Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics 1/2021

Open Access 01.12.2021 | Research article

Physical growth and neurodevelopment during the first year of life: a cohort study of the Japan Environment and Children’s Study

verfasst von: Masafumi Sanefuji, Yuri Sonoda, Yoshiya Ito, Masanobu Ogawa, Vlad Tocan, Hirosuke Inoue, Masayuki Ochiai, Masayuki Shimono, Reiko Suga, Ayako Senju, Satoshi Honjo, Koichi Kusuhara, Shouichi Ohga, Japan Environment and Children’s Study Group

Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics | Ausgabe 1/2021

Abstract

Background

The association between a slower physical growth and poorer neurodevelopment has been established in infants born preterm or small for gestational age. However, this association is inconsistent in term-born infants, and detailed investigations in infancy, when intervention is most beneficial for improving outcomes, are lacking. We therefore examined this association separately by sex during the first year of life in term-born infants.

Methods

Using data collected until children reached 12 months old in an ongoing prospective cohort of the Japan Environment and Children’s Study, we analyzed 44,264 boys and 42,541 girls with singleton term-birth. The exposure variables were conditional variables that disentangle linear growth from weight gain relative to linear growth, calculated from the length and weight at birth and 4, 7 and 10 months old. Neurodevelopmental delay was identified using the Japanese-translated version of Ages & Stages Questionnaires, third edition.

Results

A reduced risk of neurodevelopmental delay at 6 months old was observed in children with a higher birth weight (adjusted relative risks [aRRs]: 0.91 and 0.93, 95 % confidence intervals [95 % CIs]: 0.87–0.96 and 0.88–0.98 in boys and girls, respectively) and increased linear growth between 0 and 4 months old (aRRs: 0.85 and 0.87, 95 % CIs: 0.82–0.88 and 0.83–0.91 in boys and girls, respectively). A reduced risk at 12 months was found in children with an increased linear growth between 0 and 4 months (aRRs: 0.92 and 0.90, 95 % CIs: 0.87–0.98 and 0.84–0.96 in boys and girls, respectively), boys with an increased relative weight gain between 0 and 4 months (aRR: 0.90, 95 % CI: 0.84–0.97), and girls with a higher birth weight (aRR: 0.89, 95 % CI: 0.83–0.96).

Conclusions

These results suggest that a slow physical growth by four months old may be a predictor of neurodevelopmental delay during infancy.
Begleitmaterial
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s12887-021-02815-9.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
aRR
Adjusted relative risk
ASQ-3
Ages & Stages Questionnaires, third edition
CI
Confidence interval
JECS
The Japan Environment and Children’s Study
JPY
Japanese Yen

Background

Prenatal and postnatal growth of the body has been linked to later neurocognitive development. Growth restriction in utero is suggested to occur in infants born small for gestational age while poor growth from birth to term-equivalent age is often observed in infants born preterm. Such growth-faltering infants are at high risk of poor neurodevelopmental outcomes later in life [1, 2]. In these infants, a lack of catch-up growth is an important indicator of a poor neurodevelopment [35]. The association between physical growth and neurodevelopment has also been examined in general populations consisting mostly of term-born infants, but the findings are less consistent in such settings. Indeed, while several studies with limited sample sizes have shown positive results [614], others have reported no association [15, 16]. Furthermore, as outcomes, a majority of these studies investigated the neurocognitive function during preschool through adulthood [610, 1316] with only a few examining those in infancy [8, 11, 12], during which intervention has the potential to improve the outcomes of both the parents and their children [17, 18].
Most of these previous studies have separately assessed anthropometric gains in either length/height or weight between two age points (e.g. birth to six months old). As the length/height and weight are strongly correlated within an individual, assessing these indices per se hampers the determination of which component is more influential. To dissociate the effects of length/height growth from those of weight growth, Adair et al. and Horta et al. used a sophisticated method of ‘conditional growth modeling’ [19, 20]. This modeling determines conditional variables by regressing the current size (i.e. length or weight) against all previous sizes [21, 22]. With respect to each period, the variables comprise ‘linear growth’ and ‘weight gain relative to linear growth’, which represent length/height change and weight change separated from change in length/height, respectively. Their studies demonstrated a closer relationship of school achievement and intelligence with linear growth than with relative weight again, noting some differences in patterns between sexes.
Using a conditional growth model with a nationwide prospective cohort from the Japan Environment and Children’s Study (JECS) that includes over 100,000 children, the present study examined the relationship between physical growth and neurodevelopment separately by sex during the first year of life in term-born infants. To collect neurodevelopmental information from such a large number of children, we used a parent-reported screening tool rather than individualized face-to-face tests. During this period, neurodevelopment is difficult to assess precisely, and important changes can manifest in the individual developmental trajectory. However, a detailed investigation of this relationship during infancy is extremely important for detecting neurodevelopmental delay for early intervention.

Methods

Design

The JECS is a nationwide, multicenter, prospective birth cohort study funded by the Ministry of Environment, Japan. The study design details have been described elsewhere [23, 24]. In brief, pregnant participants were registered between January 2011 and March 2014 in 15 regional centers covering a wide geographical area in Japan. During pregnancy, data were obtained during the first and second/third trimesters using self-administered questionnaires. Detailed information regarding the mother and child was obtained from medical records transcripts during the first trimester, at the time of delivery, and when the child was 1 month old. After delivery, data were collected at one and six months old and every six months until the child was six years old, then twice a year thereafter via self-reported questionnaires completed by the parents.
The JECS protocol was reviewed and approved by the Ministry of Environment’s Institutional Review Board for Epidemiological Studies and by the ethics committees of all participating institutions (#15000141). The ethical approval for this study was an extension of the ethical approval for the JECS protocol. Written informed consent was obtained from all parents.

Participants

In this study, we used the fixed dataset “jecs-an-20180131” released in March 2018. The dataset includes all available data of 104,065 fetuses, linked to their mothers’ data, collected until children were 12 months old. As subjects, we selected 89,953 children with live singleton term birth (≥ 37 and < 42 gestational weeks) who had parents of Japanese nationality and complete basic information on mother’s parity, child’s sex, length and weight at birth (Fig. 1). After excluding 3,148 children who had malformation/severe diseases, we eventually analyzed 86,805 children, including 44,264 boys and 42,541 girls.

Exposure

The exposure variable was body growth from conception to 10 months old. Length and weight at birth, which can be viewed as growth from conception (length and weight = 0) to birth, were obtained from medical records. Subsequent anthropometric data were all derived from the personal health record of the child. Nearly all Japanese children receive a public health checkup at four months old. Their length and weight were thus directly measured by a public health nurse or a hospital nurse and officially documented in the record. They also receive similar checkups at 7 or 10 months old, occasionally at both 7 and 10 months old, depending on residency. Parents were asked to transcribe these anthropometric data at the 4-month checkup in the 6-month questionnaire and those at the 7- and 10-month checkups in the 12-month questionnaire. Because the data were not measured at exactly 4, 7 and 10 months, the values were extrapolated from the anthropometric data at birth and those at the 4-, 7- and 10-month checkups [25]. The extrapolated data were referenced to the Japanese growth standards separately by sex and then converted into z-scores (i.e. length-for-age, weight-for-age) [26, 27]. In the standards, the mother’s parity and child’s gestational age are additionally taken into consideration when calculating z-scores at birth.
These z-scored length and weight values at different ages were processed using conditional growth modeling, as in the studies conducted by Adair et al. and Horta et al. [19, 20] but additionally including birth length in our study. Conditional variables were obtained by regressing the current size (i.e. length or weight) against all previous sizes [21, 22]. At each time point, a conditional variable represents physical growth during a time interval, and a positive value represents a linear growth or relative weight gain faster than predicted in that period. For example, the conditional length at seven months old indicates the linear growth from four to seven months old. To estimate the conditional length, the current length was regressed against all previous lengths and weights. The conditional length at seven months old was derived by regressing the length at seven months old against the length and weight at four months old and birth. To estimate the conditional relative weight, the current weight was regressed against the current length and all previous lengths and weights. For instance, the conditional relative weight at seven months old was calculated by regressing the weight at seven months old against the length at seven months old and the length and weight at four months old and birth. Note that the length and weight at birth per se were treated as conditional variables at birth, as they had no previous anthropometric data to be regressed against. Consequently, our conditional variables included length and weight at birth and conditional length and relative weight at 4, 7 and/or 10 months old.

Outcomes

The outcome measure was neurodevelopmental delay at 6 and 12 months old using the Japanese-translated version of the Ages & Stages Questionnaires: third edition (ASQ-3). This version was prepared through a back-translation procedure and was approved by the publisher of the original English version [28]. The ASQ-3 can identify infants or young children who require further neurodevelopmental assessments to determine their eligibility for early intervention. The findings of the questionnaire basically agree with those of professionally administered developmental batteries [29, 30]. It has been used in clinical and research settings and translated into several languages [3133].
The ASQ-3 assesses five developmental domains: Communication, Gross Motor, Fine Motor, Problem Solving, and Personal-Social domains. For each domain, six skills are described to which parents answer “yes,” “sometimes,” or “not yet,” depending on whether or not their child has demonstrated the described skill. The responses are converted to points, with “yes” receiving 10 points; “sometimes”, 5 points; and “not yet”, 0 points. The child’s score for each developmental domain is the sum of all points received for the items under that domain and ranges from 0 to 60 points. The cut-off score for each domain is defined as two standard deviations below the mean score of large, standardized samples in the United States of America. A child is defined as having a neurodevelopmental delay if a score is at or below the cut-off level in any developmental domain. Although preliminary cut-off scores of the Japanese translation were recently proposed [34], these were not recommended to be used with confidence before 24 months old because of very limited sample sizes. Therefore, the cut-off scores were determined using the same methodologies as the original version, based on available data at 6 (n = 82,994) and 12 months old (n = 78,947) (Fig. 1), which represent the general Japanese population.

Statistical analyses

To examine which component and periods of physical growth are specifically associated with infant neurodevelopment, we conducted multivariable regression analyses using conditional variables at different ages. Because these variables are not correlated with each other, the variables and covariates were included together in the same model without concerns about collinearity [20]. The covariates were (i) mother’s age; (ii) maternal smoking during pregnancy, as recorded in the first trimester; (iii) maternal and (iv) paternal education level (junior high school, high school, and university or graduate school); (v) annual family income (< 4 000 000, 4 000 000–5 999 999, ≥ 6 000 000 JPY); (vi) home speech stimulation at 1 month old (whether a mother did or did not talk to her baby habitually). The ‘home speech stimulation’ covariate was used instead of the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment scale [35], which is not employed in the JECS.
All analyses were performed using the R software program, version 4.0.3 (The R Foundation, Vienna, Austria). Quasi-Poisson regression models were used to estimate adjusted relative risks (aRRs) with 95 % confidence intervals (CIs).

Results

Among the 44,264 boys and 42,541 girls eligible for the analysis, anthropometric data were available at 4 months of age in 37,136 (83.9 %) boys and 35,732 (84.0 %) girls, at 7 months in 16,231 (36.7 %) boys and 15,758 (37.0 %) girls, and at 10 months in 25,616 (57.9 %) boys and 24,570 (57.8 %) girls, respectively. Ultimately, 10,016 (22.6 %) boys and 9,801 (23.0 %) girls had complete data at all timepoints of birth, 4, 7 and 10 months of age.
In these children, neurodevelopmental delay at 6 months old was observed at similar rates in boys (8.9 %) and girls (8.5 %), whereas that at 12 months old was more frequently identified in boys (16.9 %) than in girls (12.6 %). The baseline characteristics of the children who did not have all data available were comparable to those of children with complete data, but their parents tended to more often be multipara, have a smoking habit and have less education and income than those of complete-data subjects (Table 1). The comparisons between children with and without anthropometric data using other combinations among 4, 7 and 10 months old showed similar tendencies (Table S1, S2, S3).
Table 1
Baseline characteristics of the children with and without all anthropometric data at 4, 7 and 10 months old (n = 86,805)
 
Boy
Girl
 
With data(n = 10,016)
Missing
Without data(n = 34,248)
Missing
Effect sizea
With data(n = 9,801)
Missing
Without data(n = 32,740)
Missing
Effect sizea
Gestational age, mean (SD), wk
39.4 (1.1)
0
39.4 (1.1)
0
0.00
39.5 (1.1)
0
39.5 (1.1)
0
0.00
Birth length, mean (SD), cm
49.4 (2.0)
0
49.4 (1.9)
0
0.01
48.8 (1.9)
0
48.8 (1.9)
0
0.01
Birth weight, mean (SD), g
3,088 (371)
0
3,117 (367)
0
0.03
2,993 (356)
0
3,021 (357)
0
0.03
Multipara (%)
5,179 (51.7)
0
21,291 (62.2)
0
0.09
5,040 (51.4)
0
20,455 (62.5)
0
0.09
Maternal age, mean (SD), y
31.4 (4.8)
1
31.1 (5.1)
4
0.03
31.4 (4.9)
0
31.0 (5.1)
1
0.03
Maternal smoking (%)
1,435 (14.5)
112
6,418 (19.3)
948
0.05
1,429 (14.7)
109
6,167 (19.4)
891
0.05
Maternal education (%)
 
83
 
856
0.05
 
88
 
799
0.06
  Junior high school
321 (3.2)
 
1,798 (5.4)
  
282 (2.9)
 
1,690 (5.3)
  
  High school
7,233 (72.8)
 
24,661 (73.9)
  
7,082 (72.9)
 
23,732 (74.3)
  
  University/graduate school
2,379 (24.0)
 
6,933 (20.8)
  
2,349 (24.2)
 
6,519 (20.4)
  
Paternal education (%)
 
137
 
1,073
0.04
 
137
 
1,010
0.04
  Junior high school
520 (5.3)
 
2,619 (7.9)
  
545 (5.6)
 
2,527 (8.0)
  
  High school
5,887 (59.6)
 
19,713 (59.4)
  
5,748 (59.5)
 
18,882 (59.5)
  
  University/graduate school
3,472 (35.1)
 
10,843 (32.7)
  
3,371 (34.9)
 
10,321 (32.5)
  
Family income (%)
 
723
 
3,207
0.04
 
687
 
2,882
0.03
  Low
3,428 (36.9)
 
12,719 (41.0)
  
3,453 (37.9)
 
12,232 (41.0)
  
  Middle
3,162 (34.0)
 
10,242 (33.0)
  
2,998 (32.9)
 
9,837 (32.9)
  
  High
2,703 (29.1)
 
8,080 (26.0)
  
2,663 (29.2)
 
7,789 (26.1)
  
Home speech stimulation (%)
8,115 (81.4)
50
26,998 (81.1)
950
0.00
7,916 (81.4)
75
25,879 (81.3)
901
0.00
ND at 6 months (%)
841 (8.9)
599
2,439 (8.5)
5,636
0.01
782 (8.5)
595
2,169 (7.9)
5,215
0.01
ND at 12 months (%)
1,576 (16.9)
712
4,253 (15.8)
7,324
0.01
1,221 (13.6)
819
3,247 (12.6)
6,945
0.01
aEffect size indicates the strength of the difference between groups and is calculated as phi/Cramer’s V and r, using chi-square and Student’s t-tests for categorical and numerical variables, respectively.
ND neurodevelopmental delay; SD standard deviation
Multivariable regression models revealed that a reduced risk of neurodevelopmental delay at 6 months old was found in boys with a higher birth weight (aRR: 0.91, 95 % CI: 0.87–0.96) and an increased conditional length at 4 months old (aRR: 0.85, 95 % CI: 0.82–0.88) (Table 2), and similarly in girls with a higher birth weight (aRR: 0.93, 95 % CI: 0.88–0.98) and an increased conditional length at 4 months old (aRR: 0.87, 95 % CI: 0.83–0.91) (Table 3). On analyzing each neurodevelopmental domain of ASQ-3, the conditional length at four months old was associated with all five domains, while the birth weight was associated with some domains, supporting the stronger effect sizes of conditional length at four months old than those of birth weight for both sexes.
Table 2
Association between conditional variables and neurodevelopmental delay at six months old in boys
Conditional variables at birth and 4 months old
Total
Communication
Gross motor
Fine motor
Problem solving
Personal-Social
2,714/31,399 (8.6 %)
578/31,595 (1.8 %)
526/31,590 (1.7 %)
823/31,506 (2.6 %)
1,204/31,590 (3.8 %)
492/31,539 (1.6 %)
Length at birth
0.99 (0.95–1.04)
0.93 (0.83–1.03)
0.90 (0.80-1.00)
1.01 (0.92–1.10)
1.01 (0.94–1.09)
1.02 (0.91–1.15)
Weight at birth
0.91 (0.87–0.96)
1.02 (0.92–1.14)
0.97 (0.86–1.08)
0.88 (0.80–0.96)
0.88 (0.82–0.95)
0.87 (0.77–0.98)
cLength at 4 months old
0.85 (0.82–0.88)
0.84 (0.76–0.92)
0.83 (0.77–0.90)
0.82 (0.77–0.88)
0.84 (0.79–0.90)
0.84 (0.77–0.92)
crWeight at 4 months old
1.03 (0.98–1.08)
0.88 (0.79–0.99)
1.24 (1.11–1.38)
0.99 (0.90–1.09)
0.98 (0.90–1.06)
1.11 (0.98–1.25)
Data represent the adjusted relative risk (95 % confidence interval). All of the conditional variables and the following covariates were included together in the model: maternal age and smoking, maternal and paternal education, family income, and home speech stimulation.
cLength conditional length; crWeight conditional relative weight
Table 3
Association between conditional variables and neurodevelopmental delay at six months old in girls
Conditional variables at birth and 4 months old
Total
Communication
Gross motor
Fine motor
Problem solving
Personal-Social
2,432/30,358 (8.0 %)
584/30,504 (1.9 %)
468/30,505 (1.5 %)
743/30,436 (2.4 %)
1,113/30,510 (3.6 %)
290/30,476 (1.0 %)
Length at birth
0.99 (0.95–1.05)
1.04 (0.93–1.16)
1.12 (0.99–1.26)
0.98 (0.89–1.07)
0.96 (0.89–1.04)
0.90 (0.78–1.05)
Weight at birth
0.93 (0.88–0.98)
0.93 (0.84–1.04)
0.83 (0.73–0.94)
0.96 (0.87–1.06)
0.95 (0.88–1.02)
0.94 (0.81–1.10)
cLength at 4 months old
0.87 (0.83–0.91)
0.86 (0.78–0.94)
0.89 (0.81–0.99)
0.88 (0.81–0.96)
0.83 (0.78–0.89)
0.80 (0.72–0.91)
crWeight at 4 months old
1.05 (0.99–1.11)
0.92 (0.82–1.03)
1.44 (1.26–1.63)
0.99 (0.89–1.10)
0.99 (0.91–1.07)
1.14 (0.97–1.34)
Data represent the adjusted relative risk (95 % confidence interval). All of the conditional variables and the following covariates were included together in the model: maternal age and smoking, maternal and paternal education, family income, and home speech stimulation.
cLength conditional length; crWeight conditional relative weight
With regard to neurodevelopmental delay at 12 months old, we first created a regression model using the data of children who had complete anthropometric data throughout the first year of age and all covariates (Tables 4 and 5, model 1). In boys, a reduced risk was related to an increased conditional length at 4 months old (aRR: 0.92, 95 % CI: 0.87–0.98) and an increased conditional relative weight at 4 months old (aRR: 0.90, 95 % CI: 0.84–0.97) (Table 4). In girls, a reduced risk was related to a higher birth weight (aRR: 0.89, 95 % CI: 0.83–0.96) and an increased conditional length at 4 months old (aRR: 0.90, 95 % CI: 0.84–0.96) (Table 5). Analyses of each neurodevelopmental domain revealed that, for both sexes, conditional length at four months old was most consistently associated with several domains compared with other conditional variables, again suggesting the importance of this variable. Conversely, the risk was largely unrelated to conditional variables at 7 and 10 months old, although an increased risk was paradoxically observed in girls with an increased conditional length at 7 months old (aRR: 1.16, 95 % CI: 1.07–1.26) (Table 5). We also built 2 other regression models using the data of children with available data from 4 to 7 months old (model 2) and those with available data from 4 to 10 months old (model 3). The results of the two models included some inconsistencies but yielded similar results concerning the importance of conditional variables in early infancy.
Table 4
Association between conditional variables and neurodevelopmental delay at 12 months old in boys
[Model 1] Conditional variables at birth, 4, 7 and 10 months old
Total
Communication
Gross motor
Fine motor
Problem solving
Personal-Social
 
1,441/8,488 (17.0 %)
396/8,534 (4.6 %)
458/8,532 (5.4 %)
372/8,526 (4.4 %)
511/8,519 (6.0 %)
303/8,506 (3.6 %)
Length at birth
1.00 (0.94–1.07)
0.99 (0.87–1.12)
1.01 (0.90–1.13)
1.03 (0.90–1.17)
0.97 (0.87–1.09)
1.00 (0.86–1.16)
Weight at birth
0.97 (0.91–1.03)
0.97 (0.85–1.10)
0.96 (0.86–1.08)
1.00 (0.88–1.14)
0.97 (0.87–1.08)
0.92 (0.79–1.06)
cLength at 4 months old
0.92 (0.87–0.98)
0.87 (0.78–0.98)
0.98 (0.89–1.09)
0.81 (0.73–0.91)
0.86 (0.78–0.95)
0.79 (0.70–0.90)
crWeight at 4 months old
0.90 (0.84–0.97)
0.96 (0.83–1.10)
0.88 (0.77-1.00)
0.89 (0.78–1.03)
0.86 (0.76–0.97)
0.84 (0.72–0.98)
cLength at 7 months old
1.07 (1.00-1.14)
1.14 (0.99–1.30)
1.13 (0.99–1.28)
1.08 (0.94–1.24)
1.05 (0.95–1.18)
1.01 (0.89–1.18)
crWeight at 7 months old
0.98 (0.89–1.07)
0.81 (0.65-1.00)
0.91 (0.74–1.10)
0.90 (0.72–1.10)
1.00 (0.85–1.15)
1.03 (0.86–1.22)
cLength at 10 months olda
0.98 (0.90–1.07)
0.91 (0.77–1.08)
1.13 (0.95–1.33)
1.00 (0.83–1.20)
1.10 (0.94–1.27)
0.87 (0.73–1.05)
crWeight at 10 months oldb
0.94 (0.87–1.03)
0.94 (0.83–1.16)
0.90 (0.81–1.05)
1.02 (0.85–1.33)
0.96 (0.84–1.17)
0.97 (0.81–1.26)
[Model 2] Conditional variables at birth, 4 and 7 months old
Total
Communication
Gross motor
Fine motor
Problem solving
Personal-Social
2,281/13,689 (16.7 %)
612/13,757 (4.4 %)
698/13,759 (5.1 %)
614/13,753 (4.5 %)
814/13,742 (5.9 %)
481/13,722 (3.5 %)
Length at birth
1.02 (0.97–1.07)
1.00 (0.90–1.10)
1.03 (0.94–1.14)
1.01 (0.91–1.12)
1.00 (0.91–1.09)
1.04 (0.93–1.18)
Weight at birth
0.96 (0.91–1.01)
0.96 (0.87–1.07)
0.94 (0.85–1.03)
0.97 (0.87–1.07)
0.94 (0.86–1.03)
0.90 (0.80–1.01)
cLength at 4 months old
0.93 (0.89–0.97)
0.88 (0.80–0.97)
0.97 (0.89–1.06)
0.85 (0.78–0.93)
0.89 (0.82–0.97)
0.81 (0.73–0.90)
crWeight at 4 months old
0.90 (0.85–0.95)
0.88 (0.79–0.99)
0.89 (0.80–0.99)
0.91 (0.81–1.01)
0.85 (0.77–0.93)
0.90 (0.79–1.01)
cLength at 7 months old
1.08 (1.02–1.14)
1.09 (0.97–1.23)
1.14 (1.02–1.27)
1.16 (1.03–1.29)
1.11 (1.00-1.23)
1.05 (0.95–1.19)
crWeight at 7 months old
1.00 (0.92–1.08)
0.87 (0.72–1.04)
0.99 (0.84–1.15)
0.94 (0.79–1.10)
0.99 (0.86–1.11)
1.15 (0.99–1.31)
[Model 3] Conditional variables at birth, 4 and 10 months old
Total
Communication
Gross motor
Fine motor
Problem solving
Personal-Social
3,478/21,577 (16.1 %)
910/21,687 (4.2 %)
1086/21,684 (5.0 %)
923/21,677 (4.3 %)
1,232/21,656 (5.7 %)
723/21,626 (3.3 %)
Length at birth
1.00 (0.96–1.04)
0.93 (0.85–1.01)
0.98 (0.91–1.06)
0.99 (0.91–1.08)
1.02 (0.95–1.10)
0.97 (0.88–1.07)
Weight at birth
0.97 (0.93–1.01)
0.98 (0.90–1.06)
0.98 (0.91–1.06)
0.99 (0.91–1.08)
0.93 (0.87-1.00)
0.92 (0.83–1.01)
cLength at 4 months old
0.92 (0.89–0.95)
0.85 (0.79–0.92)
0.97 (0.90–1.04)
0.85 (0.79–0.91)
0.87 (0.82–0.93)
0.83 (0.76–0.90)
crWeight at 4 months old
0.92 (0.88–0.96)
0.94 (0.85–1.03)
0.91 (0.84-1.00)
0.89 (0.82–0.98)
0.86 (0.80–0.93)
0.87 (0.79–0.96)
cLength at 10 months olda
1.00 (0.96–1.05)
0.91 (0.85–0.99)
1.00 (0.92–1.09)
1.00 (0.91–1.09)
1.05 (0.97–1.14)
1.01 (0.92–1.11)
crWeight at 10 months oldb
1.02 (0.97–1.07)
1.01 (0.92–1.11)
0.91 (0.85–0.99)
0.96 (0.88–1.06)
1.01 (0.93–1.10)
1.08 (0.98–1.17)
Data represent the adjusted relative risk (95 % confidence interval). All of the conditional variables and the following covariates were included together in the model: maternal age and smoking, maternal and paternal education, family income, and home speech stimulation. a,bThe values of the variables were different between the models because these variables were estimated from different combinations of the previous data ([model 1] at birth, 4 and 7 months old; [model 3] at birth and 4 months old).
cLength conditional length; crWeight conditional relative weight
Table 5
Association between conditional variables and neurodevelopmental delay at 12 months old in girls
[Model 1] Conditional variables at birth, 4, 7 and 10 months old
Total
Communication
Gross motor
Fine motor
Problem solving
Personal-Social
 
1,117/8,211 (13.6 %)
199/8,253 (2.4 %)
540/8,254 (6.5 %)
235/8,254 (2.8 %)
348/8,244 (4.2 %)
202/8,229 (2.5 %)
Length at birth
1.04 (0.97–1.12)
0.92 (0.77–1.11)
1.05 (0.94–1.17)
1.05 (0.88–1.24)
1.01 (0.88–1.16)
0.99 (0.82–1.18)
Weight at birth
0.89 (0.83–0.96)
0.96 (0.80–1.16)
0.87 (0.78–0.97)
0.87 (0.73–1.03)
0.86 (0.75–0.99)
0.94 (0.78–1.13)
cLength at 4 months old
0.90 (0.84–0.96)
0.85 (0.73–1.01)
1.02 (0.92–1.12)
0.77 (0.66–0.90)
0.82 (0.73–0.93)
0.78 (0.67–0.92)
crWeight at 4 months old
0.96 (0.89–1.04)
0.80 (0.66–0.97)
0.94 (0.84–1.05)
0.97 (0.81–1.16)
0.91 (0.79–1.06)
0.88 (0.72–1.06)
cLength at 7 months old
1.16 (1.07–1.26)
1.17 (0.95–1.43)
1.21 (1.08–1.37)
1.06 (0.91–1.29)
1.14 (0.98–1.33)
1.10 (0.91–1.35)
crWeight at 7 months old
1.10 (0.98–1.23)
0.91 (0.67–1.20)
1.19 (1.00-1.39)
0.92 (0.70–1.21)
1.01 (0.81–1.25)
0.98 (0.73–1.28)
cLength at 10 months olda
0.97 (0.88–1.08)
0.94 (0.73–1.21)
1.04 (0.89–1.21)
1.04 (0.82–1.32)
1.07 (0.88–1.29)
0.87 (0.69–1.12)
crWeight at 10 months oldb
1.02 (0.91–1.18)
0.87 (0.73–1.17)
1.03 (0.87–1.27)
1.17 (0.86–1.65)
1.23 (0.94–1.63)
0.90 (0.75–1.23)
[Model 2] Conditional variables at birth, 4 and 7 months old
Total
Communication
Gross motor
Fine motor
Problem solving
Personal-Social
1,751/13,220 (13.2 %)
313/13,289 (2.4 %)
810/13,293 (6.1 %)
371/13,292 (2.8 %)
544/13,278 (4.1 %)
317/13,250 (2.4 %)
Length at birth
1.01 (0.95–1.07)
0.93 (0.80–1.08)
1.06 (0.97–1.16)
1.01 (0.88–1.16)
0.94 (0.84–1.05)
0.98 (0.85–1.14)
Weight at birth
0.94 (0.88–0.99)
0.95 (0.82–1.10)
0.86 (0.78–0.94)
0.96 (0.84–1.10)
0.96 (0.86–1.07)
0.95 (0.82–1.10)
cLength at 4 months old
0.90 (0.86–0.95)
0.85 (0.75–0.97)
1.01 (0.94–1.10)
0.83 (0.74–0.94)
0.82 (0.75–0.91)
0.81 (0.71–0.92)
crWeight at 4 months old
0.93 (0.88–0.99)
0.78 (0.67–0.92)
0.92 (0.83–1.01)
0.96 (0.84–1.11)
0.89 (0.79–0.99)
0.94 (0.81–1.09)
cLength at 7 months old
1.17 (1.10–1.26)
1.13 (0.96–1.35)
1.22 (1.10–1.35)
1.15 (0.98–1.34)
1.14 (1.01–1.30)
1.07 (0.92–1.27)
crWeight at 7 months old
1.10 (0.99–1.21)
0.89 (0.72–1.13)
1.19 (1.03–1.37)
0.94 (0.76–1.18)
1.07 (0.89–1.27)
0.98 (0.78–1.24)
[Model 3] Conditional variables at birth, 4 and 10 months old
Total
Communication
Gross motor
Fine motor
Problem solving
Personal-Social
2,669/20,756 (12.9 %)
441/20,857 (2.1 %)
1,263/20,864 (6.1 %)
537/20,858 (2.6 %)
827/20,837 (4.0 %)
463/20,799 (2.2 %)
Length at birth
1.02 (0.97–1.07)
0.95 (0.84–1.07)
1.03 (0.95–1.10)
1.03 (0.92–1.15)
1.03 (0.94–1.12)
1.04 (0.92–1.17)
Weight at birth
0.92 (0.88–0.97)
0.95 (0.84–1.08)
0.92 (0.85–0.99)
0.89 (0.79–0.99)
0.91 (0.83–0.99)
0.87 (0.77–0.98)
cLength at 4 months old
0.91 (0.88–0.95)
0.85 (0.77–0.95)
0.95 (0.89–1.01)
0.88 (0.80–0.97)
0.86 (0.80–0.93)
0.87 (0.78–0.97)
crWeight at 4 months old
0.98 (0.93–1.03)
0.86 (0.76–0.99)
0.98 (0.91–1.06)
0.94 (0.83–1.06)
0.96 (0.87–1.05)
0.88 (0.78–1.01)
cLength at 10 months olda
1.06 (1.01–1.12)
0.99 (0.88–1.13)
1.06 (0.98–1.15)
1.08 (0.96–1.22)
1.15 (1.04–1.26)
1.02 (0.90–1.16)
crWeight at 10 months oldb
1.05 (0.99–1.13)
0.98 (0.84–1.15)
1.04 (0.94–1.14)
0.96 (0.85–1.11)
1.05 (0.93–1.18)
0.95 (0.83–1.11)
Data represent the adjusted relative risk (95 % confidence interval). All of the conditional variables and the following covariates were included together in the model: maternal age and smoking, maternal and paternal education, family income, and home speech stimulation. a,bThe values of the variables were different between the models because these variables were estimated from different combinations of the previous data ([model 1] at birth, 4 and 7 months old; [model 3] at birth and 4 months old).
cLength conditional length; crWeight conditional relative weight

Discussion

The present study investigated the relationship between physical growth and neurodevelopmental delay during the first year of life in term-born infants. As indices of physical growth, we discriminated linear growth from relative weight gain, represented as ‘conditional length’ and ‘conditional relative weight’, respectively. A reduced risk of neurodevelopmental delay at six months old was observed in boys and girls with a higher birth weight and an increased conditional length at four months old. A reduced risk of neurodevelopmental delay at 12 months old was found in boys and girls with a higher conditional length at 4 months old, boys with a relatively high conditional relative weight at 4 months old and girls with a higher birth weight. These findings indicate that children with a poor growth in early infancy are at an increased risk for neurodevelopmental delay. In contrast, conditional variables at 7 and 10 months old did not increase the risk.
This was one of the largest birth cohort studies yet conducted to examine the association between physical growth and the neurodevelopmental outcome. Our large sample size had strong statistical power and revealed subtle but significant associations. Furthermore, body sizes were converted to z-scores using domestically standardized growth curves, and the sizes at birth were further adjusted for the mother’s parity and child’s gestational age, enabling the precise estimation of the body growth in utero. However, the present study included several limitations. First, the information was mainly collected via self-administered questionnaires. Although the anthropometric data were based on official records, the identified neurodevelopmental delay might be somewhat equivocal, relying on the parent-reported screening test of the Japanese version of ASQ-3. Next, a considerable proportion of children had missing information on physical growth at different time points and neurodevelopmental outcomes and were thus excluded from the analysis, possibly producing selection bias. Finally, the results of our regression analyses were not adjusted for other unmeasured confounders, including the maternal intelligent quotient, which greatly influences the neurocognitive development of offspring.
We found that a higher conditional length at 4 months old was consistently associated with a reduced risk of neurodevelopmental delay at 6 and 12 months of age for both sexes. The results agree with the findings from previous studies using conditional modeling, which reported a greater impact of linear growth than relative weight gain on schooling and intelligence [19, 20]. The biological mechanisms underlying this association remains unclear but is likely multifactorial. Prenatal and postnatal nutritional deficiency can induce growth failure of the body and brain, as observed in low-income countries [36]. Although severe malnutrition is less likely to occur in Japan than in other countries, placental dysfunction and maternal excessive dieting may cause a poor nutrient supply to the fetus, while inappropriate feeding habits might produce nutritional insufficiency during infancy. Another possible explanation is hormonal factors, such as the growth hormone/insulin-like growth factor-I pathway and the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis. These endocrine systems are regulated by nutritional and genetic factors [3741] and play key roles in both the physical and cerebral growth of the fetus and child [4246]. Subtle differences in such hormonal levels might be responsible for the association between physical growth and neurodevelopment.
We also found other associations between physical growth and neurodevelopmental delay during infancy. The association between a higher birth weight and a reduced risk of neurodevelopmental delay at 6 and 12 months old is concordant with the well-known finding that birth weight is a significant determinant of later neurodevelopment [1, 4749]. The boy-specific association between the conditional relative weight at 4 months and neurodevelopmental delay at 12 months was in line with the association between conditional relative weight and school achievement reported by Adair et al. [20]. The aforementioned endocrine factors have differential effects on somatic and cognitive development between sexes [46, 50] and might account at least partially for this association. Conversely, higher conditional variables at 7 and 10 months old did not reduce the risk of neurodevelopmental delay at 12 months, suggesting less susceptibility of the developing brain in late infancy than in early infancy. An increased risk at 12 months was also paradoxically observed in girls with an increased conditional length at either 7 or 10 months old in all the regression models. When an infant shows slow growth in early infancy, they may be inclined to be fed more by their parents and thus may grow faster thereafter. Such modification of parents’ feeding behavior might yield this paradoxical association, although this information was not collected in the JECS.

Conclusions

Using appropriate conditional modeling, we showed that poor early physical growth was associated with an increased risk of neurodevelopment delay during the first year of life in term-born infants partly in a sex-dependent manner. If a child presents with growth failure in early infancy, the infant should be monitored carefully to detect neurodevelopmental delay for early intervention. Ongoing investigations in the JECS cohort may reveal later-term outcomes.

Acknowledgements

We thank all participants of the JECS and all staff members involved in data collection. The findings and conclusions of this article are the sole responsibility of the authors and do not represent the official views of the Japanese government. Members of the JECS Group as of 2020: Michihiro Kamijima (principal investigator; Nagoya City University, Nagoya, Japan), Shin Yamazaki (National Institute for Environmental Studies, Tsukuba, Japan), Yukihiro Ohya (National Center for Child Health and Development, Tokyo, Japan), Reiko Kishi (Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan), Nobuo Yaegashi (Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan), Koichi Hashimoto (Fukushima Medical University, Fukushima, Japan), Chisato Mori (Chiba University, Chiba, Japan), Shuichi Ito (Yokohama City University, Yokohama, Japan), Zentaro Yamagata (University of Yamanashi, Chuo, Japan), Hidekuni Inadera (University of Toyama, Toyama, Japan), Takeo Nakayama (Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan), Hiroyasu Iso (Osaka University, Suita, Japan), Masayuki Shima (Hyogo College of Medicine, Nishinomiya, Japan), Youichi Kurozawa (Tottori University, Yonago, Japan), Narufumi Suganuma (Kochi University, Nankoku, Japan), Koichi Kusuhara (University of Occupational and Environmental Health, Kitakyushu, Japan), and Takahiko Katoh (Kumamoto University, Kumamoto, Japan).

Declarations

The JECS protocol was reviewed and approved by the Ministry of Environment’s Institutional Review Board for Epidemiological Studies and by the ethics committees of all participating institutions (#15000141). The ethical approval for this study was an extension of the ethical approval for the JECS protocol. Written informed consent was obtained from all parents.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Anhänge

Supplementary Information

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Walker DM, Marlow N. Neurocognitive outcome following fetal growth restriction. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2008;93:F322-5.PubMed Walker DM, Marlow N. Neurocognitive outcome following fetal growth restriction. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2008;93:F322-5.PubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Sutton PS, Darmstadt GL. Preterm birth and neurodevelopment: a review of outcomes and recommendations for early identification and cost-effective interventions. J Trop Pediatr. 2013;59:258–65.PubMedCrossRef Sutton PS, Darmstadt GL. Preterm birth and neurodevelopment: a review of outcomes and recommendations for early identification and cost-effective interventions. J Trop Pediatr. 2013;59:258–65.PubMedCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Guellec I, Lapillonne A, Marret S, Picaud JC, Mitanchez D, Charkaluk ML, et al. Effect of intra- and extrauterine growth on long-term neurologic outcomes of very preterm infants. J Pediatr. 2016;175:93–9 e91.PubMedCrossRef Guellec I, Lapillonne A, Marret S, Picaud JC, Mitanchez D, Charkaluk ML, et al. Effect of intra- and extrauterine growth on long-term neurologic outcomes of very preterm infants. J Pediatr. 2016;175:93–9 e91.PubMedCrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Taine M, Charles MA, Beltrand J, Roze JC, Leger J, Botton J, et al. Early postnatal growth and neurodevelopment in children born moderately preterm or small for gestational age at term: a systematic review. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2018;32:268–80.PubMedCrossRef Taine M, Charles MA, Beltrand J, Roze JC, Leger J, Botton J, et al. Early postnatal growth and neurodevelopment in children born moderately preterm or small for gestational age at term: a systematic review. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2018;32:268–80.PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruys CA, Hollanders JJ, Broring T, van Schie PEM, van der Pal SM, van de Lagemaat M, et al. Early-life growth of preterm infants and its impact on neurodevelopment. Pediatr Res. 2019;85:283–92.PubMedCrossRef Ruys CA, Hollanders JJ, Broring T, van Schie PEM, van der Pal SM, van de Lagemaat M, et al. Early-life growth of preterm infants and its impact on neurodevelopment. Pediatr Res. 2019;85:283–92.PubMedCrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Martorell R, Horta BL, Adair LS, Stein AD, Richter L, Fall CH, et al. Weight gain in the first two years of life is an important predictor of schooling outcomes in pooled analyses from five birth cohorts from low- and middle-income countries. J Nutr. 2010;140:348–54.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Martorell R, Horta BL, Adair LS, Stein AD, Richter L, Fall CH, et al. Weight gain in the first two years of life is an important predictor of schooling outcomes in pooled analyses from five birth cohorts from low- and middle-income countries. J Nutr. 2010;140:348–54.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Heinonen K, Raikkonen K, Pesonen AK, Kajantie E, Andersson S, Eriksson JG, et al. Prenatal and postnatal growth and cognitive abilities at 56 months of age: a longitudinal study of infants born at term. Pediatrics. 2008;121:e1325-33.PubMedCrossRef Heinonen K, Raikkonen K, Pesonen AK, Kajantie E, Andersson S, Eriksson JG, et al. Prenatal and postnatal growth and cognitive abilities at 56 months of age: a longitudinal study of infants born at term. Pediatrics. 2008;121:e1325-33.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Hamadani JD, Tofail F, Huda SN, Alam DS, Ridout DA, Attanasio O, et al. Cognitive deficit and poverty in the first 5 years of childhood in Bangladesh. Pediatrics. 2014;134:e1001-8.PubMedCrossRef Hamadani JD, Tofail F, Huda SN, Alam DS, Ridout DA, Attanasio O, et al. Cognitive deficit and poverty in the first 5 years of childhood in Bangladesh. Pediatrics. 2014;134:e1001-8.PubMedCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Pongcharoen T, Ramakrishnan U, DiGirolamo AM, Winichagoon P, Flores R, Singkhornard J, et al. Influence of prenatal and postnatal growth on intellectual functioning in school-aged children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166:411–6.PubMedCrossRef Pongcharoen T, Ramakrishnan U, DiGirolamo AM, Winichagoon P, Flores R, Singkhornard J, et al. Influence of prenatal and postnatal growth on intellectual functioning in school-aged children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166:411–6.PubMedCrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Emond AM, Blair PS, Emmett PM, Drewett RF. Weight faltering in infancy and IQ levels at 8 years in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Pediatrics. 2007;120:e1051-8.PubMedCrossRef Emond AM, Blair PS, Emmett PM, Drewett RF. Weight faltering in infancy and IQ levels at 8 years in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Pediatrics. 2007;120:e1051-8.PubMedCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Lasky RE, Klein RE, Yarbrough C, Engle PL, Lechtig A, Martorell R. The relationship between physical growth and infant behavioral development in rural Guatemala. Child Dev. 1981;52:219–26.PubMedCrossRef Lasky RE, Klein RE, Yarbrough C, Engle PL, Lechtig A, Martorell R. The relationship between physical growth and infant behavioral development in rural Guatemala. Child Dev. 1981;52:219–26.PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Skuse D, Pickles A, Wolke D, Reilly S. Postnatal growth and mental development: evidence for a “sensitive period”. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1994;35:521–45. Skuse D, Pickles A, Wolke D, Reilly S. Postnatal growth and mental development: evidence for a “sensitive period”. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1994;35:521–45.
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Horta BL, Sibbritt DW, Lima RC, Victora CG. Weight catch-up and achieved schooling at 18 years of age in Brazilian males. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2009;63:369–74.PubMedCrossRef Horta BL, Sibbritt DW, Lima RC, Victora CG. Weight catch-up and achieved schooling at 18 years of age in Brazilian males. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2009;63:369–74.PubMedCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Raikkonen K, Forsen T, Henriksson M, Kajantie E, Heinonen K, Pesonen AK, et al. Growth trajectories and intellectual abilities in young adulthood: the Helsinki Birth Cohort study. Am J Epidemiol. 2009;170:447–55.PubMedCrossRef Raikkonen K, Forsen T, Henriksson M, Kajantie E, Heinonen K, Pesonen AK, et al. Growth trajectories and intellectual abilities in young adulthood: the Helsinki Birth Cohort study. Am J Epidemiol. 2009;170:447–55.PubMedCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Belfort MB, Rifas-Shiman SL, Rich-Edwards JW, Kleinman KP, Oken E, Gillman MW. Infant growth and child cognition at 3 years of age. Pediatrics. 2008;122:e689-95.PubMedCrossRef Belfort MB, Rifas-Shiman SL, Rich-Edwards JW, Kleinman KP, Oken E, Gillman MW. Infant growth and child cognition at 3 years of age. Pediatrics. 2008;122:e689-95.PubMedCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Beyerlein A, Ness AR, Streuling I, Hadders-Algra M, von Kries R. Early rapid growth: no association with later cognitive functions in children born not small for gestational age. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;92:585–93.PubMedCrossRef Beyerlein A, Ness AR, Streuling I, Hadders-Algra M, von Kries R. Early rapid growth: no association with later cognitive functions in children born not small for gestational age. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;92:585–93.PubMedCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Benzies KM, Magill-Evans JE, Hayden KA, Ballantyne M. Key components of early intervention programs for preterm infants and their parents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2013;13(Suppl 1):10.CrossRef Benzies KM, Magill-Evans JE, Hayden KA, Ballantyne M. Key components of early intervention programs for preterm infants and their parents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2013;13(Suppl 1):10.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Cioni G, Inguaggiato E, Sgandurra G. Early intervention in neurodevelopmental disorders: underlying neural mechanisms. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2016;58(Suppl 4):61–6.PubMedCrossRef Cioni G, Inguaggiato E, Sgandurra G. Early intervention in neurodevelopmental disorders: underlying neural mechanisms. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2016;58(Suppl 4):61–6.PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Horta BL, Victora CG, de Mola CL, Quevedo L, Pinheiro RT, Gigante DP, et al. Associations of linear growth and relative weight gain in early life with human capital at 30 years of age. J Pediatr. 2017;182:85–91 e83. Horta BL, Victora CG, de Mola CL, Quevedo L, Pinheiro RT, Gigante DP, et al. Associations of linear growth and relative weight gain in early life with human capital at 30 years of age. J Pediatr. 2017;182:85–91 e83.
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Adair LS, Fall CH, Osmond C, Stein AD, Martorell R, Ramirez-Zea M, et al. Associations of linear growth and relative weight gain during early life with adult health and human capital in countries of low and middle income: findings from five birth cohort studies. Lancet. 2013;382:525–34.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Adair LS, Fall CH, Osmond C, Stein AD, Martorell R, Ramirez-Zea M, et al. Associations of linear growth and relative weight gain during early life with adult health and human capital in countries of low and middle income: findings from five birth cohort studies. Lancet. 2013;382:525–34.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Keijzer-Veen MG, Euser AM, van Montfoort N, Dekker FW, Vandenbroucke JP, Van Houwelingen HC. A regression model with unexplained residuals was preferred in the analysis of the fetal origins of adult diseases hypothesis. J Clin Epidemiol. 2005;58:1320–4.PubMedCrossRef Keijzer-Veen MG, Euser AM, van Montfoort N, Dekker FW, Vandenbroucke JP, Van Houwelingen HC. A regression model with unexplained residuals was preferred in the analysis of the fetal origins of adult diseases hypothesis. J Clin Epidemiol. 2005;58:1320–4.PubMedCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Li H, Stein AD, Barnhart HX, Ramakrishnan U, Martorell R. Associations between prenatal and postnatal growth and adult body size and composition. Am J Clin Nutr. 2003;77:1498–505.PubMedCrossRef Li H, Stein AD, Barnhart HX, Ramakrishnan U, Martorell R. Associations between prenatal and postnatal growth and adult body size and composition. Am J Clin Nutr. 2003;77:1498–505.PubMedCrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Kawamoto T, Nitta H, Murata K, Toda E, Tsukamoto N, Hasegawa M, et al. Rationale and study design of the Japan environment and children’s study (JECS). BMC Public Health. 2014;14:25.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kawamoto T, Nitta H, Murata K, Toda E, Tsukamoto N, Hasegawa M, et al. Rationale and study design of the Japan environment and children’s study (JECS). BMC Public Health. 2014;14:25.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Michikawa T, Nitta H, Nakayama SF, Yamazaki S, Isobe T, Tamura K, et al. Baseline profile of participants in the Japan Environment and Children’s Study (JECS). J Epidemiol. 2018;28:99–104.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Michikawa T, Nitta H, Nakayama SF, Yamazaki S, Isobe T, Tamura K, et al. Baseline profile of participants in the Japan Environment and Children’s Study (JECS). J Epidemiol. 2018;28:99–104.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang X, Tilling K, Martin RM, Oken E, Naimi AI, Aris IM, et al. Analysis of ‘sensitive’ periods of fetal and child growth. Int J Epidemiol. 2019;48:116–23.PubMedCrossRef Zhang X, Tilling K, Martin RM, Oken E, Naimi AI, Aris IM, et al. Analysis of ‘sensitive’ periods of fetal and child growth. Int J Epidemiol. 2019;48:116–23.PubMedCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Tanaka T, Yokoya S, Kato N, Ito Y, Tachibana K, Sugihara S, et al. Basic view for assessment of physical size in Japanese children. J Jpn Pediatr Soc. 2011;115:1705–9. Tanaka T, Yokoya S, Kato N, Ito Y, Tachibana K, Sugihara S, et al. Basic view for assessment of physical size in Japanese children. J Jpn Pediatr Soc. 2011;115:1705–9.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Squires J, Elizabeth T, Bricker D, Potter L. ASQ-3 User’s Guide. Baltimore: Brookes Publishing; 2009. Squires J, Elizabeth T, Bricker D, Potter L. ASQ-3 User’s Guide. Baltimore: Brookes Publishing; 2009.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Limbos MM, Joyce DP. Comparison of the ASQ and PEDS in screening for developmental delay in children presenting for primary care. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2011;32:499–511.PubMedCrossRef Limbos MM, Joyce DP. Comparison of the ASQ and PEDS in screening for developmental delay in children presenting for primary care. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2011;32:499–511.PubMedCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Schonhaut L, Armijo I, Schonstedt M, Alvarez J, Cordero M. Validity of the Ages and Stages Questionnaires in term and preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2013;131:e1468-74.PubMedCrossRef Schonhaut L, Armijo I, Schonstedt M, Alvarez J, Cordero M. Validity of the Ages and Stages Questionnaires in term and preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2013;131:e1468-74.PubMedCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Lopes S, Graca P, Teixeira S, Serrano AM, Squires J. Psychometric properties and validation of Portuguese version of Ages & Stages Questionnaires (3rd edition): 9, 18 and 30 Questionnaires. Early Hum Dev. 2015;91:527–33.PubMedCrossRef Lopes S, Graca P, Teixeira S, Serrano AM, Squires J. Psychometric properties and validation of Portuguese version of Ages & Stages Questionnaires (3rd edition): 9, 18 and 30 Questionnaires. Early Hum Dev. 2015;91:527–33.PubMedCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Bernard JY, De Agostini M, Forhan A, Alfaiate T, Bonet M, Champion V, et al. Breastfeeding duration and cognitive development at 2 and 3 years of age in the EDEN mother-child cohort. J Pediatr 2013;163:36–42 e31. Bernard JY, De Agostini M, Forhan A, Alfaiate T, Bonet M, Champion V, et al. Breastfeeding duration and cognitive development at 2 and 3 years of age in the EDEN mother-child cohort. J Pediatr 2013;163:36–42 e31.
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Filgueiras A, Pires P, Maissonette S, Landeira-Fernandez J. Psychometric properties of the Brazilian-adapted version of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire in public child daycare centers. Early Hum Dev. 2013;89:561–76.PubMedCrossRef Filgueiras A, Pires P, Maissonette S, Landeira-Fernandez J. Psychometric properties of the Brazilian-adapted version of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire in public child daycare centers. Early Hum Dev. 2013;89:561–76.PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Mezawa H, Aoki S, Nakayama SF, Nitta H, Ikeda N, Kato K, et al. Psychometric profile of the Ages and Stages Questionnaires, Japanese translation. Pediatr Int. 2019;61:1086–95.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mezawa H, Aoki S, Nakayama SF, Nitta H, Ikeda N, Kato K, et al. Psychometric profile of the Ages and Stages Questionnaires, Japanese translation. Pediatr Int. 2019;61:1086–95.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. Home observation for measurement of the environment. Little Rock: University of Arkansas; 1984. Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. Home observation for measurement of the environment. Little Rock: University of Arkansas; 1984.
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Martorell R. Improved nutrition in the first 1000 days and adult human capital and health. Am J Hum Biol. 2017;29:e22952.CrossRef Martorell R. Improved nutrition in the first 1000 days and adult human capital and health. Am J Hum Biol. 2017;29:e22952.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Xia K, Yu Y, Ahn M, Zhu H, Zou F, Gilmore JH, et al. Environmental and genetic contributors to salivary testosterone levels in infants. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne). 2014;5:187.CrossRef Xia K, Yu Y, Ahn M, Zhu H, Zou F, Gilmore JH, et al. Environmental and genetic contributors to salivary testosterone levels in infants. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne). 2014;5:187.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Hawkes CP, Grimberg A. Insulin-like growth factor-I is a marker for the nutritional state. Pediatr Endocrinol Rev. 2015;13:499–511.PubMedPubMedCentral Hawkes CP, Grimberg A. Insulin-like growth factor-I is a marker for the nutritional state. Pediatr Endocrinol Rev. 2015;13:499–511.PubMedPubMedCentral
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Gluckman PD, Gunn AJ, Wray A, Cutfield WS, Chatelain PG, Guilbaud O, et al. Congenital idiopathic growth hormone deficiency associated with prenatal and early postnatal growth failure. The International Board of the Kabi Pharmacia International Growth Study. J Pediatr. 1992;121:920–3.PubMedCrossRef Gluckman PD, Gunn AJ, Wray A, Cutfield WS, Chatelain PG, Guilbaud O, et al. Congenital idiopathic growth hormone deficiency associated with prenatal and early postnatal growth failure. The International Board of the Kabi Pharmacia International Growth Study. J Pediatr. 1992;121:920–3.PubMedCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Woods KA, Camacho-Hubner C, Savage MO, Clark AJ. Intrauterine growth retardation and postnatal growth failure associated with deletion of the insulin-like growth factor I gene. N Engl J Med. 1996;335:1363–7.PubMedCrossRef Woods KA, Camacho-Hubner C, Savage MO, Clark AJ. Intrauterine growth retardation and postnatal growth failure associated with deletion of the insulin-like growth factor I gene. N Engl J Med. 1996;335:1363–7.PubMedCrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Abuzzahab MJ, Schneider A, Goddard A, Grigorescu F, Lautier C, Keller E, et al. IGF-I receptor mutations resulting in intrauterine and postnatal growth retardation. N Engl J Med. 2003;349:2211–22.PubMedCrossRef Abuzzahab MJ, Schneider A, Goddard A, Grigorescu F, Lautier C, Keller E, et al. IGF-I receptor mutations resulting in intrauterine and postnatal growth retardation. N Engl J Med. 2003;349:2211–22.PubMedCrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat de Bie HM, Oostrom KJ, Delemarre-van de Waal HA. Brain development, intelligence and cognitive outcome in children born small for gestational age. Horm Res Paediatr. 2010;73:6–14.PubMedCrossRef de Bie HM, Oostrom KJ, Delemarre-van de Waal HA. Brain development, intelligence and cognitive outcome in children born small for gestational age. Horm Res Paediatr. 2010;73:6–14.PubMedCrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat van Pareren YK, Duivenvoorden HJ, Slijper FS, Koot HM, Hokken-Koelega AC. Intelligence and psychosocial functioning during long-term growth hormone therapy in children born small for gestational age. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2004;89:5295–302.PubMedCrossRef van Pareren YK, Duivenvoorden HJ, Slijper FS, Koot HM, Hokken-Koelega AC. Intelligence and psychosocial functioning during long-term growth hormone therapy in children born small for gestational age. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2004;89:5295–302.PubMedCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Chaplin JE, Kristrom B, Jonsson B, Tuvemo T, Albertsson-Wikland K. Growth hormone treatment improves cognitive function in short children with growth hormone deficiency. Horm Res Paediatr. 2015;83:390–9.CrossRef Chaplin JE, Kristrom B, Jonsson B, Tuvemo T, Albertsson-Wikland K. Growth hormone treatment improves cognitive function in short children with growth hormone deficiency. Horm Res Paediatr. 2015;83:390–9.CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Gunnell D, Miller LL, Rogers I, Holly JM, Team AS. Association of insulin-like growth factor I and insulin-like growth factor-binding protein-3 with intelligence quotient among 8- to 9-year-old children in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Pediatrics. 2005;116:e681-6.PubMedCrossRef Gunnell D, Miller LL, Rogers I, Holly JM, Team AS. Association of insulin-like growth factor I and insulin-like growth factor-binding protein-3 with intelligence quotient among 8- to 9-year-old children in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Pediatrics. 2005;116:e681-6.PubMedCrossRef
46.
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Pryor J, Silva PA, Brooke M. Growth, development and behaviour in adolescents born small-for-gestational-age. J Paediatr Child Health. 1995;31:403–7.PubMedCrossRef Pryor J, Silva PA, Brooke M. Growth, development and behaviour in adolescents born small-for-gestational-age. J Paediatr Child Health. 1995;31:403–7.PubMedCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Geva R, Eshel R, Leitner Y, Valevski AF, Harel S. Neuropsychological outcome of children with intrauterine growth restriction: a 9-year prospective study. Pediatrics. 2006;118:91–100.PubMedCrossRef Geva R, Eshel R, Leitner Y, Valevski AF, Harel S. Neuropsychological outcome of children with intrauterine growth restriction: a 9-year prospective study. Pediatrics. 2006;118:91–100.PubMedCrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Keeffe MJ, O’Callaghan M, Williams GM, Najman JM, Bor W. Learning, cognitive, and attentional problems in adolescents born small for gestational age. Pediatrics. 2003;112:301–7.PubMedCrossRef O’Keeffe MJ, O’Callaghan M, Williams GM, Najman JM, Bor W. Learning, cognitive, and attentional problems in adolescents born small for gestational age. Pediatrics. 2003;112:301–7.PubMedCrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Mayer M, Schmitt K, Kapelari K, Frisch H, Kostl G, Voigt M. Spontaneous growth in growth hormone deficiency from birth until 7 years of age: development of disease-specific growth curves. Horm Res Paediatr. 2010;74:136–44.PubMedCrossRef Mayer M, Schmitt K, Kapelari K, Frisch H, Kostl G, Voigt M. Spontaneous growth in growth hormone deficiency from birth until 7 years of age: development of disease-specific growth curves. Horm Res Paediatr. 2010;74:136–44.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Physical growth and neurodevelopment during the first year of life: a cohort study of the Japan Environment and Children’s Study
verfasst von
Masafumi Sanefuji
Yuri Sonoda
Yoshiya Ito
Masanobu Ogawa
Vlad Tocan
Hirosuke Inoue
Masayuki Ochiai
Masayuki Shimono
Reiko Suga
Ayako Senju
Satoshi Honjo
Koichi Kusuhara
Shouichi Ohga
Japan Environment and Children’s Study Group
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2021
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Pediatrics / Ausgabe 1/2021
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2431
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-021-02815-9

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2021

BMC Pediatrics 1/2021 Zur Ausgabe

Neuer Typ-1-Diabetes bei Kindern am Wochenende eher übersehen

23.04.2024 Typ-1-Diabetes Nachrichten

Wenn Kinder an Werktagen zum Arzt gehen, werden neu auftretender Typ-1-Diabetes und diabetische Ketoazidosen häufiger erkannt als bei Arztbesuchen an Wochenenden oder Feiertagen.

Neue Studienergebnisse zur Myopiekontrolle mit Atropin

22.04.2024 Fehlsichtigkeit Nachrichten

Augentropfen mit niedrig dosiertem Atropin können helfen, das Fortschreiten einer Kurzsichtigkeit bei Kindern zumindest zu verlangsamen, wie die Ergebnisse einer aktuellen Studie mit verschiedenen Dosierungen zeigen.

Spinale Muskelatrophie: Neugeborenen-Screening lohnt sich

18.04.2024 Spinale Muskelatrophien Nachrichten

Seit 2021 ist die Untersuchung auf spinale Muskelatrophie Teil des Neugeborenen-Screenings in Deutschland. Eine Studie liefert weitere Evidenz für den Nutzen der Maßnahme.

Fünf Dinge, die im Kindernotfall besser zu unterlassen sind

18.04.2024 Pädiatrische Notfallmedizin Nachrichten

Im Choosing-Wisely-Programm, das für die deutsche Initiative „Klug entscheiden“ Pate gestanden hat, sind erstmals Empfehlungen zum Umgang mit Notfällen von Kindern erschienen. Fünf Dinge gilt es demnach zu vermeiden.

Update Pädiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.