skip to main content
10.1145/1124772.1124908acmconferencesArticle/Chapter ViewAbstractPublication PageschiConference Proceedingsconference-collections
Article

The paradox of the assisted user: guidance can be counterproductive

Published:22 April 2006Publication History

ABSTRACT

This paper investigates the influence of interface styles on problem solving performance. It is often assumed that performance on problem solving tasks improves when users are assisted by externalizing task-related information on the interface. Although externalization requires less recall and relieves working memory, it does not instigate planning, understanding and knowledge acquisition. Without this assistance, task-information must be internalized, stored in the user's memory, leading to more planning and thinking and perhaps to better performance and knowledge. Another variable that can influence behavior is "Need for Cognition" (NFC), the tendency to engage in effortful cognitive tasks. We investigated the effects of interface style and cognitive style on performance using a conference planning application. Interface style influenced behavior and performance, but NFC did not. The internalization interface led to more planful behavior and smarter solutions. When planning and learning are the aim, designers should thus beware of giving a user (too) much assistance. Understanding how people react to interface information can be crucial in designing effective software, especially important in the areas of education and learning.

References

  1. Brusilovsky, P. Adaptive hypermedia. User Modeling and User-Adapted Interaction, 11 (2001), 87--110. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  2. Cacioppo, J.T., and Petty, R.E. The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42 (1982), 116--131.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  3. Cacioppo, J.T., Petty, R.E. and Kao, C.E. The efficient assessment of need for cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48 (1984), 306--307.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  4. Carrol, J.M. The Nurnberg Funnel: Designing Minimalist Instruction for Practical Computer Skill. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1990. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  5. Carroll, J.M. and Rosson, M.B. The paradox of the active user. In J.M. Carroll (Ed.), Interfacing Thought: Cognitive Aspects of Human-Computer Interaction. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, USA, 1987. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  6. Crystal, A. and Kalyanaraman, S. Personality variables and information processing on the web: Methodological issues and empirical evidence. Paper presented at the International Communication Association Annual Meeting (2005), 26--30.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  7. De Mul, S. and Van Oostendorp, H. Learning user interfaces by exploration. Acta Psychologica, 91 (1996), 325--344.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  8. European UNFOLD Project. http://www.unfold-project.netGoogle ScholarGoogle Scholar
  9. Fu, W. and Gray, W.D. Resolving the paradox of the active user: stable suboptimal performance in interactive tasks. Cognitive Science, 28 (2004), 901--935.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  10. Kort, B., Reilly, R. and Picard, R. External representation of learning process and domain knowledge: Affective state as a determinate of its structure and function. Paper presented at the 10th International Conference on Artificial Intelligence in Education, San Antonio, TX (2001).Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  11. Mayes, J. T., Draper, S.W., McGregor, A.M. and Oatley, K. Information flow in a user interface: the effect of experience and context on the recall of MacWrite screens. In Proc. of the 4th Conference of the British Computer Society on People and computers (1988), 275--289. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  12. Nielsen, J. Usability Engineering. Morgan Kaufmann, San Francisco, CA, USA, 1994. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  13. O'Hara, K.P. and Payne, S.J. The effects of operator implementation cost on planfulness of problem solving and learning. Cognitive Psychology, 35, 1 (1998), 34--70.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  14. O'Hara, K.P. and Payne, S.J. Planning and the user interface: The effects of lockout time and error recovery cost. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 50, 1 (1999), 41--59. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  15. Palmiter, S. and Elkerton, J. An evaluation of demonstrations for learning computer based tasks. In Proc. of the SIGCHI conference on Human factors in computing systems: Reaching through technology (1991), 257--263. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  16. Payne S.J., Howes, A. and Reader, W.R. Adaptively distributing cognition: A decision-making perspective on human-computer interaction. Behaviour & Information Technology, 20, 5 (2001), 339--346.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  17. Scaife, M. and Rogers, Y. External cognition: How do graphical representations work? International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 45, 2 (1996), 185--213. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  18. Svendsen, G.B. The influence of interface style on problem solving. International Journal for Man-Machine Studies, 35 (1991), 379--397. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  19. Tognazzini, B. Ten Most Wanted Design Bugs (2004). http://www.asktog.com/Bughouse/10MostWantedDesignBugs.htmlGoogle ScholarGoogle Scholar
  20. Van Nimwegen, C., Van Oostendorp, H. and Tabachneck-Schijf, H.J.M. Does context sensitivity in the interface help? In Proc. of the Twelfth European Conference on Cognitive Ergonomics, European Asociation of Cognitive Ergonomics (2004), 29--33.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  21. Van Nimwegen, C., Van Oostendorp, H. and Tabachneck-Schijf, H.J.M. The over-assisting interface: The role of support in knowledge acquisition. In Proc. of HCI2004: Design for Life, Research Press International (2004), 133--136. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  22. Van Nimwegen, C., Van Oostendorp, H. and Tabachneck-Schijf, H.J.M. The role of interface style in planning during problem solving. In Proc. The 27th Annual Cognitive Science Conference, Lawrence Erlbaum (2005), 2771--2776.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  23. Van Oostendorp, H. and De Mul, S. Learning by exploration: Thinking aloud while exploring an information system. Instructional Science, 27 (1999), 269--284.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  24. Zhang, J., and Norman, D. A. Representations in distributed cognitive tasks. Cognitive Science, 18 (1994), 87--122.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref

Index Terms

  1. The paradox of the assisted user: guidance can be counterproductive

    Recommendations

    Comments

    Login options

    Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

    Sign in
    • Published in

      cover image ACM Conferences
      CHI '06: Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems
      April 2006
      1353 pages
      ISBN:1595933727
      DOI:10.1145/1124772

      Copyright © 2006 ACM

      Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from [email protected]

      Publisher

      Association for Computing Machinery

      New York, NY, United States

      Publication History

      • Published: 22 April 2006

      Permissions

      Request permissions about this article.

      Request Permissions

      Check for updates

      Qualifiers

      • Article

      Acceptance Rates

      Overall Acceptance Rate6,199of26,314submissions,24%

    PDF Format

    View or Download as a PDF file.

    PDF

    eReader

    View online with eReader.

    eReader