Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics 1/2021

Open Access 01.12.2021 | Research article

Neurobehavioural and cognitive effects of prenatal exposure to organochlorine compounds in three year old children

verfasst von: Griet Vermeir, Adrian Covaci, Nik Van Larebeke, Greet Schoeters, Vera Nelen, Gudrun Koppen, Mineke Viaene

Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics | Ausgabe 1/2021

Abstract

Background

We report data of a Belgian observational prospective cohort study regarding cognitive and behavioural development until the age of 36 months in relation to internal exposure to organochlorine pollutants [sum of polychlorinated biphenyls (sum PCB), dioxin-like activity, PCB118, PCB170, hexachlorobenzene (HCB) and p,p’-dichlorodiphenyldichloroethylene (DDE)] measured in cord blood.

Methods

Participants were recruited as part of an Flemish Environmental Health Survey (2002–2006). Two hundred and six mother-child pairs were recruited. Hundred twenty five toddlers [Reynell Taal Ontwikkelings Schalen (language development, RTOS), Snijders-Oomen Niet-verbale intelligentietest (non-verbal intelligence, SON), Bayley Scales, milestones, Infant Behaviour Questionnaire (IBQ), gender specific play behaviour, Neurobehavioral Evaluation System (NES)-attentional task] and their mothers [Home Observation Measurement of the Environment (HOME), Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI), State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), general questionnaires] were tested. Statistical analysis was performed with the SPSS program. Much attention was paid to confounding factors.

Results

In the first years of development, higher organochlorine pollutants were associated with less active children (delayed crawling: sum PCB*HCB (p < 0.05), sumPCB*DDE (p < 0.1); delayed first steps alone: sum PCB (p < 0.5), PCB118 (p < 0.01), PCB170 (p < 0.01), HCB (p < 0.01); less switching between toys: sum PCB (p < 0.01); less switching between toys in boys: PCB118 (p < 0.01), sum PCB(p < 0.01)). At 12 months children with higher dioxin-like activity tended to show less fear responses(p < 0.1) (IBQ 12 months). At 36 months, a slower development of language comprehension (RTOS) was related to all organochlorine exposure parameters(p < 0.1 or p < 0.05) except DDE. Lower nonverbal IQ scores (SON) were related to PCB118 in boys only(p < 0.05 or p < 0.01). Less masculine and more non-gender specific play behaviour was associated with sum PCB in boys and girls at 36 months(p < 0.1). Moreover, PCB118 (p < 0.05), PCB170 (p < 0.1), HCB(p < 0.05) and DDE(p < 0.05) were associated with diminished masculine play behaviour in boys.

Conclusion

Our data confirm the observations that neurobehavioral development of young children is adversely influenced by environmental concentrations of PCBs, especially in boys. In this context, observation of play behaviour seems to be a reliable, easy to perform and sensitive test to detect neurotoxic effects of chemicals like PCB’s and dioxin-like compounds in very young children. On the basis of our results, we hypothesize that an underarrousal pattern may play a role in the spectrum of effects measured in toddlers prenatally exposed to PCBs and dioxin-like compounds.
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s12887-021-02533-2.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
BMI
Body Mass Index
CALUX-TEQ
Dioxin-like activity measured via the CALUX® assay
DDE
P,p’-dichlorodiphenyldichloroethylene
HCB
Hexachlorobenzene
HOME
Home Observation Measurement of the Environment
IBQ
Infant Behavior Questionnaire
IQ
Intelligence quotient
NES
Neurobehavioral Evaluation System
PCB
Polychlorinated biphenyls
POPs
Persistent organic pollutants
PS IQ
Performance Scale IQ (SON)
RS IQ
Reasoning Scale IQ (SON)
RTOS
Reynell TaalOntwikkelingsSchalen (language development)
SD
Standard Deviation
SON
Snijders-Oomen Niet-verbale intelligentietest (non-verbal intelligence)
SON-IQ
Total intelligence scale (SON)
STAI
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory
SumPCB
Sum of polychlorinated biphenyls
WASI
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence

Background

PCBs (polychlorinated biphenyls) are classified as POPs (persistent organic pollutants) due to their very stable chemical structures and consequently very long half lives in the environment and living species, including humans [1]. Therefore the handling of PCBs has been strictly regulated in many countries for quite some time. Nevertheless, they will persist in many surroundings for some more decades and will stay a potential health hazard. Human exposure to PCBs is largely due to ingestion of contaminated foods, especially milk, fish and meat products. PCBs can cross the placenta causing prenatal exposure in the developing foetus and accumulates in breast milk. Literature suggests that compared to exposures later in life, foetuses and children are much more susceptible to the neurotoxic effects of PCB exposure, especially prenatally and possibly also in the first years of life [2]. The organochlorine pesticides HCB and DDE also are classified as persistent organic pollutants. and endocrine disruptors [3, 4]. Also animal data [5] have shown neurotoxic effects of PCB exposures. The neurotoxicity of PCBs may rest on many different mechanisms, including disruption of thyroid function, altering several neurotransmitter signalling pathways e.g. serotonergic, glutaminergic, GABA-ergic (dopaminergic and γ-aminobutyric acid) [4, 68]. Also, interference with intracellular Ca2+ dynamics may lead to altered dendritic arborisation and synaptogenesis [9].
Play behaviour (masculine/feminine) has been reported to change in children according to the level of prenatal PCB exposure, although in the opposite direction for boys and girls [10]. Gender specific effects might also occur with respect to cognition as higher exposed boys did worse on constructive tasks in the Yucheng study [11]. PCB’s, and other endocrine disrupting substances can disrupt brain sexual differentiation and may affect brain development in a gender specific way [12, 13].
In this study, part of the Flemish Environmental Health Survey (2002–2006), we measured internal exposure in cord blood to PCBs, compounds with ‘dioxin-like’ activity, chlorinated pesticides [DDE and hexachlorobenzene (HCB) and we also assessed IQ, behaviour, temperament and milestones in motor development up to 36 months of age. Much attention was paid to confounding factors including, amongst others, assessment of the environmental support, personality traits and intelligence of the mother.
We hypothesized that known effects on development and sex differences in effect were likely to occur and especially that gender specific play behavioural patterns would be influenced by prenatal exposure levels of PCB’s and CALUX-TEQ.

Methods

Study design and study population (participants)

The study was set up as an observational prospective cohort study. Internal exposure of neonates was measured at birth using cord blood. At age 12, 24 and 36 months emotional and behavioural problems were assessed, and at 36 months a neurobehavioral evaluation of mother and child was performed.
Participants were recruited as part of an Flemish Environmental Health Survey (2002–2006) in Flanders. Between September 2002 and February 2004, mother-child pairs from the general population were recruited through 26 maternities which were selected by stratified sampling in eight study areas. The study areas included two urban areas (Antwerp and Ghent), an area characterized by fruit orchards, a rural area and four types of industrial areas (harbour, non-ferrous smelters, chemical industry and household waste incinerators). The selected areas contained 20% of the inhabitants of Flanders. Pregnant mothers could be included if they lived for at least five years in the area of interest, if they gave informed consent, and if they were able to fill out Dutch questionnaires. Participants in the neurobehavioral part of the study were recruited in the rural areas and in the areas around the harbours, the waste incinerators, and the areas near the non-ferrous smelters.
The study group was selected based on following inclusion criteria by the co-workers of the Provincial Institute of Hygiene -Antwerp: pregnancy without major complications (eclampsia, risk of imminent preterm birth), born at term, no major congenital abnormalities or diseases, no twins, no abnormal or asymmetrical reflexes during standard neurological screening during the first days and the children having only Dutch as their mother-tongue to prevent differences in language development. Informed consent was asked to participants whom were eligible in the sampled areas. At the end, 214 baby-mother pairs had given informed consent. Numbers were diminished because biomonitoring results could not be obtained in all participants (between N = 148 CALUX-TEQ and N = 206 DDE, see Table 2). One hundred and twenty nine pairs in which biomonitoring results were available, could be fully followed up until the age of 36 months (60,3%). The number of participants in each analysis may be lower as not all tests could be completed in all children due to various factors (e.g. tiredness of the child, timing issues, cooperation of the child, failure of test systems).

Blood sampling and measurements

PCBs, Calux-TEQ, DDE, HCB

Cord blood was aliquoted and plasma was separated by centrifugation within one day in either the maternity or blood bank laboratories. The aliquoted samples were kept in the refrigerator for maximal one week. Afterwards they were put at − 20 °C until analysis. In cord plasma, marker polychlorinated biphenyls (PCB 138, 153 and 180), PCB 118 and 170 which may have more neurotoxic potentials, chlorinated pesticides [DDE and hexachlorobenzene (HCB)], and dioxin-like compounds (CALUX® assay) were analysed. The PCBs and chlorinated pesticides were analysed by GC/ECD using the method of Gomara et al. [18]. The analyses were performed by two labs. Both laboratories participated in the AMAP proficiency testing scheme (Institute National de Santé Publique, Quebec, Canada). The measurement uncertainty (sum of systematic error and two times the reproducibility) was estimated from the results of the ClinChek and AMAP samples, and ranged between 21 and 34% for all the compounds except for HCB (64%). The limit of detection for all chlorinated compounds was 0.02 μg/L. Routinely measured cholesterol and triglycerides were used to express the results on a lipid weight basis [19]. Exposure to dioxin-like compounds was assessed via the CALUX® assay, based on in vitro activation of the aryl hydrocarbon receptor (AhR) of cultured H4IIE rat hepatoma cells by the dioxin-like compounds present in 5 mL cord plasma (BioDetection Systems BV, Amsterdam, The Netherlands). The extraction and clean-up procedures were performed as described by Koppen et al. [20]. The limit of detection was calculated as the light signal measured from the dimethyl sulfoxide-control plus 3 times its standard deviation on each well plate (= 16 pg CALUX-TEQ/g cord plasma fat).
Lead (Pb) and cadmium (Cd) were measured in cord blood using standard clinical biological techniques.

General history, medical file data and general health questionnaires

Before birth

Shortly after birth/delivery, data regarding the child, data on life style of the parents and information about the current and former pregnancies were obtained through a general self-assessment questionnaire. These data and questionnaires were collected by trained staff members of the Provincial Institute for Hygiene and Epidemiology (V. Nelen). Women were asked whether they had had any infections, hypertension, diabetes or other diseases and/or complications during pregnancy (0/1), whether they had used medication (0/1), alcohol (0/1) or drugs (0/1), or had been smoking (0/1) during pregnancy. Almost all mothers reported to have taken some vitamin supplementation throughout their pregnancy.

At birth

Data regarding mother and child were also collected in the medical files of the hospital: data regarding the mother were medical history of the mother, weeks of pregnancy, complications during birth (0/1), age of the mother, congenital diseases (0/1) and breastfeeding (0/1). All children included were born at term (> 36 weeks). Data regarding the child were weight, length, Apgar scores, standard medical and neurological examination as performed by a specialized paediatrician. Two children had an asymmetrical Babinski sign, which were excluded. Four children had minor birth defects (a supplementary finger, a small skin-patch, minor dysplasia of the outer ears, one foot with moderate valgus malformation), which were considered as without significance. One child suffered from hydronephrosis and was excluded. All the other clinical signs were normal in all babies. After screening the files one baby was going to be raised in a foreign language and was excluded. In total, 202 mother-children pairs met all the inclusion criteria. If the Apgar score at the beginning (1 min) was 6 or less and/or other signs of foetal asphyxia were present (meconium stained amniotic fluid, aspiration, abnormal foetal heart rate, neonatal resuscitation) the baby was considered to have had perinatal asphyxia (0/1).
The first month (4–6 weeks after delivery) all mothers received a supplementary questionnaire. This questionnaire repeated and extended the questions used in the general questionnaire at birth as an extra control on the data. Semi-quantitative ethanol consumption (g ethanol/week) was calculated.

After birth

In the first year the mothers filled out a monthly written questionnaire (by post) on child development, feeding, diseases of the child, imported life events, medication, smoking, alcohol or drug use of the mother. After the first year this was repeated every three months (by post) until at the age of 36 months the child was investigated. Additionally, important life events and stress factors were yearly questioned with the STAI (State-Trait Anxiety Inventory).

Neurobehavioral test methods and neurobehavioral questionnaires

An overview of the tests is shown in Tables 1, 2 and Fig. 1.
Table 1
Children: tests on behaviour, motor development, language skills, intelligence
Questionnaires
Parameter
Positive β indicates more or better
Age of child
Bayley mental scale [21]
Bayley motor scale [21]
Visual and auditory discernment, eye-hand coordination, imitation, language development, memory and problem solving capacity. Mean score for the general population of 100 (± SD 15).
Motor development. Mean score for the general population of 100 (± SD 15).
Cognitive development
Motor development
36 months
Infant Behaviour Questionnaire (IBQ) [22]: Temperament as observed by the mother
Scale 1 = stress and latency to approach sudden or novel stimuli
Scale 2 = smiling and laughter
Scale 3 = distress to limitations
Scale 4 = activity level
Scale 5 = duration of orienting
Fear responses.
Smiling or laughter from the child in general caretaking and play situations.
Persistence and goal oriented behaviour.
Gross motor activity: movement of arms and legs, squirming and locomotor activity.
Habituation: more attention to and/or interaction with a single object for extended periods of time.
12 months
Milestones: developmental abilities
1 = grasping
2 = rolling over
3 = sitting up
4 = crawling
5 = first steps alone
Time needed to achieve the milestone:
Grasping and picking up objects.
Learning to flip over from his back to his tummy and vice versa.
To sit up without support.
Crawling.
First steps alone unaided (first steps alone).
0–36 months
Tests
Measurement type
Positive β indicates a higher capacity in terms of
Age of child
Reynell Language Developmental Scale (RTOS): receptive language development level measured by oral instructions to carry out small tasks while playing with toys [23].
Verbal comprehension scale
Receptive language development.
36 months
Snijders-Oomen non-verbal intelligence test (SON 2.5–7), [24]: Intelligence: reasoning and visuospatial abilities
Three intelligence quotients (IQ) can be calculated with this intelligence tests with a mean score for the general population of 100 (± SD 15)
Total intelligence scale: SON-IQ
Performance Scale (PS) IQ
Reasoning Scale (RS) IQ
(Nonverbal) intelligence (IQ).
Performance IQ: better in making puzzles, mosaics and patterns.
Reasoning IQ: better in categories, situations and analogies.
36 months
Observation of toy preference: 7 min. Lasting observation of masculine/feminine play behaviour [25, 26]
Masculine play behaviour
Feminine play behaviour
Non-gender specific play behaviour
Switching attention
Time playing with masculine toys: little cars, fire truck, construction blocks, toy gun.
Time playing with feminine toys: little dolls, hair doll, baby doll, kitchen accessories.
Time playing with non-gender specific toys: puzzle, book
The number of times the child switches between toys.
36 months
NES 3 (Neurobehavioral Evaluation System) [27]
Continuous Performance Test
Computerised vigilance task adapted according to Patandin et al., [28].
36 months
Table 2
Mothers: Tests concerning caring environment, intelligence, anxiety
Tests
Measurement type
Time of assessment
HOME [14, 15]
Home observation for measurement in the environment
36 months
WASI [16]
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence
36 months
STAI [17]
Trait and state anxiety measurement
12 months
At the time the child reached the age of 12, 24 and 36 months, the mother and pre-school playgroup teacher completed questionnaires regarding possible emotional and behavioural problems [Infant Behaviour Questionnaire (IBQ) [22]. The response rate of the IBQ at 24 months was too low to include the results of the analyses (< 40%).
At the time the child reached the age of 36 months, an appointment was made to carry out the neurobehavioral evaluation of mother and child and the HOME assessment (HOME, 1988).
All psychological tests and questionnaires used in this study are validated in Flanders for clinical use and are suitable for use in a research context. The NES (Neurobehavioral Evaluation System; [27]) was adapted according to the attentional task described by Patandin et al. [28] (catch the kitten). The tests were administered by trained test leaders with substantial experience in psychological testing of children and adults. The tests were divided over two test sessions and took place in a quiet room.
The Bayley Scales measure the mental and motor development and test behaviour of infants from one to forty-two months of age. The scales have been used extensively worldwide to assess the development of infants. The examiner presents a series of test materials to the child and observes the child’s responses and behaviours. The test contains items designed to identify young children at risk for developmental delay. In this study, mental en motor scales were assessed according to Van der Meulen et al. [21]. In the Mental scale, several types of abilities are evaluated: sensory/perceptual acuities, discriminations, and response; acquisition of object constancy; memory learning and problem solving; vocalization and beginning of verbal communication; basis of abstract thinking; habituation; mental mapping; complex language; and mathematical concept formation. In the Motor scale, the degree of body control, large muscle coordination, fine motor skills of the hands and fingers, dynamic movement, postural imitation, and the ability to recognize objects by sense of touch (stereo gnosis) were assessed.
Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) is a descriptive profile which yields a systematic assessment of the caring environment in which the child is reared. The primary goal of the instrument is to measure, within a naturalistic context, the quality and quantity of stimulation and support available to a child in the home environment. Its focus is on the experience of the child in the home environment, the child as an active recipient of inputs from objects, events and transactions occurring in connection with the family surroundings [14, 15].
The Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI, [16]) has been used in this study to obtain a reliable measure of intellectual ability of the mother. The WASI assessment provides traditional verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores.
A nominal STAI score was computed as a marker of prenatal stress [17]. The trait score of the STAI scale has proven to be stable across years, whereas the state score is more vulnerable to acute mood changes. Therefore a 0/1 variable was created, based on the P90 of STAI trait score (measured at 12 months).

Statistical analysis

The relation between neurobehavioral development, including play behaviour, and the prenatal exposure levels of PCB and dioxin-like compounds, DDE, HCB and interaction effects between the prenatal exposure levels of sum PCB and DDE or HCB were analysed in the total group and in boys and girls separately.
Database management and statistical analyses were performed with the SPSS program.
With ANCOVA analysis the difference between the different test-leaders (scoring and testing) was checked, using the same covariates as for the stepwise regression analysis (see below). No significant differences were found (p > 0,050) (multiple linear regression).
Correlation analysis of the covariates in our study group, showed that the highest education level of the parents was very highly correlated with the total IQ of the mother (r2 = .52; p < .001) and parity was highly correlated with the HOME score (r2 = .34; p < .05). Highest education level and parity were therefore not included in the regression analyses.
Stepwise regression was performed in the total group using the cord blood concentrations of the sum of the marker polychlorinated biphenyls PCBs 138, 153 and 180 (sum PCB), dioxin like substances measured with the Calux test, DDE and HCB as biomarkers of exposure. As covariates for the neurobehavioral test data and questionnaire results, we used total IQ of the mother (WASI), gender, mother’s age at birth, smoking during pregnancy, alcohol use during pregnancy, neonatal asphyxia, medication use during pregnancy, infections or other interfering diseases during pregnancy, breastfeeding, STAI (trait, as a marker of prenatal stress), and total score of the HOME. Total IQ of the child (SON) was also taken into account for the stepwise multiple regression analyses of the questionnaire results. Stepwise regression analysis concerning gender-specific behaviour was done in the total group and in boys and girls separately. Gender specific results are only presented when statistically significant.
As group numbers differed over different tests, the number of subject is mentioned in every analysis.
In order to look at possible interaction and/or addition effects, the test and scale scores were transformed into standardised (z) scores. All potential factors were brought into the model (stepwise Multiple Linear Regression).

Results

Personal characteristics and measured parameters of participants

The personal characteristics and the data concerning measured parameters for all of the participants are summarized in Tables 3 and 4. No significant differences were noted in organochlorine cord blood concentrations between boys and girls (see supplementary material Table 1). No correlation was found between the organochlorines and the other parameters mentioned in Tables 3 and 4, except for a negative correlation of Pb with HCB (r2 = − 0,146, p = 0,045), and Pb with Calux-TEQ (r2 = − 0,191, p = 0,025).
Table 3
Characteristics and exposure of partcipants
Total group
N = 214
 
N
Mean
Minimum
Maximum
Std. Deviation
HOME
127
47,7
28
55
4,3
Parity
206
1,6
1,0
4,0
0,8
Total IQ mother (WASI)
129
103,9
75
136
12,1
Highest education level of both parents
206
4,3
0
6
1,3
Age of mother at birth
206
29,5
20,3
41,6
3,9
BMI child at birth
206
13,6
9,8
20,6
1,4
SON, Total IQ-scale
125
113,7
70
150
13,6
Total STAI
169
0,1
0
1
0,4
Sum PCB ng/g total lipid *
198
87,9
2,8
305,6
59,6
PCB 118 ng /g total lipid
198
14,9
0
71,1
11,3
PCB 170 ng/g total lipid
197
8,9
0
55,6
7,4
Calux-TEQ pg/g total lipid
148
30,1
5,1
130,2
20,5
DDE ng/g total lipid
206
198,2
8,2
1274,5
193,5
HCB ng/g total lipid
197
27,8
2,4
132,1
20,4
Lead μg/L
201
19.2
1.0
87.3
15.8
Cadmium μg/L
201
0.7
0.1
13.9
1.1
HOME, WASI, SON total IQ-scale, Total STAI: see overview tests in Tables 1 and 2
Sum PCB, PCB 118, PCB 170, Calux-TEQ, DDE-conc., HCB-conc.: see “Blood sampling and measurements”
*Sum PCB = PCB 138+ PCB 153+ PCB 180
Table 4
Gender, events during pregnancy and at birth (N = 214)
 
Number (%)
Missing data
Number (%)
Boys
107 (50,0%)
0
Breastfeeding (0/1)
70 / 130 (33,0% / 61,3%)
12 (5,7%)
Smoked during pregnancy (0/1)
185/25 (87,3% / 11,8%)
2 (0,9%)
Alcohol use during pregnancy (0/1)
197 / 15 (92% / 7,1%)
2 (0,9%)
Asphyxia baby (0/1)
189 / 23 (89,2% / 10,8%)
0

Description of neurobehavioral characteristics

Observed neurobehavioral characteristics are summarized in Table 5.
Table 5
Descriptive results: behaviour, motor development, language skills, intelligence
 
N
Mean
Median
Std. Deviation
Minimum
Maximum
Valid
Missing
Bayley Scales of infant Development II NL: Mental Scale Index score
112
102
109,51
108,00
15,14
69
163
Bayley Scales of infant Development II NL: Motor Scale index score
93
121
101,97
101,00
12,78
79
137
Snijders-Oomen Non-Verbal Intelligence test: Performance Scale
125
89
108,66
110,00
15,92
67
148
Snijders-Oomen Non-Verbal Intelligence test: Reasoning Scale
125
89
115,13
115,00
12,50
66
150
Snijders-Oomen Non-Verbal Intelligence test: Total Scale
125
89
113,72
114,00
13,59
70
150
Reynell Language Development Scale: Language Development Total score (language comprehension)
101
113
102,80
104,00
17,82
59
142
milestones: smiling (months)
176
38
1,44
1,00
1,01
1
9
milestones: reach for and take a toy (months)
180
34
2,91
3,00
1,19
1
8
milestones: rolling over (months)
156
58
5,30
5,00
1,66
1
10
milestones: sit up straight (months)
154
60
7,29
7,00
1,44
4
15
milestones: crawling (months)
130
84
9,25
9,00
2,15
4
21
Milestones: first steps alone
69
145
13,06
14,00
2,57
8
20
IBQ 12 mths: Stress and Latency to Approach Sudden or Novel Stimuli
151
63
2,81
2,81
,81
1,20
4,58
IBQ 12 mths: Smiling and Laughter
151
63
5,41
5,47
,74
3,36
7,00
IBQ 12 mths: Distress to Limitations
151
63
3,04
3,00
,70,412
1,33
5,05
IBQ 12 mths: Activity Level
151
63
3,32
3,29
,88
1,24
5,47
IBQ 12 mths: Duration of Orienting
150
64
3,41
3,41
,947
1,36
6,29
Masculine play behaviour (%)
100
114
43,68
37,67
31,08
0,00
100,00
Feminine play behaviour (%)
100
114
41,51
36,99
30,39
0,00
100,00
Non-gender specific play behaviour (%)
100
114
14,81
9,61
18,31
0,00
94,00
There was no significant difference in any of the neurobehavioral outcome variables regarding the different regions (p > 0,050) (multiple linear regression).

Associations with neurobehavioral characteristics using questionnaires

Associations with neurobehavioral characteristics measured using questionnaires are summarized in Table 6.
Table 6
Associations between exposure and behaviour assessed through IBQ (beta coefficients)
IBQ
12 months
 
N
scale 1
(stress)
scale 2
(smiling and laughter)
scale 3
(distress to limitations)
scale 4
(activity)
scale 5
(duration of orienting)
Sum PCB ng/g lipid
T = 96
−.081
−.164
.014
.047
−0.125
Girls
♀ = 44
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
♂ = 52
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
Calux-TEQ pg/g lipid
T = 68
−.247*
.117
.048
.119
.085
Girls
♀ = 37
NS
NS
NS
NS
0.468**
Boys
♂ = 31
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
PCB-118 ng/g lipid
T = 95
−.092
.007
−.201*
−.131
−.061
Girls
♀ = 51
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
♂ = 44
NS
NS
--0.379*
NS
NS
PCB-170 ng/g lipid
T = 95
.007
.187
−.103
−.054
−.083
Girls
♀ = 51
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
♂ = 44
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
HCB ng/g lipid
T = 93
−.150
.193*
−.022
.045
.182
Girls
♀ = 50
NS
NS
NS
NS
0.343*
Boys
♂ = 43
NS
0.386*
NS
NS
NS
DDE ng/g lipid
T = 100
−.201*
.071
−.147
.103
−.158
Girls
♀ = 54
−0.299*
NS
NS
NS
−0.308*
Boys
♂ = 46
NS
0.347*
NS
NS
NS
Sum PCB*HCB
T = 93
−.079
−.046
−.048
−.034
.031
Girls
♀ = 50
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
♂ = 43
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
Sum PCB*DDE
T = 96
.026
.078
.054
.159
.078
Girls
♀ = 52
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
♂ = 44
NS
0.376*
NS
NS
NS
IBQ and IBQ scales: see overview tests in Table 1
PCBs, Calux-TEQ, DDE, HCB: see blood sampling and measurements
Su PCB*HCB, sum PCB*DDE: see statistical analysis
* = 0.050 ≤ p < 0.10, ** = 0.010 ≤ p < 0.050, *** = 0.001 ≤ p < 0.010
In boys and girls together higher CALUX-TEQ and DDE values were associated with a decrease in stress and latency to approach sudden or novel stimuli (scale1), HCB was associated with an increase in smiling and laughter (scale 2) and PCB-118 was associated with a decrease in distress to limitations (scale 3).
In girls, DDE was associated with a decrease in stress and latency to approach sudden or novel stimuli (scale 1) and a decrease duration of orienting (scale 5), and CALUX-TEQ and HCB were, contrary to DDE, associated with an increase in duration of orienting (scale 5).
In boys, higher internal exposure levels to HCB, DDE and the factor sum PCB*DDE were associated with an increase in smiling and laughter (scale 2), and PCB 118 was associated with an increase in distress to limitations. Interestingly, different significant associations between internal exposure to organochlorine compounds and temperament as observed by the mother were observed for boys and girls.

Associations with differences in milestones

Associations with differences in milestones are summarized in Table 7.
Table 7
Associations between exposure and motor development in terms of milestones (beta coefficients)
Milestonesss
Total Group
Grasping
T = 109
♀ = 56
♂ = 53
rolling over
T = 98
♀ = 50
♂ = 48
sitting up
T = 99
♀ = 50
♂ = 49
Crawling
T = 46
♀ = 28
♂ = 18
First steps alone
T = 46
♀ = 28
♂ = 18
Sum PCB ng/g lipid
−.178
.020
.212
.230
.338**
Girls
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
−.371*
−.427*
NS
NS
NS
Calux-TEQ pg/g lipid
.043
−.015
.015
.145
.078
Girls
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
PCB-118 ng/g lipid
.013
−.090
−.064
−.016
.451***
Girls
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
NS
NS
NS
NS
.577**
PCB-170 ng/g lipid
−.081
−.043
.049
.193
.366**
Girls
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
NS
NS
NS
NS
.378*
HCB ng/g lipid
.049
.039
.049
.175
.330**
Girls
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
NS
NS
NS
NS
.450*
DDE ng/g lipid
.082
.019
−.260***
.028
.119
Girls
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
NS
NS
−.296*
NS
.642**
Sum PCB*HCB
−.065
−.101
−.032
.278**
.463***
Girls
NS
NS
NS
.327*
NS
Boys
NS
NS
NS
NS
.538**
Sum PCB*DDE
−.183
−.077
−.163
.279*
.343**
Girls
−.312*
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
NS
NS
NS
NS
.630**
Milestones: see overview tests in Table 1
PCBs, Calux-TEQ, DDE, HCB: see blood sampling and measurements
Sum PCB*HCB, sum PCB*DDE: see statistical analysis
* = 0.050 ≤ p < 0.10, ** = 0.010 ≤ p < 0.050, *** = 0.001 ≤ p < 0.010
In the first 36 months (girls and boys together), increasing levels of sum PCB as well as increasing levels of individual PCBs (PCB118, PCB 170) and of HCB were associated with a significant later onset of first steps alone. The interaction between PCBs and pesticides was not only associated with a delay in the first steps, but was also associated with a delay in the age of crawling. In boys only, earlier grasping and earlier rolling over tended to be associated with sum PCB (Table 7). In girls sum PCB*DDE tended to be associated with earlier grasping. CALUX-TEQ was not associated with a negative effect on milestones in the total group. Higher DDE concentrations were significantly associated with delayed first steps alone in boys. Both in boys and in the total group a significant association was found between higher prenatal DDE concentrations and ability to sit up earlier.
Effects seem to be more pronounced in boys than in girls, except that in girls delayed crawling showed a positive association with the interaction of sum PCB*HCB and that the ability to grasp showed a positive association with higher sum PCB*DDE.

Cognitive tests and behavioural observations

Associations with differences in cognitive tests are summarized in Table 8.
Table 8
Associations between exposure and cognitive functioning (beta coefficients)
Cognitive tests
     
 
N
RTOS
(language development)
T = 97
♀ = 50
♂ = 47
SON-RS IQ
(Reasoning)
T = 125
♀ = 62
♂ = 63
SON-PS IQ
(performance)
T = 125
♀ = 62
♂ = 63
SON-TIQ
(total intelligence)
T = 125
♀ = 62
♂ = 63
Sum PCB ng/g lipid
99
−.103**
−.036
−.054
−.043
Girls
 
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
 
NS
NS
NS
NS
Calux-TEQ pg/g lipid
60
−.267**
−.002
−.042
−.010
Girls
 
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
 
NS
NS
NS
NS
PCB-118 ng/g lipid
99
−.125*
NS
NS
NS
Girls
 
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
 
NS
−.353**
−.401***
−.424***
PCB-170 ng/g lipid
98
−.243**
−.050
−.107
−.083
Girls
 
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
 
NS
NS
NS
NS
HCB ng/g lipid
98
−.262**
−.212
−.055
−.147
Girls
 
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
 
NS
NS
NS
NS
DDE ng/g lipid
79
−.028
.052
−.009
.026
Girls
 
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
 
NS
NS
NS
NS
Sum PCB*HCB
97
−.327***
−.168
−.072
−.121
Girls
 
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
 
NS
NS
NS
NS
Sum PCB*DDE
79
−.245**
−.048
.045
−.010
Girls
 
NS
NS
NS
NS
Boys
 
NS
NS
NS
NS
RTOS, SON-RS IQ, SON-PS IQ, SON-TIQ: see overview tests in Table 1
PCBs, Calux-TEQ, DDE, HCB: see blood sampling and measurements
Sum PCB*HCB, sum PCB*DDE: see statistical analysis
* = 0.050 ≤ p < 0.10, ** = 0.010 ≤ p < 0.050, *** = 0.001 ≤ p < 0.010
At 36 months, increasing levels of sum PCB as well as increasing levels of individual PCBs (PCB118, PCB 170) and CALUX-TEQ were associated with significant lower performance on language development (RTOS). Compared to the lowest sum PCB exposed children, the highest exposed children had RTOS scores that were 38% lower. Increasing concentrations of HCB and higher values for the interaction between PCB and pesticides were associated with the same effect. One of the neurotoxic compounds, PCB118, was associated with a significant deterioration, in boys only, of reasoning (SON-RS IQ), performance (SON-PS IQ) and total IQ (SON-TIQ). The interaction factor sum PCB*HCB may have an additive effect. DDE does not seem to be associated with an adverse effect, except in interaction with sum PCB (Table 8), possibly suggesting a synergistic effect.
The NES results were not analysed (see discussion).

Associations with observed differences in play behaviour

Associations with observed differences in behaviour are summarized in Table 9.
Table 9
Associations between exposure and play behaviour (beta coefficients)
  
Girls
   
Boys
   
 
girls/boys
N
masculine
play behaviour
feminine play behavior
non-gender specific play behaviour
switching attention
masculine
play behaviour
feminine play behaviour
non-gender specific play behaviour
switching attention
Sum PCB ng/g lipid
47/37
−.247*
−.121
.250*
−.342**
−.269*
−.002
.266*
−.383**
Calux-TEQ pg/g lipid
47/37
−.010
−.162
.313*
.126
−.199
.257
.014
−.244
PCB-118 ng/g lipid
45/39
−.313**
.202
.052
.273
−.326**
−.087
.361**
−.325**
PCB-170 ng/g lipid
47/37
−.031
−.181
.314**
−.035
−.286*
−.096
.278*
.091
HCB ng/g lipid
51/43
−.066
.114
−.100
−.091
−.374**
−.027
.363**
−.163
DDE ng/g lipid
53/43
.307
−.248
−.047
−.041
−.331**
.228
.145
−.006
Sum PCB*HCB
47/37
.066
−.101
.075
.081
−.079
.056
.051
−.106
Sum PCB*DDE
47/37
.103
−.121
.054
−.031
.146
−.034
−.166
.161
Play behaviour: see overview tests in Table 1
PCBs, Calux-TEQ, DDE, HCB: see blood sampling and measurements
Sum PCB*HCB, sum PCB*DDE: see statistical analysis
* = 0.050 ≤ p < 0.10, ** = 0.010 ≤ p < 0.050, *** = 0.001 ≤ p < 0.010
In the toy preference task at 36 months, several significant associations were observed for as well girls as boys. Higher sum PCB exposed children alternate less between toys. They play more with a puzzle or book (non-gender specific play behaviour) compared to children with lower prenatal sum PCB-levels and they display less masculine play behaviour. An increase in concentration of sum PCB of 58,82 ng/g lipids is associated with one time less switching between toys during 7 min. Considering the observed range of sum PCB (2,81 till 305,56 ng/g lipids) the highest exposed child switches toys almost once per minute less compared to the lowest exposed child. Switching in this case means also crawling, first steps alone and turning, as toys were displayed in a semi-circle. PCB 118 exposure was associated with a decrease in masculine play behaviour in as well girls as boys. PCB 17 exposure was associated with an increase in non-gender specific play behaviour in as well girls as boys.
As to significant association observed only in girls, CALUX-TEQ exposure was associated with an increase in non-gender specific play behaviour.
As to significant associations observed only in boys, PCB118 exposure was associated with an increase in non-gender specific play behaviour, and a decrease in switching between toys. PCB 170 exposure was associated with a decrease in masculine play behaviour. HCB exposure was associated with a decrease in masculine play behaviour and with an increase in non-gender specific play behaviour. DDE exposure was associated with a decrease in masculine play behaviour.
As to the effect of siblings on the play behaviour of the tested children, we noted that 13 of the boys had a sister, and 12 of the girls had a brother. As can be seen from supplementary material Table 3, having a sister did not weaken the associations between internal exposure and play behaviour in boys. Neither did having a brother weaken the associations between internal exposure and play behaviour in girls as shown in supplementary material Table 4.
As can be seen from Table 9, observed associations were more pronounced in boys than in girls. In boys, sum PCB exposure was associated with a more pronounced diminishment in switching behaviour compared to boys and girls considered together. An increase of 37,04 ng/g lipids in the sum PCB levels is associated with 1 switch between toys less per 7 min, 64% less playing time with boys-specific toys and 62% more with non-gender specific toys.

Discussion

Limitations of this study include the fact that the neurological examination at the time of birth, performed by a specialized pediatrician, was not performed by certified researchers. Also, although the children were examined at 36 months by specialized clinical psychologists, no complete medical assessment nor school results were available at the end of the study. As the Flemish Agency for Mental Health involved in the Study closed down many years ago and that many documents and computer files got lost, some details of the statistical models used are not available any more.
In this older cohort (2002–2004), representative for the Dutch speaking Flemish population, attention was paid to controlling for confounding factors, although not all confounding factors could be taken into consideration (e.g. the possible influence of air pollution on brain function) [29]. Analyses did not indicate bias by co-exposure of Pb and Cd, which were measured as ubiquitous neurotoxic environmental polluting metals in cord blood .
Comparing the exposure levels in our study with other studies is not straightforward because different parameters are measured in the different studies, and because different ratios were observed between maternal serum concentrations and cord serum concentrations. We made a tentative comparison summarized in supplementary material Table 2, which indicates that the internal exposure to PCBs in our study is situated in a middle range of internal exposures measured in other studies on general populations [28, 3042], and is more than 20 times lower than in the Yucheng incident in Taiwan [42].
The results of all neurobehavioral tests and questionnaires are described extensively and are consistent with neurobehavioral effects reported before (2, review). Compared to the lowest exposed children, the highest exposed children had language comprehension scores (RTOS) that were 38% lower. One of the neurotoxic compounds, PCB118, was associated with a significant deterioration, in boys only, of reasoning (SON-RS IQ), performance (SON-PS IQ) and total IQ (SON-TIQ.
Our results resemble those reported by Rogan et al. [34], Boucher et al. [43], Berghuis et al. [31] in which it was shown that first milestones may be reached earlier (grasping, rolling over) but later milestones (first steps alone) may be delayed with higher prenatal exposures to PCBs and dioxin-like compounds. These both effects seem to be more pronounced in boys compared to girls (sum PCB, sum PCB*HCB, sum PCB*DDE, DDE, PCB118, PCB170).
The milestones like crawling and first steps alone are markers of motor development requiring balance and coordination, but also requiring active behaviour. Active motor behaviour is one of the six elements of temperament [44]. We measured temperament at 12 months of age as a basic characteristic of behaviour by the IBQ. Temperament is described to be influenced by heredity, maturation and experience [45, 46] but may also be influenced by prenatal toxic factors e.g. parental smoking [47]. Effects on temperament related to PCBs or dioxin-like compounds have not been frequently reported in toddlers [2]. Our results reported in Table 6. suggest that prenatal exposure to these endocrine disruptors might result in less distressed children. This could result in gradually less reactive children to stimuli and possibly underarousal patterns in higher exposed children [45]. Our results seem to be similar to those of Jacobson et al. [48], showing a rather reduced activity in 4-year old children associated with higher PCB levels.
We observed a less masculine, more non-gender specific, less active play behaviour with less switching between toys in association with PCB exposure, which was more pronounced in boys. An underarousal pattern may explain this observed reduced play activity as well as the observations made in terms of the milestones and IBQ.
Vreugdenhil et al. [10] also found a less masculinised play behaviour, although it was not statistically significant, and although in girls postnatal PCB exposure through breastmilk was positively related to more masculine play behaviour. Vreugdenhil et al. [10] used the PSAI questionnaire to measure sex linked play behaviour, which has some drawbacks [49]. In contrast to the PSAI, our experience with the Observation of Toy Preference is that this test is an easy to applicate, easy to interpret, parent independent measurement of gender-specific behaviour, with a high (0.99) inter-observer reliability.
Patandin et al. [38] described a play behavioural test with high and low level play episodes. High level play, number of episodes of non-play behaviour and period of time at non-play behaviour were significantly diminished in association with prenatal exposure to PCBs. Although the measurements are not totally comparable, they both measure adequate play behaviour which seems to be diminished in our and Patandin’s group. If this is a result of attentional deficit or due to reduced activity is a matter of debate [28, 48]. Patandin [28] did not found a relation between cord blood PCB concentrations and most attentional parameters (mean reaction time, slope of reaction time, Groninger Behaviour Observation Scale parents questionnaire) in 42 months old children, but measuring attention may not be valid under 4.5 years [50]. We tried to measure attention in 36 month old children by a test described by Jacobson et al. [51] and used by Patandin [28] and Stewart [52]. The frame of the continuous performance test of the NES3 was used to develop a test which might be feasible for 3 year old children. We noticed that most children didn’t complete the test (not fully validated in children of this age), probably because sustained attention cannot be reliably measured at this age (results not reported). However, diminished attention related to perinatal PCB exposures are frequently reported in older children (> 4 years) [2].
In contrast to others, we didn’t find any influence of breastfeeding [5355].
Sex-linked differences as reported here, in other human studies [10, 11, 5658] and in animal studies [5962] might result from oestrogenic and anti-oestrogenic actions [4, 63, 64] as oestrogens are essential in the neurogenesis and functionality of neurons which are important in learning processes and behaviour such as anxious and gender-specific behaviour [5, 65]. In mammalians, the ontogenetic evolution towards the female phenotype appears in some way to be the default process and occurs to a large extent without sex hormone influence [12], while even small differences in levels and timing of exposure to hormones or endocrine disruptors may disrupt brain sexual differentiation [12]. The group of Andrea Gore showed in rats that PCBs cause changes in sexually-dimorphic social interactions and communications and that these changes were associated with (and probably rested on) modifications of gene expression in the medial preoptic nucleus, part of the social decision-making network in the brain. In females PCBs changed expression of several steroid hormone receptor and neuropeptide genes, whereas in males only one gene (the circadian gene Per2) was affected, although more behavioral changes were observed in males [66]. Even adult males and females do not necessarily react in the same way to exogenous substances. In the Flemish biomonitoring, changes in gene expression associated with internal exposure to DDE, hexachlorobenzene, marker PCBs and dioxin-like activity were predominantly in opposite direction for men and women [67].

Conclusion

Our data confirm the observations that neurobehavioral development of young children is adversely influenced by environmental concentrations of organochlorine pollutants, especially in boys. Motor development, playing activity and cognitive development tend to be delayed or diminished. We found observation of play behaviour to be a reliable, easy to perform and sensitive test to detect neurotoxic effects of chemicals like PCB’s and dioxin-like compounds in very young children. On the basis of our results, we hypothesize that an underarousal pattern may play a role in the spectrum of effects measured in toddlers prenatally exposed to PCBs and dioxin-like compounds.

Acknowledgements

This study was commissioned and financed by the Ministry of the Flemish Community (Department of Economics, Science and Innovation; Flemish Agency for Care and Health; and Department of Environment, Nature and Energy). A report containing the methods and the results for part of the subjects has been submitted to the Flemish Government that funded the research. This work has not been previously submitted elsewhere. We wish to thank for their competent collaboration Rita Verachtert, Mieke Thijs, Jan Laenen and Natalie Verelst from the Flemish Agency for Mental Health, Prof. B. Van Den Bergh, Ine Nijs, Lien Van Laer and Veerle Nullens from the Faculty of Psychology, KU Leuven, Prof. F. Comhaire of the department of Endocrinology of Ghent University, the field workers of the Provinciaal Institut voor Hygiëne (Antwerp) and Liesbeth Bruckers from the Interuniversity Institute for Biostatistics and statistical Bioinformatics, Universiteit Hasselt.
The study protocol was approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of Antwerp on July 3, 2002.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Nawrot TS, Staessen JA, Den Hond EM, Koppen G, Schoeters G, Fagard R, Thijs L, Winneke G, Roel HA. Host and environmental determinants of polychlorinated aromatic hydrocarbons in serum of adolescents. Enviro Health Perspect. 2002;110:583–9.CrossRef Nawrot TS, Staessen JA, Den Hond EM, Koppen G, Schoeters G, Fagard R, Thijs L, Winneke G, Roel HA. Host and environmental determinants of polychlorinated aromatic hydrocarbons in serum of adolescents. Enviro Health Perspect. 2002;110:583–9.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Berghuis SA, Roze E. Prenatal exposure to PCBs and neurological and sexual/pubertal development from birth to adolescence. Curr Probl Pediatr Adolesc Health Care. 2019;49:133–59.PubMedCrossRef Berghuis SA, Roze E. Prenatal exposure to PCBs and neurological and sexual/pubertal development from birth to adolescence. Curr Probl Pediatr Adolesc Health Care. 2019;49:133–59.PubMedCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Birgelen AP. Hexachlorobenzene as a possible major contributor to the dioxin activity of human milk. Environ Health Perspect. 1998;106:683–8.PubMedPubMedCentral Van Birgelen AP. Hexachlorobenzene as a possible major contributor to the dioxin activity of human milk. Environ Health Perspect. 1998;106:683–8.PubMedPubMedCentral
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Faroon O, Jones D, de Rosa C. Effects of polychlorinated biphenyls on the nervous system. Toxicol Ind Health. 2000;16:305–33.PubMedCrossRef Faroon O, Jones D, de Rosa C. Effects of polychlorinated biphenyls on the nervous system. Toxicol Ind Health. 2000;16:305–33.PubMedCrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Boix J, Cauli O. Alteration of serotonin system by polychlorinated biphenyls exposure. Neurochem Int. 2012;60:809–16.PubMedCrossRef Boix J, Cauli O. Alteration of serotonin system by polychlorinated biphenyls exposure. Neurochem Int. 2012;60:809–16.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Kodavanti PR, Tilson HA. Neurochemical effects of environmental chemicals: in vitro and in vivo correlations on second messenger pathways. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2000;919:97–105.PubMedCrossRef Kodavanti PR, Tilson HA. Neurochemical effects of environmental chemicals: in vitro and in vivo correlations on second messenger pathways. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2000;919:97–105.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Maervoet J, Vermeir G, Covaci A, Van Larebeke N, Koppen G, Schoeters G, Nelen V, Baeyens W, Schepens P, Viaene MK. Association of thyroid hormone concentrations with levels of organochlorine compounds in cord blood of neonates. Environ Health Perspect. 2007;115:1780–6.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Maervoet J, Vermeir G, Covaci A, Van Larebeke N, Koppen G, Schoeters G, Nelen V, Baeyens W, Schepens P, Viaene MK. Association of thyroid hormone concentrations with levels of organochlorine compounds in cord blood of neonates. Environ Health Perspect. 2007;115:1780–6.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Klocke C, Sethi S, Lein PJ. The developmental neurotoxicity of legacy vs. contemporary polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs): similarities and differences. Environ Sci Pollut Res. 2020;27:8885–96.CrossRef Klocke C, Sethi S, Lein PJ. The developmental neurotoxicity of legacy vs. contemporary polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs): similarities and differences. Environ Sci Pollut Res. 2020;27:8885–96.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Vreugdenhil HJ, Slijper FM, Mulder PG, Weisglas-Kuperus N. Effects of perinatal exposure to PCBs and dioxins on play behaviour in Dutch children at school age. Environ Health Perspect. 2002;110:593–8.CrossRef Vreugdenhil HJ, Slijper FM, Mulder PG, Weisglas-Kuperus N. Effects of perinatal exposure to PCBs and dioxins on play behaviour in Dutch children at school age. Environ Health Perspect. 2002;110:593–8.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Guo YL, Lai TJ, Chen SJ, Hsu CC. Gender-related decrease in Raven’s progressive matrices scores in children prenatally exposed to polychlorinated biphenyls and related contaminants. Bull Environ Contam Toxicol. 1995;55:8–13.PubMedCrossRef Guo YL, Lai TJ, Chen SJ, Hsu CC. Gender-related decrease in Raven’s progressive matrices scores in children prenatally exposed to polychlorinated biphenyls and related contaminants. Bull Environ Contam Toxicol. 1995;55:8–13.PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Gore AC. Developmental programming and endocrine disruptor effects on reproductive neuroendocrine systems. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2008;29:358–74.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Gore AC. Developmental programming and endocrine disruptor effects on reproductive neuroendocrine systems. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2008;29:358–74.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
13.
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Bradley RH. Children’s home environments, health, behavior, and intervention efforts: a review using the HOME inventory as a marker measure. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr. 1993;19:437–90. Bradley RH. Children’s home environments, health, behavior, and intervention efforts: a review using the HOME inventory as a marker measure. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr. 1993;19:437–90.
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Bradley RH, Caldwell BM. Home observation for measurement of the environment: a revision of the preschool scale. Am J Ment Defic. 1979;84:235–44.PubMed Bradley RH, Caldwell BM. Home observation for measurement of the environment: a revision of the preschool scale. Am J Ment Defic. 1979;84:235–44.PubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D, Van der Steene G, Vertommen H, Bleichrodt N, Uiterwijk JM. WAIS-III Nederlandstalige bewerking. Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger; 2000. Wechsler D, Van der Steene G, Vertommen H, Bleichrodt N, Uiterwijk JM. WAIS-III Nederlandstalige bewerking. Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger; 2000.
17.
Zurück zum Zitat van der Ploeg HM. De Zelf-Beoordelings Vragenlijst (STAI-DY). De ontwikkeling en validatie van een Nederlandstalige vragenlijst voor het meten van angst. Tijdschrift Voor Psychiatrie. 1982;24:576–88. van der Ploeg HM. De Zelf-Beoordelings Vragenlijst (STAI-DY). De ontwikkeling en validatie van een Nederlandstalige vragenlijst voor het meten van angst. Tijdschrift Voor Psychiatrie. 1982;24:576–88.
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Gómara B, Ramos L, González MJ. Determination of polychlorinated biphenyls in small-size serum samples by solid-phase extraction followed by gas chromatography with micro-electron-capture detection. J Chromatogr B Analyt Technol Biomed Life Sci. 2002;766:279–87.PubMedCrossRef Gómara B, Ramos L, González MJ. Determination of polychlorinated biphenyls in small-size serum samples by solid-phase extraction followed by gas chromatography with micro-electron-capture detection. J Chromatogr B Analyt Technol Biomed Life Sci. 2002;766:279–87.PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Covaci A, Voorspoels S, Thomsen C, van Bavel B, Neels H. Evaluation of total lipids using enzymatic methods for the normalization of persistent organic pollutant levels in serum. Sci Total Environ. 2006;366:361–6.PubMedCrossRef Covaci A, Voorspoels S, Thomsen C, van Bavel B, Neels H. Evaluation of total lipids using enzymatic methods for the normalization of persistent organic pollutant levels in serum. Sci Total Environ. 2006;366:361–6.PubMedCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Koppen G, Covaci A, Van Cleuvenbergen R, Schepens P, Winneke G, Nelen V, Schoeters G. Comparison of CALUX-TEQ values with PCB and PCDD/F measurements in human serum of the Flanders environmental and health study (FLEHS). Toxicol Lett. 2001;123:59–67.PubMedCrossRef Koppen G, Covaci A, Van Cleuvenbergen R, Schepens P, Winneke G, Nelen V, Schoeters G. Comparison of CALUX-TEQ values with PCB and PCDD/F measurements in human serum of the Flanders environmental and health study (FLEHS). Toxicol Lett. 2001;123:59–67.PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Van der Meulen BF, Ruiter SAJ, Lutje Spelberg HC, Smrkovský M. Bayley scales of infant development (BSID) – II. Nederlandse versie. Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger; 2002. Van der Meulen BF, Ruiter SAJ, Lutje Spelberg HC, Smrkovský M. Bayley scales of infant development (BSID) – II. Nederlandse versie. Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger; 2002.
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Rothbart MK. Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Dev. 1981;52:569–78.CrossRef Rothbart MK. Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Dev. 1981;52:569–78.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Schaerlaekens A, Zink I, Van Ommeslaeghe K. Reynell Taalontwikkelingsschalen (RTOS). Amsterdam: Pearson; 2003. Schaerlaekens A, Zink I, Van Ommeslaeghe K. Reynell Taalontwikkelingsschalen (RTOS). Amsterdam: Pearson; 2003.
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Tellegen PJ, Winkel M, Wijnberg-Williams BJ, Laros JA. Snijders-Oomen niet-verbale intelligentietest SON-R 21/2–7, handleiding en verantwoording [Snijders-Oomen Non-verbal Intelligence Test SON-R 2 1/2–7. Manual]. Lisse (Netherlands): Swets & Zeitlinger; 1998. Tellegen PJ, Winkel M, Wijnberg-Williams BJ, Laros JA. Snijders-Oomen niet-verbale intelligentietest SON-R 21/2–7, handleiding en verantwoording [Snijders-Oomen Non-verbal Intelligence Test SON-R 2 1/2–7. Manual]. Lisse (Netherlands): Swets & Zeitlinger; 1998.
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Berenbaum SA, Hines M. Early androgynes are related to childhood sex-typed toy preferences. Psychol Sci. 1992;3:203–6.CrossRef Berenbaum SA, Hines M. Early androgynes are related to childhood sex-typed toy preferences. Psychol Sci. 1992;3:203–6.CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Servin A, Bohlin G, Berling L. Sex differences in 1-, 3- and 5-year-olds’ toy choice in a structured play session. Scand J Psychol. 1999;40:43–8.PubMedCrossRef Servin A, Bohlin G, Berling L. Sex differences in 1-, 3- and 5-year-olds’ toy choice in a structured play session. Scand J Psychol. 1999;40:43–8.PubMedCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Letz R, Dilorio CK, Shafer PO, Yeager KA, Schomer DL, Henri TR. Futher standardisation of some NES3 tests. Neurotoxicoly. 2003;24:491–501. Letz R, Dilorio CK, Shafer PO, Yeager KA, Schomer DL, Henri TR. Futher standardisation of some NES3 tests. Neurotoxicoly. 2003;24:491–501.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Patandin S, Veenstra J, Mulder PGH, Sewnaik A, Sauer PJJ, Weisglas-Kuperus N. Attention and activity in 42-month-old Dutch children with environmental exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and dioxins. In: Effects of Environmental Exposure to Polychlorinated Biphenyls and Dioxins on Growth and Development in Young Children. Patandin, S. Thesis. Rotterdam: Erasmus University; 1999b. p. 123–42. Patandin S, Veenstra J, Mulder PGH, Sewnaik A, Sauer PJJ, Weisglas-Kuperus N. Attention and activity in 42-month-old Dutch children with environmental exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and dioxins. In: Effects of Environmental Exposure to Polychlorinated Biphenyls and Dioxins on Growth and Development in Young Children. Patandin, S. Thesis. Rotterdam: Erasmus University; 1999b. p. 123–42.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Shang L, Yang L, Yang W, Huang L, Qi C, Yang Z, Fu Z, Chung MC. Effects of prenatal exposure to NO2 on children's neurodevelopment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Environ Sci Pollut Res Int. 2020;27(20):24786–98.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Shang L, Yang L, Yang W, Huang L, Qi C, Yang Z, Fu Z, Chung MC. Effects of prenatal exposure to NO2 on children's neurodevelopment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Environ Sci Pollut Res Int. 2020;27(20):24786–98.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang H, Yolton K, Webster GM, Sjödin A, Calafat AM, Dietrich KN, Xu Y, Xie C, Braun JM, Lanphear BP, Chen A. Prenatal PBDE and PCB exposures and reading, cognition, and externalizing behavior in children. Environ Health Perspect. 2017;125:746–52.PubMedCrossRef Zhang H, Yolton K, Webster GM, Sjödin A, Calafat AM, Dietrich KN, Xu Y, Xie C, Braun JM, Lanphear BP, Chen A. Prenatal PBDE and PCB exposures and reading, cognition, and externalizing behavior in children. Environ Health Perspect. 2017;125:746–52.PubMedCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Berghuis SA, Van Braeckel KNJA, Sauer PJJ, Bos AF. Prenatal exposure to persistent organic pollutants and cognition and motor performance in adolescence. Environ Int. 2018;121:13–22.PubMedCrossRef Berghuis SA, Van Braeckel KNJA, Sauer PJJ, Bos AF. Prenatal exposure to persistent organic pollutants and cognition and motor performance in adolescence. Environ Int. 2018;121:13–22.PubMedCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim S, Eom S, Kim HJ, Lee JJ, Choi G, Choi S, Kim S, Kim SY, Cho G, Kim YD, Suh E, Kim SK, Kim S, Kim GH, Moon HB, Park J, Kim S, Choi K, Eun SH. Association between maternal exposure to major phthalates, heavy metals, and persistent organic pollutants, and the neurodevelopmental performances of their children at 1 to 2 years of age- CHECK cohort study. Sci Total Environ. 2018;624:377–84.PubMedCrossRef Kim S, Eom S, Kim HJ, Lee JJ, Choi G, Choi S, Kim S, Kim SY, Cho G, Kim YD, Suh E, Kim SK, Kim S, Kim GH, Moon HB, Park J, Kim S, Choi K, Eun SH. Association between maternal exposure to major phthalates, heavy metals, and persistent organic pollutants, and the neurodevelopmental performances of their children at 1 to 2 years of age- CHECK cohort study. Sci Total Environ. 2018;624:377–84.PubMedCrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosenquist AH, Hoyer BB, Julvez J, Sunyer J, Pedersen HS, Lenters V, Jonsson BA, Bonde JP, Toft G. Prenatal and Postnatal PCB-153 and p,p'-DDE Exposures and Behavior Scores at 5–9 Years of Age among Children in Greenland and Ukraine. Environ Health Perspect. 2017;125(10):CID 107002. https://doi.org/10.1289/EHP553.CrossRef Rosenquist AH, Hoyer BB, Julvez J, Sunyer J, Pedersen HS, Lenters V, Jonsson BA, Bonde JP, Toft G. Prenatal and Postnatal PCB-153 and p,p'-DDE Exposures and Behavior Scores at 5–9 Years of Age among Children in Greenland and Ukraine. Environ Health Perspect. 2017;125(10):CID 107002. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1289/​EHP553.CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Kyriklaki A, Vafeiadi M, Kampouri M, Koutra K, Roumeliotaki T, Chalkiadaki G, Anousaki D, Rantakokko P, Kiviranta H, Fthenou E, Bitsios P, Kyrtopoulos SA, Kogevinas M, Chatzi L. Prenatal exposure to persistent organic pollutants in association with offspring neuropsychological development at 4 years of age: the Rhea mother-child cohort, Crete. Greece Environ Int. 2016;97:204–11.PubMedCrossRef Kyriklaki A, Vafeiadi M, Kampouri M, Koutra K, Roumeliotaki T, Chalkiadaki G, Anousaki D, Rantakokko P, Kiviranta H, Fthenou E, Bitsios P, Kyrtopoulos SA, Kogevinas M, Chatzi L. Prenatal exposure to persistent organic pollutants in association with offspring neuropsychological development at 4 years of age: the Rhea mother-child cohort, Crete. Greece Environ Int. 2016;97:204–11.PubMedCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Rogan WJ, Glaben BC, McKinney JD, Carreras N, Hardy P, Thullen J, Tinglestad J, Tully M. Neonatal effects of transplacental exposure of PCBs and DDE. J Pediatr. 1986;109:335–41.PubMedCrossRef Rogan WJ, Glaben BC, McKinney JD, Carreras N, Hardy P, Thullen J, Tinglestad J, Tully M. Neonatal effects of transplacental exposure of PCBs and DDE. J Pediatr. 1986;109:335–41.PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW. Intellectual impairment in children exposed to polychlorinated biphenyls in utero. N Engl J Med. 1996;335:783–9.PubMedCrossRef Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW. Intellectual impairment in children exposed to polychlorinated biphenyls in utero. N Engl J Med. 1996;335:783–9.PubMedCrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Winneke G, Bucholski A, Heinzbow B, Kramer U, Schmidt E, Walkowiak J, Wiener JA, Steingrüber HJ. Developmental neurotoxicity of polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs): cognitive and psychomotor functions in 7-month old children. Toxicol Let. 1998;102–103:423–8.CrossRef Winneke G, Bucholski A, Heinzbow B, Kramer U, Schmidt E, Walkowiak J, Wiener JA, Steingrüber HJ. Developmental neurotoxicity of polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs): cognitive and psychomotor functions in 7-month old children. Toxicol Let. 1998;102–103:423–8.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Patandin S, Lanting CI, Mulder PG, Boersma ER, Sauer PJ, Wiesglas-Kuperus N. Effects of environmental exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and dioxins on cognitive abilities in Dutch children at 42 months of age. J Pediatr. 1999a;134(1):33–41.PubMedCrossRef Patandin S, Lanting CI, Mulder PG, Boersma ER, Sauer PJ, Wiesglas-Kuperus N. Effects of environmental exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and dioxins on cognitive abilities in Dutch children at 42 months of age. J Pediatr. 1999a;134(1):33–41.PubMedCrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Huisman M, Koopman-Esseboom C, Fidler V, Hadders-Algra M, van der Paauw CG, Tuinstra LG, Weisglas-Kuperus N, Sauer PJ, Touwen BC, Boersma ER. Perinatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and dioxins and its effect on neonatal neurological development. Early Hum Dev. 1995a;41:111–27.PubMedCrossRef Huisman M, Koopman-Esseboom C, Fidler V, Hadders-Algra M, van der Paauw CG, Tuinstra LG, Weisglas-Kuperus N, Sauer PJ, Touwen BC, Boersma ER. Perinatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and dioxins and its effect on neonatal neurological development. Early Hum Dev. 1995a;41:111–27.PubMedCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Huisman M, Koopman-Esseboom C, Cl L, van der Paauw CG, Tuinstra LG, Fidler V, Weisglas-Kuperus N, Sauer PJ, Boersma ER, Touwen BC. Neurological conditions in 18-month-old children perinatally exposed to polychloirinated biphenyls and dioxins. Early Hum Dev. 1995b;41:165–76.CrossRef Huisman M, Koopman-Esseboom C, Cl L, van der Paauw CG, Tuinstra LG, Fidler V, Weisglas-Kuperus N, Sauer PJ, Boersma ER, Touwen BC. Neurological conditions in 18-month-old children perinatally exposed to polychloirinated biphenyls and dioxins. Early Hum Dev. 1995b;41:165–76.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Steuerwald UP, Weihe PJ, Jorgensen K, Bjerve K, Brock J, Heinzow B, Budtz-Jorgensen E, Grandjean P. Maternal seafood diet, methylmercury exposure, and neonatal neurologic function. J Pediatr. 2000;136(5):599–605.PubMedCrossRef Steuerwald UP, Weihe PJ, Jorgensen K, Bjerve K, Brock J, Heinzow B, Budtz-Jorgensen E, Grandjean P. Maternal seafood diet, methylmercury exposure, and neonatal neurologic function. J Pediatr. 2000;136(5):599–605.PubMedCrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Guo YL, Lambert GH, Hsu C-C, Hsu MM. Yucheng: health effects of prenatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and dibenzofurans. Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 2004;177:153–8.CrossRef Guo YL, Lambert GH, Hsu C-C, Hsu MM. Yucheng: health effects of prenatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and dibenzofurans. Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 2004;177:153–8.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Boucher O, Muckle G, Ayotte P, Dewailly E, Jacobson SW, Jacobson JL. Altered fine motor function at school age in Inuit children exposed to PCBs, methylmercury, and lead. Environ Int. 2016;95:144–51.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Boucher O, Muckle G, Ayotte P, Dewailly E, Jacobson SW, Jacobson JL. Altered fine motor function at school age in Inuit children exposed to PCBs, methylmercury, and lead. Environ Int. 2016;95:144–51.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Sajaniemi N, Salokorpi T, von Wendt L. Temperament profiles and their role in neurodevelopment assessed preterm children at two years of age. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;7(3):145–52.PubMedCrossRef Sajaniemi N, Salokorpi T, von Wendt L. Temperament profiles and their role in neurodevelopment assessed preterm children at two years of age. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;7(3):145–52.PubMedCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK. Studying infant temperament with revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav Dev. 2003;26:64–86.CrossRef Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK. Studying infant temperament with revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav Dev. 2003;26:64–86.CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Gartstein MA, Skinner MK. Prenatal influences on temperament development: the role of environmental epigenetics. Dev Psychopathol. 2018;30:1269–303.PubMedCrossRef Gartstein MA, Skinner MK. Prenatal influences on temperament development: the role of environmental epigenetics. Dev Psychopathol. 2018;30:1269–303.PubMedCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Rush D, Callahan KR. Exposure to passive cigarette smoking and child development. A critical review. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1989;562:74–100.PubMedCrossRef Rush D, Callahan KR. Exposure to passive cigarette smoking and child development. A critical review. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1989;562:74–100.PubMedCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW, HEB H. Effects of exposure to PCBs and related compounds on growth and activity in children. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1990;12:319–26.PubMedCrossRef Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW, HEB H. Effects of exposure to PCBs and related compounds on growth and activity in children. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1990;12:319–26.PubMedCrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaufman AS. Critique of Vreugdenhil et al.'s study linking PCBs to the play behaviors of Dutch girls and boys. Environ Health Perspect. 2003;111(7):A380–1.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kaufman AS. Critique of Vreugdenhil et al.'s study linking PCBs to the play behaviors of Dutch girls and boys. Environ Health Perspect. 2003;111(7):A380–1.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Breckenridge K, Braddick O, Atkinson J. The organization of attention in typical development: a new preschool attention test battery. Br J Dev Psychol. 2013;31:271–88.PubMedCrossRef Breckenridge K, Braddick O, Atkinson J. The organization of attention in typical development: a new preschool attention test battery. Br J Dev Psychol. 2013;31:271–88.PubMedCrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW, Padgett RJ, Brummitt GA, Billings L. Effects of prenatal PCB exposure on cognitive processing efficiency and sustained attention. Dev Psychol. 1992;28(2):297–306.CrossRef Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW, Padgett RJ, Brummitt GA, Billings L. Effects of prenatal PCB exposure on cognitive processing efficiency and sustained attention. Dev Psychol. 1992;28(2):297–306.CrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Stewart P, Fitzgerald S, Reihman J, Gump B, Lonky E, Darvill T, Pagano J, Hauser P. Prenatal PCB exposure, the corpus callosum, and response inhibition. Environ Health Perspect. 2003;111(13):1670–7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Stewart P, Fitzgerald S, Reihman J, Gump B, Lonky E, Darvill T, Pagano J, Hauser P. Prenatal PCB exposure, the corpus callosum, and response inhibition. Environ Health Perspect. 2003;111(13):1670–7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW. Breastfeeding and gender as moderators of teratogenic effects on cognitive development. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2002;24:349–58.PubMedCrossRef Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW. Breastfeeding and gender as moderators of teratogenic effects on cognitive development. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2002;24:349–58.PubMedCrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW. Prenatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and attention at school age. J Paediatr. 2003;143(6):780–8.CrossRef Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW. Prenatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and attention at school age. J Paediatr. 2003;143(6):780–8.CrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Vreugdenhil HJ, Mulder PG, Emmen HH, Weisglas-Kuperus N. Effects of perinatal exposure to PCBs on neuropsychological function in the Rotterdam cohort at 9 years of age. Neuropsychology. 2004;18(1):185–93.PubMedCrossRef Vreugdenhil HJ, Mulder PG, Emmen HH, Weisglas-Kuperus N. Effects of perinatal exposure to PCBs on neuropsychological function in the Rotterdam cohort at 9 years of age. Neuropsychology. 2004;18(1):185–93.PubMedCrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Caspersen IH, Haugen M, Schjolberg S, Vejrup K, Knutsen HK, Brantsaeter AL, Meltzer HM, Alexander J, Magnus P, Kvalem HE. Maternal dietary exposure to dioxins and polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) is associated with language delay in 3year old Norwegian children. Environ Int. 2016a;91:180–7.PubMedCrossRef Caspersen IH, Haugen M, Schjolberg S, Vejrup K, Knutsen HK, Brantsaeter AL, Meltzer HM, Alexander J, Magnus P, Kvalem HE. Maternal dietary exposure to dioxins and polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) is associated with language delay in 3year old Norwegian children. Environ Int. 2016a;91:180–7.PubMedCrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Caspersen IH, Aase H, Biele G, Brantsaeter AL, Haugen M, Kvalem HE, Skogan AH, Zeiner P, Alexander J, Meltzer HM, Knutsen HK. The influence of maternal dietary exposure to dioxins and PCBs during pregnancy on ADHD symptoms and cognitive functions in Norwegian preschool children. Environ Int. 2016b;94:649–60.PubMedCrossRef Caspersen IH, Aase H, Biele G, Brantsaeter AL, Haugen M, Kvalem HE, Skogan AH, Zeiner P, Alexander J, Meltzer HM, Knutsen HK. The influence of maternal dietary exposure to dioxins and PCBs during pregnancy on ADHD symptoms and cognitive functions in Norwegian preschool children. Environ Int. 2016b;94:649–60.PubMedCrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Ikeno T, Miyashita C, Nakajima S, Kobayashi S, Yamazaki K, Saijo Y, Kita T, Sasaki S, Konishi K, Kajiwara J, Hori T, Kishi R. Effects of low-level prenatal exposure to dioxins on cognitive development in Japanese children at 42months. Sci Total Environ. 2018;618:1423–30.PubMedCrossRef Ikeno T, Miyashita C, Nakajima S, Kobayashi S, Yamazaki K, Saijo Y, Kita T, Sasaki S, Konishi K, Kajiwara J, Hori T, Kishi R. Effects of low-level prenatal exposure to dioxins on cognitive development in Japanese children at 42months. Sci Total Environ. 2018;618:1423–30.PubMedCrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Karkaba A, Soualeh N, Soulimani R, Bouayed J. Perinatal effects of exposure to PCBs on social preferences in young adult and middle-aged offspring mice. Horm Behav. 2017;96:137–46.PubMedCrossRef Karkaba A, Soualeh N, Soulimani R, Bouayed J. Perinatal effects of exposure to PCBs on social preferences in young adult and middle-aged offspring mice. Horm Behav. 2017;96:137–46.PubMedCrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Miller MM, Meyer AE, Sprowles JL, Sable HJ. Cocaine self-administration in male and female rats perinatally exposed to PCBs: evaluating drug use in an animal model of environmental contaminant exposure. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2017;25(2):114–24.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Miller MM, Meyer AE, Sprowles JL, Sable HJ. Cocaine self-administration in male and female rats perinatally exposed to PCBs: evaluating drug use in an animal model of environmental contaminant exposure. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2017;25(2):114–24.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Bell MR, Thompson LM, Rodriguez K, Gore AC. Two-hit exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls at gestational and juvenile life stages: 1. Sexually dimorphic effects on social and anxiety-like behaviors. Horm.Behav. 2016;78:168–77.PubMedCrossRef Bell MR, Thompson LM, Rodriguez K, Gore AC. Two-hit exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls at gestational and juvenile life stages: 1. Sexually dimorphic effects on social and anxiety-like behaviors. Horm.Behav. 2016;78:168–77.PubMedCrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Cauli O, Piedrafita B, Llansola M, Felipo V. Gender differential effects of developmental exposure to methyl-mercury, polychlorinated biphenyls 126 or 153, or its combinations on motor activity and coordination. Toxicology. 2013;311:61–8.PubMedCrossRef Cauli O, Piedrafita B, Llansola M, Felipo V. Gender differential effects of developmental exposure to methyl-mercury, polychlorinated biphenyls 126 or 153, or its combinations on motor activity and coordination. Toxicology. 2013;311:61–8.PubMedCrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Cao Y, Winneke G, Wilhelm M, Wittsiepe J, Lemm F, Fürst P, Ranft U, Imöhl L, Kraft M, Oesch-Bartlomowicz B, Kramer U. Environmental exposure to dioxins and polychlorinated biphenyls reduce levels of gonadal hormones in newborns: results from the Duisburg cohort study. Int J Hyg Environ Health. 2008;211:30–9.PubMedCrossRef Cao Y, Winneke G, Wilhelm M, Wittsiepe J, Lemm F, Fürst P, Ranft U, Imöhl L, Kraft M, Oesch-Bartlomowicz B, Kramer U. Environmental exposure to dioxins and polychlorinated biphenyls reduce levels of gonadal hormones in newborns: results from the Duisburg cohort study. Int J Hyg Environ Health. 2008;211:30–9.PubMedCrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Colciago A, Casati L, Mornati O, Vergoni AV, Santagostino A, Celotti F, Negri-Cesi P. Chronic treatment with polychlorinated biphenyls (PCB) during pregnancy and lactation in the rat part 2: effects on reproductive parameters, on sex behavior, on memory retention and on hypothalamic expression of aromatase and 5alpha-reductases in the offspring. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 2009;239:46–54.PubMedCrossRef Colciago A, Casati L, Mornati O, Vergoni AV, Santagostino A, Celotti F, Negri-Cesi P. Chronic treatment with polychlorinated biphenyls (PCB) during pregnancy and lactation in the rat part 2: effects on reproductive parameters, on sex behavior, on memory retention and on hypothalamic expression of aromatase and 5alpha-reductases in the offspring. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 2009;239:46–54.PubMedCrossRef
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Topper VY, Reilly MP, Wagner LM, Thompson LM, Gillette R, Crews D, Gore AC. Social and neuromolecular phenotypes are programmed by prenatal exposures to endocrine-disrupting chemicals. Mol Cell Endocrinol. 2019;479:133–46.PubMedCrossRef Topper VY, Reilly MP, Wagner LM, Thompson LM, Gillette R, Crews D, Gore AC. Social and neuromolecular phenotypes are programmed by prenatal exposures to endocrine-disrupting chemicals. Mol Cell Endocrinol. 2019;479:133–46.PubMedCrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat De Coster S, van Leeuwen DM, Jennen DG, Koppen G, Den Hond E, Nelen V, Schoeters G, Baeyens W, van Delft JH, Kleinjans JC, Van Larebeke N. Gender-specific transcriptomic response to environmental exposure in Flemish adults. Environ Mol Mutagen. 2013;54(7):574–88.PubMedCrossRef De Coster S, van Leeuwen DM, Jennen DG, Koppen G, Den Hond E, Nelen V, Schoeters G, Baeyens W, van Delft JH, Kleinjans JC, Van Larebeke N. Gender-specific transcriptomic response to environmental exposure in Flemish adults. Environ Mol Mutagen. 2013;54(7):574–88.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Neurobehavioural and cognitive effects of prenatal exposure to organochlorine compounds in three year old children
verfasst von
Griet Vermeir
Adrian Covaci
Nik Van Larebeke
Greet Schoeters
Vera Nelen
Gudrun Koppen
Mineke Viaene
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2021
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Pediatrics / Ausgabe 1/2021
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2431
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-021-02533-2

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2021

BMC Pediatrics 1/2021 Zur Ausgabe

Neuer Typ-1-Diabetes bei Kindern am Wochenende eher übersehen

23.04.2024 Typ-1-Diabetes Nachrichten

Wenn Kinder an Werktagen zum Arzt gehen, werden neu auftretender Typ-1-Diabetes und diabetische Ketoazidosen häufiger erkannt als bei Arztbesuchen an Wochenenden oder Feiertagen.

Neue Studienergebnisse zur Myopiekontrolle mit Atropin

22.04.2024 Fehlsichtigkeit Nachrichten

Augentropfen mit niedrig dosiertem Atropin können helfen, das Fortschreiten einer Kurzsichtigkeit bei Kindern zumindest zu verlangsamen, wie die Ergebnisse einer aktuellen Studie mit verschiedenen Dosierungen zeigen.

Spinale Muskelatrophie: Neugeborenen-Screening lohnt sich

18.04.2024 Spinale Muskelatrophien Nachrichten

Seit 2021 ist die Untersuchung auf spinale Muskelatrophie Teil des Neugeborenen-Screenings in Deutschland. Eine Studie liefert weitere Evidenz für den Nutzen der Maßnahme.

Fünf Dinge, die im Kindernotfall besser zu unterlassen sind

18.04.2024 Pädiatrische Notfallmedizin Nachrichten

Im Choosing-Wisely-Programm, das für die deutsche Initiative „Klug entscheiden“ Pate gestanden hat, sind erstmals Empfehlungen zum Umgang mit Notfällen von Kindern erschienen. Fünf Dinge gilt es demnach zu vermeiden.

Update Pädiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.