on an unfavorable trajectory. Second, we hypothesized that chronic peer victimization in late childhood modifies early trajectories by increasing the likelihood of transitioning from a low symptom trajectory in childhood (before exposure) to an elevated trajectory in adolescence (after exposure). We also explored the effect of the presence or absence of victimization for those already on an elevated trajectory in early childhood.

Methods

Sample

The ALSPAC is a well-established prospective, longitudinal birth cohort study. The enrolled core sample consisted of 14,541 mothers living in Avon, England, who had expected delivery dates between April 1, 1991, and December 31, 1992. Of these pregnancies, 13,988 children were alive at 1 year. When the oldest children were approximately 7 years of age, the sample was augmented with eligible cases who had not joined the study originally, resulting in enrollment of 713 additional children. The resulting total sample size of children alive at 1 year was 14,701. After quality control, genotype data were available for 8,365 children. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and local research ethics committees. Full details of the study, measures, and sample can be found elsewhere (19, 20). The study web site contains details of all the data available through a fully searchable data dictionary (http://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/data-dictionary). Where families included multiple births, we included the oldest sibling.

Emotional Problems

Emotional problems were assessed using the parent-rated five-item emotional problems subscale (range=0–10) of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (21), which includes primarily anxiety items (often complains of headaches; many worries; often unhappy, downhearted; nervous or clingy in new situations; many fears, easily scared). “Childhood” data were collected at ages 47, 81, and 97 months (approximately ages 4–8 years), and “adolescent” data were collected at ages 140, 157, and 198 months (approximately ages 12–17 years).

Polygenic Risk Scores

Schizophrenia PRSs were generated as the weighted mean number of disorder risk alleles in approximate linkage equilibrium (R2<0.25), derived from dosage data of 1,813,169 imputed autosomal single-nucleotide polymorphisms using standard procedures (7). Risk alleles were defined as those associated with case status in the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium analyses of schizophrenia (35,476 cases and 46,839 controls) at a threshold of p<0.05 because this optimally captures phenotypic variance (6). Associations across a range of p-value thresholds are shown in Figure S1 in the online supplement. Genotyping details, as well as full methods for generating the PRS, can be found elsewhere (8).

Peer Victimization

Peer victimization was assessed by interviews with the children using a nine-item modified version of the bullying and friendship interview schedule (13, 22) that asked about different types of overt and relational victimization experienced in the past 6 months. Data were collected at ages 8.5 and 10.5 years (after the “childhood” and before the “adolescent” emotional problems assessments). Individuals who reported having been victim to any of the items “frequently” or “very frequently” (several times a month or week) at both ages were classified as having been exposed to (chronic) peer victimization (11.6% of the sample). Those who reported being victims “seldom” or “never” for all items at either age were categorized as not having been exposed to (chronic) peer victimization.

Statistical Analysis

Analyses were conducted in several steps, as depicted in Figure 1 and in the final model in Figure 2. We modeled emotional problem trajectories by fitting growth mixture models separately for the three childhood time points and for the three adolescent time points. Separate models for childhood and adolescence were fitted to enable investigation of trajectory change before and after exposure to victimization. Growth mixture modeling groups individuals into trajectory classes (categories) based on patterns of growth (change) (23). Grouping individuals enabled us to investigate whether exposure to victimization was associated with transitioning from having low emotional problems in childhood to having problems in adolescence. A latent class approach enabled us to identify unmeasured probabilistic subgroups (classes) based on the developmental patterns (trajectories) from repeated measures of observed variables (emotional problems). Starting with a single k-class solution, k+1 solutions were fitted until the optimum solution was reached. Growth mixture model solutions were selected based on fit indices (see the online supplement).

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. Analysis steps in a study of the developmental contributions of schizophrenia risk alleles and childhood peer victimization to early-onset mental health trajectoriesa

a C1=class 1 (childhood); C2=class 2 (adolescence); EP=emotional problems; GMM=growth mixture model; i=intercept; LTA=latent transition analysis; PRS=polygenic risk score; s=slope; V=victimization.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2. Final model of associations between the polygenic risk score (PRS), victimization (V), and transition between childhood and adolescent emotional problem (EP) classesa

a C1=class 1 (childhood); C2=class 2 (adolescence); i=intercept; s=slope.

Once trajectory classes had been defined, we assessed whether schizophrenia PRS predicted childhood and adolescent trajectory classes. We also checked for associations between schizophrenia PRS and child reports of victimization.

Next, we examined transitions between childhood and adolescent emotional problem classes (i.e., before and after exposure to victimization). These were modeled by latent transition analysis using a bias-adjusted three-step approach, which accounts for measurement error in class assignment (24). Measurement invariance between the childhood and adolescent trajectories was not assumed, given possible differences in longitudinal patterns (25).

We tested whether schizophrenia PRS and victimization were associated with change (transition) in emotional problem trajectory class (from before exposure to victimization in childhood to after exposure in adolescence) (26). Testing association with transition combines the direct effect of schizophrenia PRS and victimization on adolescent trajectory classes and the moderating effect of schizophrenia PRS and victimization on the association between the childhood and adolescence classes.

Sensitivity analyses were conducted to include potential confounders, to exclude individuals who (according to an earlier parent-report measure) were exposed to prior peer victimization (before the self-reported victimization assessment at age 8.5 years), and to assess the impact of missing data (see below). Full details of the sensitivity analyses are provided in the online supplement.

All analyses were conducted in Mplus using a maximum likelihood parameter estimator, for which standard errors are robust to nonnormality (27). Binary or multinomial logistic regressions were used as appropriate, and associations are presented as odds ratios. Multinomial logistic regression technically estimates multinomial odds ratios (or relative risk ratios); however, we refer to effects as odds ratios (usually used for two exhaustive categories) throughout the Results section for clarity.

Missing Data

Details of the available sample sizes at each step of analysis are shown in shown in Figure S2 in the online supplement. The starting sample size included individuals with data on emotional problems for at least two of the three relevant time points in either childhood (N=8,425) or adolescence (N=7,018). These individuals were included in the growth mixture models that were used to derive the emotional problems classes; growth mixture models were conducted in Mplus using full information maximum likelihood estimation (27). Growth mixture models data in both childhood and adolescence were available for 6,146 participants, of whom 75% had genetic data and 83% had victimization data; 65% had both genetic and victimization data and thus formed our “main sample” (N=3,988). Inverse probability weighting (28) was used to assess the impact of missing data, whereby observations were weighted based on measures assessed in pregnancy that were predictive of variables in the analysis and/or inclusion in the main sample (see the online supplement).

Results

Modeling Emotional Problem Developmental Trajectories

Descriptive statistics, including gender differences, are listed in Table S1 in the online supplement. As shown in Figure 3, in both childhood (N=8,425) and adolescence (N=7,018), we observed three emotional problem trajectory classes: low (79.2% in childhood; 83.3% in adolescence), decreasing (11.0% in childhood; 7.6% in adolescence), and increasing (9.8% in childhood; 9.1% in adolescence). Subsequent analyses of trajectories focus on the (less favorable) increasing class compared with the (most favorable) low emotional problems class.

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3. Emotional problem classes by age in a study of the developmental contributions of schizophrenia risk alleles and childhood peer victimization to early-onset mental health trajectoriesa

a For each class, solid lines indicate mean trajectories, and dotted lines indicate 95% confidence intervals.

There was strong evidence for an association between childhood and adolescent emotional problem trajectory classes, although confidence intervals were wide; compared with the low class in childhood, a much higher proportion of the individuals in the increasing class in childhood were also in the increasing class in adolescence (odds ratio=17.07, 95% CI=10.30–28.30, p<0.001). All transition probabilities for the latent transition analysis are shown in Figure S3 in the online supplement.

Schizophrenia PRS and Emotional Problem Developmental Trajectories

Higher schizophrenia PRSs were associated with an elevated likelihood of being in the increasing emotional problem trajectory class in childhood (odds ratio=1.18, 95% CI=1.02–1.36, p=0.030). Although the adolescent trajectory classes were strongly associated with prior childhood trajectories, schizophrenia PRS still showed some independent association with emotional problem trajectories in adolescence (odds ratio=1.17, 95% CI=1.00–1.36, p=0.050) when compared with the low class.

Self-Reported Victimization Exposure

Victimization exposure in late childhood was predicted by earlier childhood emotional problems—the increasing childhood trajectory class (odds ratio=1.79, 95% CI=1.21–2.66, p=0.004). However, schizophrenia PRSs were not associated with exposure to child-reported chronic victimization (odds ratio=0.95, 95% CI=0.86–1.04, p=0.292).

Schizophrenia PRS and Victimization: Associations With Trajectory Changes

Schizophrenia PRSs were not associated with transitioning from the low childhood trajectory class to the increasing trajectory class in adolescence (odds ratio=1.04, 95% CI=0.81–1.34, p=0.758).

Chronic peer victimization, however, was associated with transition from the low trajectory class in childhood (before exposure) to the increasing trajectory class in adolescence (after exposure; odds ratio=2.59, 95% CI=1.48–4.53, p=0.001). This association held when schizophrenia PRSs were included in the model (odds ratio=2.57, 95% CI=1.46–4.52, p=0.001).

Post hoc analyses suggested that for those already in the childhood increasing trajectory class, victimization did not alter the trajectory in adolescence because it was not associated with transitions for this trajectory class (overall Wald χ2=3.61, df=2, p=0.165) (adolescent increasing relative to low trajectory class, odds ratio=2.51, 95% CI=0.54–11.68; decreasing relative to low trajectory class, odds ratio=4.26, 95% CI=0.94–19.39).

Sensitivity Analyses

A similar pattern of results was obtained when sex, social class, maternal depression, home ownership, education, and marital status were included as covariates (see Table S3 in the online supplement).

Excluding individuals who were exposed to prior peer victimization (maternal reports at ages 4–8 years) also revealed a similar pattern of results, with the exception that the association between victimization and transitioning from the low to the increasing class was reduced (odds ratio=1.90, 95% CI= 0.90–4.00, p=0.093; see the online supplement). Thus, we observed that the association between chronic peer victimization and transitioning from the low to the increasing class may be driven by individuals exposed to particularly chronic victimization (i.e., that which occurred in early childhood as well as at ages 8.5 and 10.5 years).

Using inverse probability weighting to assess the impact of missing data did not change the interpretation of results (see the online supplement).

Discussion

Our aim in this study was to investigate the contribution of schizophrenia risk alleles to developmental trajectories of emotional problems across childhood and adolescence in the general population. We also set out to assess whether early developmental trajectories could be shifted by an environmental stressor—peer victimization. Specifically, we tested the hypotheses that schizophrenia risk alleles, indexed by PRS, would be associated with an elevated trajectory of early-onset emotional problems that persisted through adolescence and that exposure to chronic peer victimization would alter the subsequent developmental course of trajectories. Our findings suggest that schizophrenia risk alleles contribute to an increasing trajectory of emotional problems in early childhood and adolescence. Later environmental risk exposure—in this case, chronic peer victimization—contributes to change over time. The findings suggest that there are at least two routes into an increasing trajectory of emotional problems during adolescence. The first is via genetically influenced childhood-onset emotional problems, which show strong continuity with adolescent emotional problems, and the second is via exposure to peer victimization, which alters the developmental course of individuals who are initially on a low-risk trajectory to a less favorable trajectory.

The results from this study supported our first hypothesis that schizophrenia PRSs contribute to a developmental trajectory of increasing emotional problems that begin early in childhood. However, the PRS did not explain the transition between childhood and adolescent trajectories, and developmental trajectories during adolescence were most strongly predicted by earlier childhood trajectories. This suggests that PRS effects during adolescence are predominantly explained by association with earlier childhood trajectories; they do not contribute substantially to changes in emotional problem trajectories. This finding is consistent with cross-sectional genetics research, including in this sample, that has shown associations between schizophrenia PRS and emotional problems in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood (811), and with twin studies, which infer genetic contribution to continuity in mental health problems (2). Taken together, these observations suggest that interventions aimed at improving developmental trajectories for individuals at elevated genetic risk of mental health problems likely need to begin very early in life—in the preschool years.

The findings also suggest that exposure to chronic peer victimization in late childhood further shapes the developmental course of emotional problems. Specifically, exposure to victimization during childhood that was not predicted by schizophrenia PRS altered subsequent adolescent trajectories. We observed an increased likelihood of individuals transitioning from a consistently low emotional problem trajectory in childhood (before victimization exposure) to a trajectory of increasing emotional problems in adolescence (after exposure). Twin studies have repeatedly highlighted environmental factors as important contributors to change in mental health over time (4), and chronic childhood peer victimization is considered a robust risk factor for emotional problems and depression, even when using genetically sensitive twin designs (15, 16, 18).

We also observed that exposure to subsequent chronic victimization was associated with prior increasing emotional problems in childhood. Psychopathology is known to increase the likelihood of exposure to environmental risk factors, including victimization (29). However, post hoc analyses found that victimization was not associated with change in emotional problems for those who were on the less favorable trajectory of (increasing) emotional problems in childhood. This suggests that although experiencing chronic victimization is associated with developing new emotional problems in adolescence, it may not drive the persistence of very early-onset chronic difficulties; that is, eliminating peer victimization may not prevent ongoing problems for those who are already on a trajectory of increasing emotional problems, although further work testing this hypothesis is required. Nevertheless, in line with previous research (30), our findings suggest that children with early-onset emotional problems may benefit from monitoring of peer relations. An interesting direction for future research would be to investigate whether protective environmental factors can alter the course of trajectories of early emotional problems away from later emotional problems (31).

This study has a number of strengths, including the integration of molecular genetic and epidemiological approaches to investigate the effects of both genetic and environmental risks on developmental trajectories. However, our findings should be considered in light of some limitations. First, the ALSPAC is a longitudinal birth cohort study that suffers from nonrandom attrition, whereby individuals with higher levels of psychopathology and higher PRSs are less likely to be retained in the study (32, 33). Analyses using inverse probability weighting to assess the effect of missing data did not change the interpretation of results, suggesting that this did not have a major impact on our results. Despite a large sample for analysis (N=3,988), sample size may have affected our ability to detect other trajectory classes, such as those with persistently high problems, which may be reflected in our wide confidence intervals when testing for associations with the different trajectories. This could also have been the result of running separate models for childhood and adolescence, although other studies that have examined trajectories of emotional problems across childhood and adolescence have also not identified a “persistent” trajectory (34). Because only three time points for each of the growth models were available, we were also able to model only linear change in emotional problems; other patterns may better reflect developmental changes. We also used a parent-report questionnaire measure to assess emotional problems, which may not generalize to diagnoses, although the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire is a well-validated measure (21), and our classes were associated with depression diagnosis at age 18 years (see the online supplement). Moreover, using the same measure and informant is a strength for assessing developmental trajectories; otherwise, change could be explained by measurement differences. In addition, schizophrenia PRS currently explains a minority of common variant liability to the disorder (6), and the effect sizes we observed are typical for this kind of work (9). For example, adopting an approach used previously to quantify effects (35), individuals in the top 2.5% of schizophrenia PRSs would have roughly a 36% increase in odds of having increasing, compared with low, emotional problems in childhood. Thus, PRSs should be regarded as indicators of genetic liability rather than as predictors. We did not find schizophrenia PRS to be associated with child-reported victimization. However, we cannot rule out the possibility of genetic confounding; findings might differ depending on who is reporting the victimization (here we used child reports, rather than parent reports) and the severity of bullying. In addition, other types of genetic variants could still contribute to links between victimization and emotional problems.

Finally, our analyses and hypotheses were shaped by previous work (16) as well as the availability in the ALSPAC of three emotional symptom assessments prior to exposure to victimization and three assessments after exposure. However, our methods could be used in other cohorts to address developmental questions relevant to other environmental exposures, such as life events (36), and for additional psychiatric outcomes (18). Additional work investigating how genetic risk variants work together with environmental risk factors, as well as with protective factors, on a range of psychiatric outcomes will be needed, although rigorous methods are needed to know which environmental risk factors are likely causal (37, 38).

We found that a higher burden of schizophrenia risk alleles is associated with a developmental trajectory of increasing emotional problems that begins in early childhood. Exposure to chronic peer victimization in late childhood alters emotional problem trajectories, whereby individuals in a stable-low state across childhood transition to a trajectory of increasing emotional problems in adolescence.

Division of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University, United Kingdom (Riglin, Collishaw, A.K. Thapar, O’Donovan, A. Thapar); Population Health Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom (Hammerton, Heron); MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King’s College London, United Kingdom (Arseneault, Maughan).
Send correspondence to Prof. Anita Thapar ().

Presented at the World Congress of Psychiatric Genetics, Orlando, Fla., Oct. 13–17, 2017.

The U.K. Medical Research Council (MRC) and the Wellcome Trust (grant 102215/2/13/2) and the University of Bristol provide core support for the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Genome-wide association study data were generated by sample logistics and genotyping facilities at the Wellcome Trust Sanger Institute and LabCorp (Laboratory Corporation of America) using support from 23andMe. This work was primarily supported by the MRC (grants MR/M012964/1, G0800509, and G0801418). The MRC and Alcohol Research UK provided additional support to Drs. Hammerton and Heron (grant MR/L022206/1). Dr. Arseneault is the Mental Health Leadership Fellow for the Economic and Social Research Council in the United Kingdom.

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

The authors acknowledge the members of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium for the publicly available data used as the discovery sample in this article. The authors are grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses.

References

1 Rutter M, Kim-Cohen J, Maughan B: Continuities and discontinuities in psychopathology between childhood and adult life. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2006; 47:276–295Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Ronald A: Is the child ‘father of the man’? Evaluating the stability of genetic influences across development. Dev Sci 2011; 14:1471–1478Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Hannigan LJ, Walaker N, Waszczuk MA, et al.: Aetiological influences on stability and change in emotional and behavioural problems across development: a systematic review. Psychopathol Rev 2017; 4:52–108Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Rutter M, Pickles A, Murray R, et al.: Testing hypotheses on specific environmental causal effects on behavior. Psychol Bull 2001; 127:291–324Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Sullivan PF, Agrawal A, Bulik CM, et al.: Psychiatric genomics: an update and an agenda. Am J Psychiatry 2018; 175:15–27LinkGoogle Scholar

6 Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium: Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature 2014; 511:421–427Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium: Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet 2013; 381:1371–1379Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Riglin L, Collishaw S, Richards A, et al.: Schizophrenia risk alleles and neurodevelopmental outcomes in childhood: a population-based cohort study. Lancet Psychiatry 2017; 4:57–62Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Jones HJ, Stergiakouli E, Tansey KE, et al.: Phenotypic manifestation of genetic risk for schizophrenia during adolescence in the general population. JAMA Psychiatry 2016; 73:221–228Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Nivard MG, Gage SH, Hottenga JJ, et al.: Genetic overlap between schizophrenia and developmental psychopathology: longitudinal and multivariate polygenic risk prediction of common psychiatric traits during development. Schizophr Bull 2017; 43:1197–1207Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Riglin L, Collishaw S, Richards A, et al.: The impact of schizophrenia and mood disorder risk alleles on emotional problems: investigating change from childhood to middle age. Psychol Med 2017; 48:2153–2158Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Silberg J, Rutter M, Neale M, et al.: Genetic moderation of environmental risk for depression and anxiety in adolescent girls. Br J Psychiatry 2001; 179:116–121Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Bowes L, Joinson C, Wolke D, et al.: Peer victimisation during adolescence and its impact on depression in early adulthood: prospective cohort study in the United Kingdom. BMJ 2015; 350:h2469Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Takizawa R, Maughan B, Arseneault L: Adult health outcomes of childhood bullying victimization: evidence from a five-decade longitudinal British birth cohort. Am J Psychiatry 2014; 171:777–784LinkGoogle Scholar

15 Silberg JL, Copeland W, Linker J, et al.: Psychiatric outcomes of bullying victimization: a study of discordant monozygotic twins. Psychol Med 2016; 46:1875–1883Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Arseneault L, Milne BJ, Taylor A, et al.: Being bullied as an environmentally mediated contributing factor to children’s internalizing problems: a study of twins discordant for victimization. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2008; 162:145–150Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Bowes L, Maughan B, Ball H, et al.: Chronic bullying victimization across school transitions: the role of genetic and environmental influences. Dev Psychopathol 2013; 25:333–346Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Singham T, Viding E, Schoeler T, et al.: Concurrent and longitudinal contribution of exposure to bullying in childhood to mental health: the role of vulnerability and resilience. JAMA Psychiatry 2017; 74:1112–1119Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Boyd A, Golding J, Macleod J, et al.: Cohort profile: the ‘children of the 90s’: the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol 2013; 42:111–127Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Fraser A, Macdonald-Wallis C, Tilling K, et al.: Cohort profile: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int J Epidemiol 2013; 42:97–110Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Goodman R: The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1997; 38:581–586Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Wolke D, Woods S, Stanford K, et al.: Bullying and victimization of primary school children in England and Germany: prevalence and school factors. Br J Psychol 2001; 92:673–696Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Muthén B, Muthén LK: Integrating person-centered and variable-centered analyses: growth mixture modeling with latent trajectory classes. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2000; 24:882–891Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Asparouhov T, Muthén B: Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: three-step approaches using Mplus. Struct Equ Modeling 2014; 21:329–341CrossrefGoogle Scholar

25 Maughan B, Collishaw S: Development and psychopathology: a life course perspective, in Rutter’s Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Edited by Thapar A, Pine DS, Leckman JF, et al.. Oxford, UK, John Wiley & Sons, 2015CrossrefGoogle Scholar

26 Lanza ST, Collins LM: A new SAS procedure for latent transition analysis: transitions in dating and sexual risk behavior. Dev Psychol 2008; 44:446–456Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Muthén LK, Muthén BO: Mplus User’s Guide, 7th ed. Los Angeles, Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2012Google Scholar

28 Seaman SR, White IR: Review of inverse probability weighting for dealing with missing data. Stat Methods Med Res 2013; 22:278–295Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Arseneault L, Bowes L, Shakoor S: Bullying victimization in youths and mental health problems: ‘much ado about nothing’? Psychol Med 2010; 40:717–729Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Bowes L, Arseneault L, Maughan B, et al.: School, neighborhood, and family factors are associated with children’s bullying involvement: a nationally representative longitudinal study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2009; 48:545–553Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Collishaw S, Hammerton G, Mahedy L, et al.: Mental health resilience in the adolescent offspring of parents with depression: a prospective longitudinal study. Lancet Psychiatry 2016; 3:49–57Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32 Wolke D, Waylen A, Samara M, et al.: Selective drop-out in longitudinal studies and non-biased prediction of behaviour disorders. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195:249–256Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Martin J, Tilling K, Hubbard L, et al.: Association of genetic risk for schizophrenia with nonparticipation over time in a population-based cohort study. Am J Epidemiol 2016; 183:1149–1158Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34 Nivard MG, Lubke GH, Dolan CV, et al.: Joint developmental trajectories of internalizing and externalizing disorders between childhood and adolescence. Dev Psychopathol 2017; 29:919–928Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 Kendler KS: The schizophrenia polygenic risk score: to what does it predispose in adolescence? JAMA Psychiatry 2016; 73:193–194Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36 Eley TC, Stevenson J: Specific life events and chronic experiences differentially associated with depression and anxiety in young twins. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2000; 28:383–394Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Rutter M: Identifying the Environmental Causes of Disease: How Should We Decide What to Believe and When to Take Action? London, Academy of Medical Sciences, 2007Google Scholar

38 Thapar A, Rutter M: Using natural experiments and animal models to study causal hypotheses in relation to child mental health problems, in Rutter’s Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 6th ed. Edited by Thapar A, Pine DS, Leckman JF, et al.. Oxford, UK, John Wiley & Sons, 2015CrossrefGoogle Scholar