Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Public Health 1/2022

Open Access 01.12.2022 | Research

Correlates of co-occurring physical child punishment and physical intimate partner violence in Colombia, Mexico and Peru

verfasst von: Sarah Bott, Ana P. Ruiz-Celis, Jennifer Adams Mendoza, Alessandra Guedes

Erschienen in: BMC Public Health | Ausgabe 1/2022

Abstract

Background

Violent discipline of children and intimate partner violence (IPV) against women are global public health and human rights problems. To address calls for more evidence on intersections, this study aimed to expand knowledge about correlates of physical child punishment, physical IPV against women and their co-occurrence (both) in the same household. 

Methods

Using national, population-based survey datasets from Colombia, Mexico and Peru, multinomial logistic regressions examined correlates of three mutually exclusive patterns of violence in the household: physical child punishment (only), physical IPV ever (only) and co-occurrence (both), each compared with no violence, after adjusting for other factors. Logistic regression was used to analyse odds ratios of physical child punishment in households affected by IPV past year and before past year compared with never, after adjusting for other factors.

Results

In all countries, adjusted odds ratios (aOR) of co-occurrence were significantly higher among women with lower education, more than one child, a child aged 2–5, a partner who tried to socially isolate her, and a history of childhood violence (caregiver violence and/or IPV exposure). They were significantly lower among women who reported collaborative partnerships (joint decision-making and/or shared chores). Co-occurrence was also significantly correlated with a history of child marriage/early motherhood in Colombia and Mexico, partner’s excess drinking in Mexico and Peru, agreement that physical child punishment was necessary in Peru and partner’s history of childhood violence in Colombia and Mexico. Evidence of shared risk factors was strongest for social isolation and caregiver histories of childhood violence and of shared protective factors for collaborative partnership dynamics. In all countries, associations between physical child punishment and physical IPV remained significant after adjusting for other factors, suggesting that correlations could not be explained by shared risk factors alone.

Conclusions

These findings are consistent with several theories relevant for violence prevention: 1) more collaborative, gender equitable partnerships may protect both children and women from violence; 2) violence between intimate partners may ‘spill over’ into violence against children (as correlations could not be explained by shared risk factors alone); and 3) there appears to be strong evidence of intergenerational transmission of violence.
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s12889-022-14453-6.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
aOR
Adjusted odds ratio
CI
Confidence interval
DHS
Demographic and Health Survey
DK
Don’t know
ENDIREH
Encuesta Nacional Sobre La Dinámica De Las Relaciones en Los Hogares
INEGI
Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía
IPV
Intimate partner violence
LAC
Latin America and the Caribbean
LMIC
Low- and middle-income countries
MICS
Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey
NM
Not measured
OR
Odds ratio
PSU
Primary sampling unit
Ref
Reference category
SDG
Sustainable Development Goal
UN
United Nations
VIF
Variance inflation factor

Background

The international community recognizes violent discipline of children and intimate partner violence (IPV) against women as global public health and human rights problems [1, 2]. As part of 2030 Sustainable Development Goal (SDG) commitments [3], all United Nations (UN) Member States agreed to work towards eliminating these forms of violence and measure progress through national data collection [4]. As of January 2021, Colombia and Peru had banned corporal punishment of children in all settings including the home, and Mexico had taken legislative steps towards full prohibition [5].
Violent discipline, defined by the UN as physical punishment and/or verbal aggression by caregivers, is the most common form of violence against children globally [6, 7]. Within Latin America and the Caribbean (LAC), an estimated 50–60% of 2–3 year old children are regularly physically punished, declining to 30–40% by age 14 [8]. Meanwhile, IPV is the most widespread form of violence against women [9], with national estimates of physical IPV (ever) against ever partnered women in LAC ranging from 7% in Uruguay to 52% in Bolivia [10].
Both violent discipline and exposure to IPV have negative consequences for children [1114]. Metanalyses have found associations between physical child punishment and cognitive, behavioural, mental health and emotional problems throughout the lifespan, even when physical punishment does not meet legal definitions of child abuse [13, 14]. Similarly, exposure to IPV has been linked to children’s emotional impairment and mental health disorders [15], malnutrition and stunting [16], aggression towards peers and siblings [17] and negative perinatal health outcomes [18].
Preliminary evidence suggests that co-occurrence —defined in this article as caregiver violence against children and IPV against women in the same household— may compound negative effects of either violent discipline or exposure to IPV alone [1921]. For example, a study from Uganda found that children who witnessed IPV and experienced violence themselves had about twice the odds of mental health difficulties as children who experienced violence but did not witness IPV [22].
Based on growing evidence of intersections between violence against children and violence against women [23], some question the feasibility of achieving the SDGs without a better understanding of the complex inter-relationships among factors associated with violence against children and violence against women [24]. A 2020 review identified 132 studies on correlates of co-occurring IPV and child maltreatment in the same household [21]; however, studies used enormously diverse research methods and definitions of violence, making findings difficult to compare. Moreover, the vast majority came from high-income settings. In fact, a 2022 systematic review found only three studies from low- and middle-income countries (LMICs) that explored risk factors for co-occurrence [25]. Studies from high-income countries may have limited generalizability to LMICs, both because drivers of violence may differ [26], and because studies from high-income settings often (though not always) focus on at-risk populations and/or severe cases of child or partner abuse reported to authorities, rather than common forms of violent discipline captured by population-based surveys [21, 27].
One barrier to understanding co-occurrence in LMICs is that most high quality, national data on violent discipline come from UNICEF-supported Multiple Indicator Cluster Surveys (MICS) [28], which do not usually measure IPV prevalence. As a result, the growing number of multi-country analyses of MICS data have not included child exposure to IPV as a potential risk factor in research on violent discipline [2936]. Conversely, most national surveys that measure IPV against women do not measure child discipline, with some exceptions, including the surveys in this study.
Nonetheless, research on co-occurrence in LMICs is growing. Some studies examine IPV against mothers as a correlate of violent discipline [3740]. Others examine correlates of co-occurrence itself [25, 26, 41, 42]. This research suggests several key findings and knowledge gaps. First, many studies find that children in households affected by IPV are more likely than other children to experience violent discipline [3740, 4345]. What pathways explain these associations are not entirely understood, however. Some researchers suggest abused women may discipline children more harshly due to depression, stress, anxiety, trauma or maternal alcohol abuse [26, 42, 4648]. Others theorize that men who abuse their partners may be more likely to abuse their children [19]. Some researchers propose a ‘spillover hypothesis’, whereby violent norms and behaviours in intimate partnerships spill over into caregiver-child relationships due to stress, social learning or attitudes normalizing violence [21, 27, 46, 49].
Others suggest that associations reflect shared risk factors that independently increase the risk of each form of violence [19, 23, 26]. Proposed shared risk factors include legal frameworks that fail to protect the rights of women or children [33, 5053], high levels of crime, armed violence or socio-economic disadvantages in communities [42, 5458], social norms supportive of violence or gender inequality within society [35, 50, 51] or households [23, 29, 31, 36, 5962], patriarchal family structures [46, 63]; men’s harmful use of alcohol [23, 6466], caregiver histories of violence in childhood [23, 37, 40, 55, 58, 64, 6774], and social isolation, including during COVID-19 lockdowns [7578].
Meanwhile, some correlates commonly explored in research on violence against children, including caregiver agreement that physical punishment is necessary for child rearing [29, 36, 59, 61], are rarely explored in IPV research. Conversely, some documented correlates of IPV such as partners’ controlling behaviour (e.g. preventing women from seeing friends and family) [69, 70, 79] and women’s histories of child marriage [80] have not been widely explored in research on violent discipline of children.
Another complexity is that gender inequality is widely theorized to be a driver of both violence against children and violence against women in LMICs by researchers [23, 46, 63, 81] and by international frameworks for violence prevention [82, 83], but measuring this domain poses challenges. For example, researchers sometimes use female participation in household decision-making as a proxy for women’s empowerment [84], but others question its validity [85]. Moreover, some studies have found higher levels of IPV among women who make household decisions alone than among women who report joint decision-making with their partner, suggesting that it may be collaborative rather than any female decision-making that is most protective for IPV [63, 65, 79, 86, 87]. In addition, researchers often find complex, nonlinear relationships between levels of violence and proxies for women’s empowerment such as education, wealth or earning power [63, 70, 81, 88].
To explore co-occurrence and possible shared risk factors, studies from Brazil [42] and Uganda [26] analysed correlates of three mutually exclusive patterns of violence in the household: caregiver violence against children (only), IPV against women (only), and both forms (co-occurrence). Using a population-based birth cohort of 3,533 mothers with four-year-old children, the Brazilian study found associations between co-occurrence and neighbourhood violence, absence of biological father, antisocial paternal behaviour, mother-partner relationships characterised by high levels of criticism, maternal depression and younger maternal age. Among 535 adolescent-caregiver dyads, the Ugandan study found associations between co-occurrence and women’s lower socioeconomic status, lower education, higher mental distress and frequent alcohol use, male caregiver support for physical child punishment and lower emotional attachment between partners. These studies focused on different child cohorts (four-year-olds versus adolescents) and used starkly different definitions of violence, making them hard to compare. Nonetheless, commonalities in their approach suggests a promising way to begin disentangling links between different forms of violence in the household.

Aims and research questions

Building on these studies from Brazil and Uganda, this article presents a secondary analysis of three national, population-based household surveys from Colombia, Mexico and Peru. This study aimed to expand what is known about correlates of IPV against women and physical punishment of children by addressing the following research questions:
  • 1. Which risk and protective factors were associated with co-occurring physical IPV against women and physical punishment of children in the same household?
  • 2. Which correlates were ‘shared’ across three patterns of physical violence in the household (physical child punishment only, physical IPV only, and co-occurrence), each compared with no violence?
  • 3. Is physical IPV against women in the household a significant risk factor for physical child punishment, after controlling for other factors?
Henceforth this article refers to women and girls aged 15–49 as ‘women’ for simplicity, while acknowledging that the Convention on the Rights of the Child considers girls below age 18 to be children not adults, even if they have already married or borne children [1].

Methods

Survey characteristics

This study involved a secondary analysis of three national, open access, population-based household surveys: the 2015 Colombia Demographic and Health Survey (DHS) [89], the 2016 Mexico Encuesta Nacional Sobre la Dinámica de las Relaciones en los Hogares (ENDIREH) [90] and the 2018 Peru DHS [91]. These surveys were identified through a systematic search, described elsewhere [43] as the only surveys from LAC that gathered national co-occurrence data among women of reproductive age between 2014 and 2019. All three were implemented through collaborations between national governments and civil society. The 2016 Mexico ENDIREH was dedicated specifically to violence against women. The 2015 Colombia DHS and 2018 Peru DHS were health surveys that included a domestic violence module. Henceforth each survey is referred to by country name.

Sample design

All three surveys used multi-stage, probability samples. Primary sampling units (PSUs) were selected from master sampling frames, usually a census. Households were randomly selected within each PSU. Full surveys included women aged 13–49 years in Colombia, 15+ years (with no age limit) in Mexico and 15–49 years in Peru. Mexico and Peru randomly selected one woman per household for questions about violence; Colombia included all ever-partnered, age-eligible women. Women were interviewed face-to-face, usually in or around the woman’s residence. All surveys had response rates above 85% and included weights for producing nationally representative estimates for women, children and households. Original survey reports provide more detail about sample design [9294].
To maximize comparability and focus on households at greatest risk of co-occurrence, this analysis limited study subsamples to women: a) asked about both IPV and child discipline in the current household; b) currently married or living with a partner as if married; c) aged 15–49; d) living with a daughter or son aged 1–14. After standardization, study subsamples sizes were 13,518 in Colombia, 38,097 in Mexico and 17,156 in Peru (Table 1).
Table 1
Denominator calculations
 
Colombia 2015 DHS
Mexico 2016 ENDIREH
Peru 2018 DHS
Original full survey sample
38,087
111,256
38,777
Original child discipline sample
21,537
90,068
22,412
Original eligibility criteria for child discipline questions
Ever partnered and ever had a live birtha
Did not select response option ‘no children’
Selected for violence module and living with 1+ daughter or son
Reason excluded:
 No child discipline datab
(16,550)
(21,188)
(16,365)
 Never partneredc
0
(3,986)
(957)
 Not age 15–49
(4)
(31,061)
0
 Not a regular household member
0
0
(184)
 Not living with a daughter or son aged 1–14
(4,463)
(11,926)
(1,770)
 Previously but not currently partnered
(3,552)
(4,998)
(2,345)
Study subsample, this analysis
13,518
38,097
17,156
All numbers are unweighted
aThese criteria matched the Colombia dataset but were different than filter instructions written on the questionnaire (living with 1+ biological or stepchild)
bWomen without child discipline data included those who were not eligible, not selected, refused participation or reported “no children”
cDefined as never married or cohabited

Safety and ethics

All three surveys were developed with participation of civil society organizations and advocates working on violence against women. All surveys adhered to World Health Organization ethical and safety recommendations for IPV research such as informed consent, privacy, confidentiality, female interviewers and interviewer training on violence against women [95]. However, Colombia asked all eligible women in the household about IPV not just one, posing a risk to confidentiality, and the informed consent process in Mexico did not emphasize women’s right to refuse any question. Published reports and manuals did not say whether interviewers made referrals to children’s services when respondents disclosed violence against children.

Measures of physical punishment

Surveys measured physical child punishment in diverse ways (Table 2). Colombia and Peru asked open-ended questions about who punishes (“castiga”) the children before asking how and recorded answers with pre-coded categories. Mexico asked respondents whether they or their husbands/partners hit their children when they became ‘angry’ or ‘desperate’ (“se enoja o desespera”). All three surveys asked about punishment of daughters and sons generally, with no specific timeframe.
Table 2
Survey items used to measure physical punishment of children
Survey itemsa (Translated by authors)
Colombia
Who punishes your sons (daughters) (or your step or adopted sons (daughters)) in the household? Anyone else?
father/stepfather; mother/respondent/ stepmother; other; no one (not punished)
if father/stepfather: How does your partner punish your (step, adopted) sons (daughters)?
if respondent: How do you punish your (step, adopted) sons (daughters)?
if other: How does that person punish your (step, adopted) sons (daughters)?
coded acts of physical punishmentb: spank/smack; push; beat with objects
Mexico
When your husband or partner becomes angry or desperate with his/your daughters and sons, does he hit them sometimes? frequently? does not hit them? no sons/daughters?
When you become angry or desperate with your daughters and sons, do you hit them sometimes? frequently? do not hit them? no sons/daughters?
coded acts of physical punishment: sometimes hits; frequently hits
Peru
Who reprimands or punishes your daughters or sons in the household? Anyone else?
biological father; biological mother; other; no one (not punished)
if father: How does your husband/partner punish your daughter(s) or son(s)?
if mother: How do you punish your daughter(s) or son(s)?
if other: How does that person punish your daughter(s) or son(s)?
coded acts of physical punishmentb: spank/smack; beat or ‘physical punishment’
aWords in small caps font were coded but not read to respondents
bColombia and Peru also coded non-violent acts of discipline not listed in this table
For the secondary analysis, physical punishment was defined as any act of physical child discipline measured, except ‘pouring water’ (“echándoles agua”) in Peru, since it did not clearly meet the UN definition of ‘corporal punishment’ [96]. Otherwise eligible women in Colombia and Peru who reported that ‘no one’ punished the children were retained in denominators and coded as ‘no physical punishment’, in keeping with the MICS but in contrast to the 2018 Peru DHS report [93].

Measures of physical IPV

This analysis was limited to physical IPV because surveys measured physical but not sexual or emotional IPV in highly comparable ways. All surveys used a modified conflict tactics scale [97], asking whether the woman’s current husband/partner had carried out specific acts (e.g. slapped, punched, etc.) ever and in the past 12 months (Table 3). The secondary analysis created two composite variables: a) physical IPV ever (dichotomous), defined as 1 = 1+ act by the current husband/partner ever and 0 = no acts by the current husband/partner ever (reference category); and b) physical IPV by timeframe (trichotomous) with three mutually exclusive timeframes: 1 = past year, 2 = before but not during the past year, and 0 = never (reference category). Women who completed the domestic violence module (or equivalent in Mexico) but did not respond to individual IPV questions were retained in denominators and classified as ‘no’ for that act, in keeping with the DHS.
Table 3
Operational definitions of physical intimate partner violence (IPV) in this analysis
Survey
Acts of physical IPV included in the operational definition
 
Her current husband or partner (ever or in the past year):
Colombia
shoved or shook her; hit her with his hand; hit her with an object; kicked or dragged her; threatened her or attacked her with a knife, firearm, or other weapon; tried to strangle or burn her
Mexico
shoved her or pulled her hair; slapped or smacked her; tied her up; kicked her; threw an object at her; hit her with a fist or object; tried to choke or strangle her; threatened her with a knife or firearm or with burning; attacked her with a knife or razor; or shot her with a firearm
Peru
shoved, shook or threw something at her; slapped her or twisted her arm; hit her with his fist or something that could hurt her; kicked or dragged her; tried to strangle or burn her; attacked or threatened her with a knife, firearm, or other weapon

Patterns of violence in the household

The key dependent variable in this analysis was pattern of physical violence in the household, with four mutually exclusive categories: 1 = physical child punishment only (no physical IPV ever); 2 = co-occurrence (physical child punishment and physical IPV ever); 3 = physical IPV ever only (no physical child punishment); and 0 (reference category) = no violence (neither physical IPV ever nor physical child punishment). A modified version limited to severe physical child punishment (beating with objects) was created for Colombia to explore how findings compared with previously published analyses of that dataset [55].

Potential correlates

Potential correlates were selected based on available data, previous research, variables found to be significant in bivariate analyses, testing for collinearity and covariance using variance inflation factor (VIF) analysis without design effects, and goodness of fit testing, with stepwise selection and elimination. Due to a lack of available tests for multinomial regression, goodness of fit testing was carried out using logistic regression after converting co-occurrence into a dichotomous variable (1 = co-occurring physical child punishment and physical IPV ever; 0 = neither of these).
Most sociodemographic variables were highly comparable across surveys, but other potential correlates were not (Table 4). All variables were constructed as dichotomous or categorical, not continuous, because many had a nonlinear relationship with violence. Missing responses were rare except for married/mother age < 18 (Supplemental Table A). For dichotomous variables, women missing data were retained in denominators and coded as not having that characteristic. Only one woman (from Mexico) was missing data for a categorical variable (education); she was excluded from regressions analyses.
Table 4
Operational definitions of potential correlates
Variable
Operational definition
Women’s Age
Age group (15–29 years; 30–39 years; and 40–49 years)
Women’s education
Highest level attended (primary or none; ≤ 3 years of lower secondary; > 3 years of upper secondary; any post-secondary)
Household wealth
Wealth terciles (poorest; middle; richest) generated by principal component analysis
Residence
Based on National Statistics Office (NSO) definitions
 Colombia and Peru
(1 = urban; 0 = rural)
 Mexico
(1 = urban; 2 = semi-urban; 0 = rural)
Indigenous ethnicity
(1 = indigenous; 0 = not indigenous)
 Colombia
Indigenous by culture, community or physical characteristics, per household questionnaire
 Mexico
Speaks indigenous language, per household questionnaire
 Peru
Maternal tongue was indigenous, per women’s questionnaire
Married/mother age < 18
(1 = married, cohabited and/or gave birth at age < 18; 0 = did not marry, cohabit or give birth at age < 18)
Number of children
(1 = more than one daughter/son aged 1–14 living in the household; 0 = only one)
Age of youngest child
Age of youngest age-eligible (1–14) daughter/son living in the household in years (1; 2–5; 6–9; 10–14)
Husband/partner tries to socially isolate her (1 = yes; 0 = no)
 Colombia
He has prevented her from meeting friends or tried to limit her contact with her family in the past year
 Mexico
He gets angry if she visits or goes out with friends or family
 Peru
He prevents her from visiting friends or tries to limit her visits or contact with her family
Husband/partner drinks to excess (1 = yes; 0 = no)
 Colombia
Not measured
 Mexico
She gets mad at her husband/partner because he drinks alcohol or takes drugs
 Peru
Her partner gets drunk sometimes or frequently
Joint decisions about money (1 = yes; 0 = no)
 Colombia, Peru
She and her husband/partner both have a say in decisions about large purchases and daily necessities
 Mexico
She and her husband/partner each have an equal say in deciding how to spend and save money
She and her husband/partner share responsibility for household chores (1 = yes; 0 = no)
 Colombia
They equally divide responsibility for cleaning the house, cooking or washing
 Mexico
They are both responsible for domestic chores such as cleaning, cooking, ironing and washing
 Peru
Not measured
She agrees wife-beating is justified for 1+ reason (1 = yes; 0 = no, don’t know or unsure all reasons)
 Colombia and Peru
Agrees with 1+ of the following reasons: 1. she leaves without telling him; 2. she neglects the children; 3. she argues with him; 4. she refuses sex; 5. she burns the food
 Mexico
Not measured
She believes physical punishment is necessary (1 = sometimes or frequently; 0 = no or never)
 Colombia, Mexico
Not measured
 Peru
She believes physical punishment is necessary for raising a child
Violence in respondent’s childhood (1 = caregiver violence only; 2 = co-occurrence; 3 = IPV only; 0 = none)
Caregiver violence against respondent in childhood (1 = yes; 0 = no)
 Colombia
Her parents or stepparents punished her by spanking/smacking, pushing or beating with objects
 Mexico
People she lived with before age 15 hit her sometimes or often
 Peru
Her parents punished her by spanking/smacking, beating or burning
Respondent exposed to physical IPV in childhood (1 = yes; 0 = no or doesn’t know)
 Colombia
Her father beat her mother
 Mexico
Adults in her childhood home hit or beat each other sometimes or frequently
 Peru
Her father ever hit her mother
Violence in husband/partner’s childhood (1 = caregiver violence only; 2 = co-occurrence; 3 = IPV only; 0 = none)
Caregiver violence against husband/partner in childhood (1 = yes; 0 = no or doesn’t know)
 Colombia
He was mistreated/abused ("maltratada") by his parents or stepparents as a child
 Mexico
He was hit or insulted in his childhood household sometimes or often before age 15
 Peru
Not measured
Husband/partner exposed to physical IPV in childhood (1 = yes; 0 = no or doesn’t know)
 Colombia
His father beat his mother
 Mexico
His mother was hit/beaten by her husband before age 15
 Peru
Not measured
IPV intimate partner violence

Sociodemographic variables

Women’s age was categorical, grouped as: 15–29, 30–39 and 40–49 years. Education was defined by highest level reached (not necessarily completed) categorized as primary or less, lower secondary, upper secondary and any post-secondary. Residence was based on national statistics office definitions: urban/rural in Colombia and Peru, and urban, semi-urban and rural in Mexico. The reduced model for Mexico combined urban and semi-urban for a more comparable analysis.
Household wealth was categorized as poorest, middle and richest terciles of wealth index scores. Wealth scores were calculated using principal components analysis of household assets and other characteristics. These were pre-coded by original research teams in Colombia and Peru. In Mexico, they were produced by authors (ARC, JAM) using SPSS (Version 26) following DHS methodology [98].
Indigenous ethnicity was based on culture, community or physical characteristics per the household questionnaire in Colombia; whether women spoke an indigenous language per the household questionnaire in Mexico; and indigenous maternal tongue per the woman’s questionnaire in Peru. Mexico and Peru also measured indigenous ethnicity based on culture or ancestry, used for sensitivity testing.

Reproductive histories

Married or mother < age 18 was based on whether the respondent gave birth and/or married/cohabited with a husband/partner before age 18. Child marriage/early motherhood was chosen instead of child marriage alone because a substantial proportion of girls in LAC give birth before marriage/cohabitation [99], and child marriage and early motherhood covaried, making it problematic to include them both in the same model as separate variables. In keeping with the DHS, missing data for this variable were imputed if possible, using variables such as respondent’s age at the time of the interview and age of the first born child.
More than one age-eligible child (1 = two or more; 0 = only one) was based on the number of daughters or sons aged 1–14 living in the household at the time of the interview.
Age of youngest child was defined as the age of the youngest daughter or son aged 1–14 living in the household drawn from the birth history in Colombia and Peru, and the household register in Mexico. Because physical child punishment had a nonlinear relationship with age of youngest child, a categorical variable was constructed with four mutually exclusive categories: age 1 (reference category); age 2–5; age 6–9; and age 10–14.

Partnership dynamics

Four dichotomous variables were constructed to measure partnership dynamics. All three surveys measured: a) partner tries to socially isolate her; and b) she and her partner make joint decisions about household finances. In addition, Mexico and Peru measured her partner’s excess drinking (Colombia did not). Colombia and Mexico also measured whether the respondent and her partner shared responsibility for key household chores (Peru did not). Surveys measured these variables in diverse ways (Table 4).

Social norms

Two dichotomous variables were constructed for social norms. Colombia and Peru measured women’s agreement that wife-beating was justified for at least one of five reasons. Peru also measured whether women agreed that physical punishment was necessary for child rearing, the sole variable measured by just one survey, included because of its importance in research on violent discipline [29, 36, 59, 61].

Histories of violence in childhood

Two variables for caregivers’ exposure to violence in childhood were constructed, one for respondents and one for partners, each with four mutually exclusive categories: 1 = caregiver violence only (no IPV exposure); 2 = co-occurrence (both); 3 = IPV exposure only (no caregiver violence); and 0 = no violence (reference category). These domains were measured in diverse ways, and Peru did not measure violence in the partner’s childhood. Violence in the respondent’s childhood and violence in her partner’s childhood covaried however, so a combined variable was constructed for sensitivity testing with 0 = no violence in either of their childhoods; 1 = co-occurrence in both of their childhoods; and 2 = all other patterns of childhood violence.

Analysis

All analyses were done with Stata 16 (StataCorp LP, Texas) except for household wealth scores in Mexico, as previously noted. Analyses applied weights from domestic violence modules (Colombia and Peru) or the women’s dataset (Mexico), normalized to equalize weighted and unweighted numbers of women in each study subsample.
Percent distributions of each pattern of physical violence in the household were produced with 95% confidence intervals (CI). Women’s characteristics were described with percent distributions of women with each characteristic among each full study subsample and each of the four, mutually exclusive cohorts, defined by pattern of household violence. Differences across cohorts were tested for significance using Pearson’s chi squared test, corrected for survey design effects and converted into an F-statistic.
Multinomial logistic regressions examined associations between potential correlates and each pattern of violence in the household: 1 = physical child punishment (only); 2 = co-occurrence; and 3 = physical IPV ever (only), each compared with no violence (0 = reference category), in other words, three pairwise comparisons (0 vs. 1; 0 vs. 2; and 0 vs. 3). Associations were examined with adjusted odds ratios (aORs) and 95% CIs, with p < 0.05 considered statistically significant. Because surveys measured different variables, multinomial logistic regressions were carried out for a more comparable, ‘reduced’ model, adjusting for variables measured by all three surveys, with results presented in figures charted with baseline value of 1 on a logarithmic scale of base 10. Results of full models, adjusted for all variables available from each country are presented in tabular form.
Finally, to explore whether associations between physical child punishment and physical IPV remained significant after controlling for other factors, logistic regression was used to produce ORs of physical child punishment (as a dependent variable) in households affected by physical IPV (past year and before past year) compared with never using four models: 1) unadjusted; 2) adjusted for age, education, wealth and residence; 3) reduced models adjusted for factors measured by all surveys; and 4) full models, adjusted for all factors.

Results

Prevalence of physical violence in the household

In all countries, women reported higher levels of physical punishment of children than physical IPV ever (Table 5). Prevalence of physical child punishment only (no physical IPV ever) ranged between one fourth and one third, including 26.1% in Peru, 28.9% in Mexico and 31.9% in Colombia. Physical IPV ever only (no physical child punishment) was reported by 6.4% of women in Mexico, 11.4% in Colombia and 12.9% in Peru. Co-occurrence (both physical child punishment and physical IPV ever) was reported by 10.7% of women in Mexico, 14.4% in Peru and 15.8% in Colombia. Just over 2 in 5 women reported neither form of violence in Colombia and Peru, as did just over half of women in Mexico.
Table 5
Percent distribution of women by pattern of physical violence in the household
Country
No violence
Physical child punishment (only)
Co-occurrence
Physical IPV ever (only)
%
(95% CI)
%
(95% CI)
%
(95% CI)
%
(95% CI)
Colombia
40.9
(39.5–42.4)
31.9
(30.5–33.4)
15.8
(14.6–16.9)
11.4
(10.5–12.2)
Mexico
53.9
(53.2–54.7)
28.9
(28.2–29.6)
10.7
(10.3–11.2)
6.4
(6.1–6.8)
Peru
46.7
(45.2–48.2)
26.1
(24.8–27.3)
14.4
(13.3–15.4)
12.9
(11.9–13.9)
CI confidence interval, IPV intimate partner violence

Women’s characteristics and bivariate analyses

Tables 6, 7 and 8 present percent distributions of women with each characteristic among the full study subsample and among each of four cohorts defined by pattern of violence. Bivariate analyses found significant differences across the four groups for almost all characteristics and countries, except age and residence in Colombia. Only 1.4% of women in Peru agreed with at least one reason for wife-beating, as did 3.4% of women in Colombia (not measured in Mexico). In contrast, almost one fourth (23.4%) of women in Peru (the only survey with data) agreed that physical child punishment was necessary for childrearing.
Table 6
Percent distribution of women with key characteristics, by pattern of violence, Colombia
Women’s characteristics
Among women who reported:
p-value
All women in study subsample
No violence
Physical child punishment only
Co-occurrence (both)
IPV only
n = 5,535
n = 4,316
n = 2,130
n = 1,537
N = 13,518
Women’s age
%
%
%
%
 
%
 15–29 years
35.7
36.1
35.9
36.7
0.258
36.0
 30–39 years
40.9
43.3
40.4
43.8
 
41.9
 40–49 years
23.4
20.6
23.8
19.5
 
22.1
Women’s education
 Primary or none
22.3
22.6
27.4
21.8
 < 0.001
23.1
 Lower secondary
10.3
10.3
15.1
15.0
 
11.6
 Upper secondary
33.8
34.4
36.7
37.3
 
34.8
 Post-secondary
33.7
32.7
20.8
25.9
 
30.5
Household wealth
 Poorest
35.0
36.4
41.4
39.3
0.005
36.9
 Middle
32.6
32.6
35.0
34.0
 
33.1
 Richest
32.5
30.9
23.6
26.6
 
29.9
Residence
 Urban
74.1
72.2
72.7
73.6
0.522
73.2
 Rural
25.9
27.8
27.3
26.4
 
26.8
Indigenous ethnicity
 No
92.4
94.6
93.7
92.2
0.002
93.3
 Yes
7.6
5.4
6.3
7.8
 
6.7
Married/mother < age 18
 No
65.5
64.7
54.3
52.4
< 0.001
62.0
 Yes
34.5
35.3
45.7
47.6
 
38.0
Number of children aged 1–14
 1
58.3
50.4
45.7
58.0
 < 0.001
53.8
 2+ 
41.7
49.6
54.3
42.0
 
46.2
Age of youngest child
 1 year
13.4
10.6
11.0
9.8
 < 0.001
11.8
 2–5 years
36.2
43.5
42.1
34.9
 
39.3
 6–9 years
24.6
26.9
27.2
28.3
 
26.2
 10–14 years
25.7
19.0
19.6
27.0
 
22.8
Partner tries to socially isolate her
 No
90.8
90.5
56.7
60.7
 < 0.001
81.9
 Yes
9.2
9.5
43.3
39.3
 
18.1
Partner drinks to excess
Not measured
    
Joint money decisions
 No
54.2
58.0
63.0
68.2
< 0.001
58.4
 Yes
45.8
42.0
37.0
31.8
 
41.6
Shared household chores
 No
61.5
67.2
74.5
74.3
< 0.001
66.8
 Yes
38.5
32.8
25.5
25.7
 
33.2
Wife beating justified
 No/DK 5 reasons
96.8
96.9
95.3
96.2
0.031
96.6
 Yes (1+ of 5 reason)
3.2
3.1
4.7
3.8
 
3.4
Physical punishment necessary
Not measured
    
Violence in her childhood
 None
37.2
18.1
13.7
28.8
 < 0.001
26.4
 By caregiver only
34.6
47.9
35.2
32.5
 
38.7
 Co-occurrence
19.5
28.8
44.0
26.9
 
27.2
 Exposure to IPV
8.7
5.1
7.2
11.9
 
7.7
Violence in partner’s childhood
 None/DK
69.9
60.0
39.9
48.5
 < 0.001
59.6
 By caregiver only
13.7
17.5
20.1
15.8
 
16.1
 Co-occurrence
8.8
13.7
26.9
21.5
 
14.7
 Exposure to IPV
7.7
8.7
13.1
14.2
 
9.6
DK Don’t know, IPV intimate partner violence, significant p-values are bolded. All figures are weighted
Table 7
Percent distribution of women with key characteristics, by pattern of violence, Mexico
Women’s characteristics
Among women who reported:
p-value
All women in study subsample
No violence
Physical child punishment only
Co-occurrence (both)
IPV only
n = 20,551
n = 11,013
n = 4,085
n = 2,448
N = 38,097
Women’s age
%
%
%
%
 
%
 15–29 years
34.4
29.1
30.1
32.3
 < 0.001
32.3
 30–39 years
41.5
45.7
46.3
42.6
 
43.3
 40–49 years
24.1
25.2
23.6
25.1
 
24.4
Women’s education
 Primary or none
22.2
22.3
28.2
26.7
 < 0.001
23.2
 Lower secondary
36.7
39.7
42.3
44.3
 
38.7
 Upper secondary
22.9
22.4
20.4
18.6
 
22.2
 Post-secondary
18.2
15.5
9.1
10.4
 
16.0
Household wealth
 Poorest
36.8
38.9
45.6
44.1
 < 0.001
38.8
 Middle
32.2
33.4
33.6
33.0
 
32.8
 Richest
31.0
27.6
20.8
23.0
 
28.4
Residence
 Urban
50.0
46.2
48.3
51.3
 < 0.001
48.8
 Semi-urban
23.7
26.7
25.3
22.3
 
24.6
 Rural
26.3
27.1
26.4
26.5
 
26.6
Indigenous ethnicity
 No
92.1
93.9
91.0
90.5
 < 0.001
92.4
 Yes
7.9
6.1
9.0
9.5
 
7.6
Married/mother < age 18
 No
71.5
71.3
58.5
61.1
 < 0.001
69.4
 Yes
28.5
28.7
41.5
38.9
 
30.6
Number of children aged 1–14
 1
44.9
32.5
31.9
40.5
 < 0.001
39.6
 2+ 
55.1
67.5
68.1
59.5
 
60.4
Age of youngest child
 1 year
13.6
9.7
8.7
11.5
 < 0.001
11.8
 2–5 years
38.2
41.8
39.2
36.5
 
39.3
 6–9 years
24.7
28.1
28.5
24.9
 
26.1
 10–14 years
23.5
20.4
23.7
27.0
 
22.9
Partner tries to socially isolate her
 No
95.9
91.3
66.0
72.9
 < 0.001
89.9
 Yes
4.1
8.7
34.0
27.1
 
10.1
Partner drinks to excess
 No
83.3
74.3
47.4
53.8
 < 0.001
74.9
 Yes
16.7
25.7
52.6
46.2
 
25.1
Joint money decisions
 No
44.2
47.6
60.7
57.4
 < 0.001
47.8
 Yes
55.8
52.4
39.3
42.6
 
52.2
Shared household chores
 No
80.1
80.8
87.3
86.4
 < 0.001
81.5
 Yes
19.9
19.2
12.7
13.6
 
18.5
Wife beating justified
Not measured
    
Physical punishment necessary
Not measured
    
Violence in her childhood
 None
69.5
41.0
24.6
41.1
 < 0.001
54.6
 By caregiver only
11.1
27.5
22.7
16.0
 
17.4
 Co-occurrence
9.9
20.3
39.6
26.1
 
17.2
 Exposure to IPV
9.5
11.2
13.1
16.9
 
10.8
Violence in partner’s childhood
 None/DK
73.4
53.4
33.1
45.4
 < 0.001
61.5
 By caregiver only
11.5
22.4
22.2
17.5
 
16.2
 Co-occurrence
8.9
14.8
31.6
25.5
 
14.1
 Exposure to IPV
6.2
9.4
13.1
11.6
 
8.2
DK Don’t know, IPV intimate partner violence; significant p-values are bolded. All figures are weighted
Table 8
Percent distribution of women with key characteristics, by pattern of violence, Peru
Women’s characteristics
Among women who reported:
p-value
All women in study subsample
No violence
Physical child punishment only
Co-occurrence (both)
IPV only
n = 8,009
n = 4,472
n = 2,462
n = 2,213
N = 17,156
Women’s age
%
%
%
%
 
%
 15–29 years
28.6
33.6
29.1
22.7
 < 0.001
29.2
 30–39 years
42.9
43.2
45.3
44.4
 
43.5
 40–49 years
28.6
23.2
25.6
32.8
 
27.3
Women's education
 Primary or none
21.6
28.5
28.3
25.7
0.001
24.9
 Lower secondary
10.7
12.3
14.2
14.9
 
12.2
 Upper secondary
29.6
29.3
30.6
31.8
 
30.0
 Post-secondary
38.1
30.0
26.9
27.6
 
33.0
Household wealth
 Poorest
35.9
42.1
44.6
40.8
 < 0.001
39.4
 Middle
32.7
32.0
32.8
37.3
 
33.2
 Richest
31.4
25.8
22.6
21.9
 
27.5
Residence
 Urban
76.8
72.8
74.0
78.8
 < 0.001
75.6
 Rural
23.2
27.2
26.0
21.2
 
24.4
Indigenous ethnicity
 No
82.0
85.2
77.7
73.8
 < 0.001
81.2
 Yes
18.0
14.8
22.3
26.2
 
18.9
Married/mother < age 18
 No
73.5
68.4
64.4
64.8
 < 0.001
69.8
 Yes
26.5
31.6
35.6
35.2
 
30.2
Number of children aged 1–14
 1
52.5
37.3
35.0
46.3
 < 0.001
45.2
 2+ 
47.5
62.7
65.0
53.7
 
54.8
Age of youngest child
 1 year
13.6
10.7
9.7
9.3
 < 0.001
11.8
 2–5 years
42.4
56.4
51.6
41.5
 
47.3
 6–9 years
26.5
24.4
26.3
29.5
 
26.3
 10–14 years
17.5
8.4
12.5
19.6
 
14.7
Partner tries to socially isolate her
 No
95.4
92.9
74.2
74.5
 < 0.001
89.0
 Yes
4.6
7.1
25.8
25.5
 
11.0
Partner drinks to excess
 No
47.5
42.3
23.2
30.1
 < 0.001
40.4
 Yes
52.5
57.7
76.8
69.9
 
59.6
Joint money decisions
 No
69.5
72.5
78.1
77.0
 < 0.001
72.5
 Yes
30.5
27.5
21.9
23.0
 
27.5
Shared household chores
Not measured
    
Wife beating justified
 No/DK all reasons
99.0
98.0
98.0
98.7
0.003
98.6
 Yes (1+ of 5 reasons)
1.0
2.0
2.0
1.3
 
1.4
Physical punishment necessary
 No/DK
88.3
61.1
59.6
85.0
 < 0.001
76.6
 Yes
11.7
38.9
40.4
15.0
 
23.4
Violence in her childhood
 None
33.6
15.5
11.1
18.5
 < 0.001
23.7
 By caregiver only
32.2
43.6
33.7
27.0
 
34.7
 Co-occurrence
24.1
34.0
46.7
44.6
 
32.6
 Exposure to IPV
10.1
6.9
8.4
9.9
 
9.0
Violence in partner's childhood
Not measured
    
DK Don’t know, IPV intimate partner violence; significant p-values are bolded. All figures are weighted

Correlates of physical violence in the household

Multinomial regression analyses examined associations between possible correlates and three mutually exclusive patterns of household violence: 1) physical child punishment (only), 2) co-occurring physical child punishment and physical IPV, and 3) physical IPV (only) –each compared with ‘no violence’, after adjusting for other variables. Results of reduced models (limited to variables measured by all three countries) are presented in Figs. 1, 2 and 3 and Supplemental Table B. Results of full models are presented in Tables 9, 10 and 11. Supplemental Table C presents unadjusted and adjusted odds ratios (from both models) of co-occurrence side by side.
Table 9
Full Model: Odds ratios of each pattern of violence, adjusted for all variables, Colombia
Women’s characteristics
Physical punishment (only)
Co-occurrence (both)
Physical IPV ever (only)
aOR
CI (95%)
p-value
aOR
CI (95%)
p-value
aOR
CI (95%)
p-value
Age (Ref: 40–49 years)
 15–29 years
1.06
(0.87–1.31)
0.555
0.69
(0.51–0.94)
0.018
1.05
(0.76–1.44)
0.781
 30–39 years
1.07
(0.87–1.31)
0.536
0.79
(0.61–1.02)
0.065
1.18
(0.90–1.56)
0.236
Education (Ref: Post-secondary)
 Primary or none
0.92
(0.73–1.16)
0.480
1.35
(0.98–1.85)
0.063
0.89
(0.64–1.23)
0.482
 Lower secondary
0.94
(0.73–1.20)
0.598
1.52
(1.11–2.10)
0.010
1.26
(0.87–1.83)
0.219
 Upper secondary
0.96
(0.76–1.20)
0.693
1.29
(0.98–1.69)
0.071
1.13
(0.82–1.56)
0.446
Household wealth (Ref: Richest)
 Poorest
1.00
(0.79–1.26)
0.999
1.54
(1.09–2.18)
0.013
1.40
(1.00–1.95)
0.048
 Middle
1.03
(0.84–1.27)
0.750
1.20
(0.86–1.67)
0.280
1.13
(0.84–1.53)
0.426
Urban residence (Ref: Rural)
0.89
(0.74–1.06)
0.186
1.31
(1.04–1.66)
0.021
1.16
(0.90–1.49)
0.261
Indigenous ethnicity (Ref: No)
0.68
(0.55–0.83)
 < 0.001
0.74
(0.56–0.99)
0.044
1.08
(0.82–1.42)
0.570
Married/mother < 18 (Ref:18+)
1.02
(0.86–1.21)
0.800
1.28
(1.04–1.58)
0.022
1.49
(1.18–1.88)
0.001
2+ children aged 1–14 (Ref: 1)
1.39
(1.15–1.67)
0.001
1.51
(1.23–1.86)
 < 0.001
0.96
(0.78–1.20)
0.744
Age youngest eligible child (Ref: 1)
 2–5 years
1.51
(1.16–1.96)
0.002
1.39
(1.04–1.87)
0.027
1.29
(0.95–1.77)
0.107
 6–9 years
1.35
(1.04–1.74)
0.023
1.14
(0.82–1.60)
0.429
1.49
(1.07–2.09)
0.019
 10–14 years
0.98
(0.71–1.34)
0.885
0.92
(0.63–1.34)
0.656
1.58
(1.06–2.36)
0.024
Partner isolates her (Ref: No)
0.92
(0.72–1.16)
0.462
5.72
(4.48–7.31)
 < 0.001
5.03
(4.09–6.20)
 < 0.001
Partner drinks to excess
Not measured
       
Joint money decisions (Ref: No)
0.83
(0.71–0.97)
0.021
0.77
(0.64–0.93)
0.006
0.65
(0.54–0.78)
 < 0.001
Shared chores (Ref: No)
0.81
(0.67–0.97)
0.022
0.70
(0.56–0.88)
0.002
0.68
(0.55–0.85)
0.001
Wife beating justified (Ref: No/DK)
1.01
(0.75–1.35)
0.953
1.40
(0.97–2.02)
0.072
1.04
(0.68–1.59)
0.845
Physical punishment needed
Not measured
       
Violence in her childhood (Ref: No)
 Caregiver violence only
2.86
(2.37–3.46)
 < 0.001
2.91
(2.24–3.78)
 < 0.001
1.26
(1.00–1.59)
0.048
 Co-occurrence
2.99
(2.42–3.70)
 < 0.001
5.20
(3.93–6.88)
 < 0.001
1.55
(1.21–1.99)
0.001
 Exposure to IPV only
1.12
(0.82–1.53)
0.474
1.83
(1.22–2.74)
0.003
1.44
(1.04–1.99)
0.030
Violence in partner's childhood (Ref: No/DK)
 Caregiver violence only
1.41
(1.08–1.84)
0.012
2.14
(1.63–2.81)
 < 0.001
1.41
(1.01–1.96)
0.045
 Co-occurrence
1.71
(1.33–2.21)
 < 0.001
3.98
(3.14–5.04)
 < 0.001
2.78
(2.11–3.66)
 < 0.001
 Exposure to IPV only
1.23
(0.90–1.69)
0.195
2.49
(1.82–3.39)
 < 0.001
2.24
(1.55–3.25)
 < 0.001
aOR adjusted odds ratios, CI Confidence interval, DK doesn't know, IPV intimate partner violence, Ref reference category; significant p-values are bolded
Table 10
Full Model: Odds ratios of each pattern of violence, adjusted for all variables, Mexico
Women’s characteristics
Physical punishment (only)
Co-occurrence (both)
Physical IPV ever (only)
aOR
CI (95%)
p-value
aOR
CI (95%)
p-value
aOR
CI (95%)
p-value
Age (Ref: 40–49 years)
 15–29 years
0.73
(0.65–0.82)
 < 0.001
0.84
(0.69–1.02)
0.078
0.92
(0.73–1.16)
0.488
 30–39 years
0.91
(0.82–1.00)
0.051
1.08
(0.93–1.26)
0.309
1.05
(0.88–1.25)
0.591
Education (Ref: Post-secondary)
 Primary or none
1.01
(0.89–1.15)
0.879
1.35
(1.07–1.72)
0.012
1.36
(1.04–1.76)
0.022
 Lower secondary
1.15
(1.03–1.29)
0.017
1.39
(1.13–1.71)
0.002
1.47
(1.15–1.87)
0.002
 Upper secondary
1.12
(0.99–1.26)
0.065
1.38
(1.12–1.70)
0.002
1.18
(0.91–1.52)
0.218
Household wealth (Ref: Richest)
 Poorest
1.03
(0.92–1.16)
0.610
1.14
(0.95–1.38)
0.162
1.15
(0.93–1.42)
0.209
 Middle
1.05
(0.95–1.16)
0.335
1.12
(0.94–1.33)
0.201
1.07
(0.89–1.30)
0.465
Residence (Ref: Rural)
 Urban
0.87
(0.79–0.97)
0.011
1.22
(1.04–1.43)
0.015
1.33
(1.11–1.60)
0.002
 Semi-urban
1.05
(0.94–1.17)
0.377
1.15
(0.98–1.34)
0.089
1.04
(0.87–1.24)
0.647
Indigenous ethnicity (Ref: No)
0.73
(0.63–0.84)
 < 0.001
1.03
(0.83–1.27)
0.803
1.12
(0.91–1.38)
0.285
Married/mother < 18 (Ref:18+)
0.93
(0.85–1.01)
0.089
1.34
(1.17–1.53)
 < 0.001
1.26
(1.08–1.48)
0.004
2+ children aged 1–14 (Ref: 1)
1.68
(1.55–1.81)
 < 0.001
1.70
(1.50–1.92)
 < 0.001
1.18
(1.02–1.36)
0.029
Age of youngest eligible child (Ref: 1)
 2–5 years
1.52
(1.34–1.72)
 < 0.001
1.62
(1.33–1.98)
 < 0.001
1.16
(0.94–1.44)
0.172
 6–9 years
1.48
(1.29–1.70)
 < 0.001
1.68
(1.35–2.10)
 < 0.001
1.15
(0.91–1.45)
0.236
 10–14 years
1.19
(1.02–1.38)
0.026
1.56
(1.22–2.01)
 < 0.001
1.30
(1.00–1.70)
0.052
Partner isolates her (Ref: No)
1.74
(1.51–2.00)
 < 0.001
6.76
(5.74–7.94)
 < 0.001
5.44
(4.55–6.51)
 < 0.001
Partner drinks to excess (Ref: No)
1.45
(1.33–1.59)
 < 0.001
3.35
(2.97–3.78)
 < 0.001
2.84
(2.47–3.26)
 < 0.001
Joint money decisions (Ref: No)
0.93
(0.86–1.00)
0.049
0.67
(0.60–0.76)
 < 0.001
0.73
(0.64–0.84)
 < 0.001
Shared chores (Ref: No)
0.98
(0.89–1.07)
0.662
0.74
(0.64–0.87)
 < 0.001
0.77
(0.63–0.94)
0.010
Wife beating justified
Not measured
       
Physical punishment needed
Not measured
       
Violence in her childhood (Ref: No)
 Caregiver violence only
3.63
(3.30–3.99)
 < 0.001
4.33
(3.70–5.06)
 < 0.001
1.95
(1.61–2.36)
 < 0.001
 Co-occurrence
2.85
(2.55–3.18)
 < 0.001
6.28
(5.37–7.35)
 < 0.001
2.74
(2.29–3.28)
 < 0.001
 Exposure to IPV only
1.72
(1.53–1.94)
 < 0.001
2.46
(2.04–2.97)
 < 0.001
2.03
(1.68–2.47)
 < 0.001
Violence in partner’s childhood (Ref: No/DK)
 Caregiver violence only
1.88
(1.70–2.07)
 < 0.001
2.45
(2.09–2.86)
 < 0.001
1.70
(1.40–2.07)
 < 0.001
 Co-occurrence
1.58
(1.40–1.79)
 < 0.001
3.62
(3.09–4.24)
 < 0.001
2.65
(2.21–3.18)
 < 0.001
 Exposure to IPV only
1.64
(1.42–1.89)
 < 0.001
2.67
(2.22–3.21)
 < 0.001
1.95
(1.57–2.42)
 < 0.001
aOR: adjusted odds ratios; CI: Confidence interval; DK: doesn't know; IPV: intimate partner violence; Ref: reference category; significant p-values are bolded
Table 11
Full Model: Odds ratios of each pattern of violence. adjusted for all variables, Peru
Women’s characteristics
Physical punishment (only)
Co-occurrence (both)
Physical IPV ever (only)
aOR
CI (95%)
p-value
aOR
CI (95%)
p-value
aOR
CI (95%)
p-value
Age (Ref: 40–49 years)
 15–29 years
1.37
(1.09–1.74)
0.008
1.11
(0.81–1.51)
0.527
0.67
(0.49–0.91)
0.011
 30–39 years
1.08
(0.87–1.34)
0.472
1.06
(0.80–1.40)
0.693
0.88
(0.68–1.14)
0.332
Education (Ref: Post-secondary)
 Primary or none
1.87
(1.44–2.42)
 < 0.001
1.44
(1.03–2.01)
0.034
1.06
(0.73–1.53)
0.774
 Lower secondary
1.43
(1.07–1.92)
0.016
1.27
(0.90–1.81)
0.179
1.12
(0.77–1.64)
0.553
 Upper secondary
1.28
(1.04–1.58)
0.018
1.14
(0.87–1.50)
0.339
1.03
(0.78–1.37)
0.818
Household wealth (Ref: Richest)
 Poorest
0.99
(0.76–1.29)
0.944
1.30
(0.94–1.81)
0.111
1.57
(1.11–2.22)
0.011
 Middle
0.94
(0.75–1.17)
0.566
1.01
(0.74–1.37)
0.950
1.30
(0.96–1.77)
0.092
Urban residence (Ref: Rural)
1.08
(0.90–1.30)
0.389
1.41
(1.11–1.79)
0.005
1.70
(1.33–2.18)
 < 0.001
Indigenous ethnicity (Ref: No)
0.63
(0.52–0.75)
 < 0.001
0.96
(0.77–1.21)
0.753
1.36
(1.09–1.70)
0.007
Married/mother < 18 (Ref:18+)
1.00
(0.85–1.18)
0.974
1.18
(0.95–1.46)
0.133
1.38
(1.07–1.77)
0.013
2+ children aged 1–14 (Ref: 1)
1.54
(1.31–1.81)
 < 0.001
1.73
(1.36–2.21)
 < 0.001
1.22
(0.98–1.51)
0.072
Age of youngest eligible child (Ref: 1)
 2–5 years
1.76
(1.53–2.02)
 < 0.001
1.74
(1.43–2.10)
 < 0.001
1.33
(1.08–1.63)
0.006
 6–9 years
1.27
(1.03–1.57)
0.027
1.42
(1.07–1.88)
0.014
1.45
(1.09–1.92)
0.011
 10–14 years
0.75
(0.53–1.05)
0.093
1.20
(0.75–1.93)
0.438
1.45
(1.01–2.08)
0.045
Partner isolates her (Ref: No)
1.46
(1.04–2.04)
0.028
6.00
(4.45–8.08)
 < 0.001
6.09
(4.53–8.18)
 < 0.001
Partner drinks to excess (Ref: No)
1.10
(0.95–1.28)
0.201
2.47
(1.96–3.12)
 < 0.001
1.88
(1.50–2.36)
 < 0.001
Joint money decisions (Ref: No)
0.92
(0.78–1.08)
0.303
0.74
(0.59–0.95)
0.016
0.77
(0.62-0.96)
0.019
Shared chores
Not measured
       
Wife beating justified (Ref: No/DK)
1.49
(0.88–2.52)
0.138
1.75
(1.04–2.94)
0.034
1.46
(0.80–2.65)
0.219
Phys. punish. needed (Ref: No/DK)
4.53
(3.77–5.45)
 < 0.001
4.50
(3.55–5.70)
 < 0.001
1.21
(0.92–1.59)
0.176
Violence in her childhood (Ref: No)
 Caregiver violence only
2.57
(2.11–3.12)
 < 0.001
2.67
(1.87–3.83)
 < 0.001
1.46
(1.13–1.87)
0.003
 Co-occurrence
2.93
(2.39–3.60)
 < 0.001
5.12
(3.64–7.20)
 < 0.001
3.02
(2.31–3.96)
 < 0.001
 Exposure to IPV only
1.68
(1.26–2.24)
 < 0.001
2.62
(1.67–4.09)
 < 0.001
1.69
(1.15–2.50)
0.008
Violence in partner’s childhood (Ref: No/DK)
 Caregiver violence only
Not measured
       
 Co-occurrence
Not measured
       
 Exposure to IPV only
Not measured
       
aOR adjusted odds ratios, CI Confidence interval, DK doesn’t know, IPV intimate partner violence, Phys. punish. Physical punishment, Ref reference category; significant p-values are bolded

Sociodemographic factors

Associations between sociodemographic factors and patterns of violence were inconsistent across countries. In both full and reduced models, younger age was significantly associated with lower odds of co-occurrence in Colombia, physical child punishment (only) in Mexico, and IPV (only) in Peru, and with higher odds of physical child punishment (only) in Peru. Age was not significantly correlated with other patterns of violence.
In full models from all countries, lower education was significantly associated with higher odds of co-occurrence compared with post-secondary, but specific levels that were significant varied by country, and the relationship between rising education level and declining odds of co-occurrence was not always linear. Associations between education and other patterns of violence were inconsistent across countries and models.
In reduced models, the poorest wealth tercile was significantly correlated with co-occurrence in all countries, but in full models, wealth was not associated with any pattern of violence except co-occurrence among the poorest women in Colombia and IPV (only) among the poorest women in Colombia and Peru.
After adjusting for all factors in full models, urban (compared with rural) residence was associated with significantly higher odds of co-occurrence in all countries and of IPV (only) in Mexico and Peru (but not Colombia). In contrast, urban residence was associated with significantly lower odds of physical child punishment (only) in Mexico (both models); but not significant in Colombia or Peru in any model.

Indigenous ethnicity

After adjusting for other factors (full and reduced models), indigenous ethnicity was protective —meaning that compared with other women, women identified as indigenous had significantly lower odds— for co-occurrence in Colombia (but not Mexico or Peru) and for physical child punishment (only) in all three countries. In contrast, odds of IPV (only) were elevated among indigenous women in all countries, but significantly so only in Peru. Sensitivity testing in Mexico and Peru using indigenous ethnicity defined by culture/ancestors did not change these findings.

Reproductive history

In full and reduced models, aORs of co-occurrence were significantly elevated among women who had a history of child marriage/early motherhood (compared with women who did not) in Colombia and Mexico, but not Peru. In all countries, women’s history of child marriage/early motherhood was significantly associated with IPV (only) but not physical child punishment (only). Having more than one child aged 1–14 in the household was significantly associated with higher odds of all patterns of violence in all countries and models except IPV (only) in Colombia and Peru.
In full and reduced models, having a child aged 2–5 was significantly associated with co-occurrence and physical child punishment (only) across all countries, and with IPV (only) in Peru. In almost all countries and models, aORs of co-occurrence and physical child punishment (only) declined as age of youngest child rose, while aORs of IPV (only) rose, but associations were not always significant.

Partnership dynamics

In all countries, aORs of both co-occurrence and IPV (only) were 5–7 times higher among women whose partner tried to socially isolate them than among other women. Social isolation was also significantly associated with higher odds physical child punishment (only) in Mexico and Peru (aOR of 1.74 and 1.46 respectively) but not Colombia.
In Mexico and Peru, partner’s excess drinking was significantly associated with all patterns of violence except physical child punishment (only) in Peru. However, the association with physical child punishment (only) did become significant (aOR 1.17; 95% CI: 1.02–1.35, p = 0.026) in Peru when the model was run without agreement with physical child punishment, suggesting some covariance. When the full model was run with frequency of partner’s drunkenness in Peru, aORs of co-occurrence were 2.22 aOR (95% CI: 1.76–2.82, p < 0.001) for ‘sometimes’ (versus never) drunk and 14.9 aOR (95% CI: 8.90–25.0, p < 0.001) for ‘frequently’ drunk.
In all models and countries, collaborative partnership dynamics, meaning joint financial decision-making (all countries) and shared domestic chores (Colombia and Mexico), were significantly protective for most patterns of violence, except shared chores and physical child punishment (only) in Mexico, and joint decision-making and physical child punishment (only) in Peru. Shared chores and joint decision-making covaried to some degree however, and their protective effect was muted when both were included in models, as seen by comparing full and reduced models in Colombia and Mexico.

Social norms

After controlling for other factors, agreement with wife-beating was not significantly associated with any pattern of violence in Colombia and Peru except co-occurrence in Peru (not measured in Mexico). However, in Colombia, when the ‘pattern of household violence’ variable was constructed with severe physical punishment of children (beating with objects) instead of any physical punishment, support for wife-beating was significantly associated with severe child punishment (only), 1.45 aOR (95% CI: 1.07-1.97, p = 0.017) and with co-occurring severe child punishment and IPV (aOR 1.53, 95% CI: 1.04–2.26, p = 0.031). The association with IPV only (no severe child punishment) remained insignificant.
In Peru, agreement with the necessity of physical child punishment was significantly associated with both co-occurrence and physical child punishment (only). The aORs of both these patterns of violence were more than four times higher among women who agreed with physical punishment than among those who did not. In contrast, the slightly elevated aOR of IPV (only) among women who agreed (vs. disagreed) was not significant.

Histories of childhood violence

In all countries and models, aORs of co-occurrence in the current household were more than five times higher among women who reported co-occurrence in childhood compared with those who did not. In fact, in all models and countries, almost all patterns of violence (caregiver violence only, co-occurrence and exposure to IPV only) in either respondents’ or partners’ childhoods were significantly correlated with elevated odds of almost all patterns of violence in the current household. Exceptions were in Peru, which did not measure violence in the partner’s childhood, and in Colombia, where histories of childhood exposure to IPV (only) among respondents or partners were not significantly associated with physical child punishment (only) in the current household.
Despite efforts to limit covariance among variables, some interactions remained, particularly between respondents’ and partners’ histories of violence in childhood. When these two variables were replaced with a consolidated variable (available only from Colombia and Mexico), aORs of co-occurrence in the current household were around 20 times higher among women who reported co-occurrence in both their own and their partner’s childhoods compared with women who reported no violence in either their own or their partner’s childhoods, with 21.60 aOR (95% CI: 14.32–32.56, p < 0.001) in Colombia and 20.71 aOR (95% CI: 16.06–26.70, p < 0.001) in Mexico.

Correlations between physical child punishment and IPV

When associations between physical child punishment (a dependent variable) and physical IPV (an independent variable) were examined with logistic regression, odds of physical child punishment remained significantly elevated (p < 0.001) in households affected by IPV (past year and before past year compared with never) across all countries and models (Table 12). Odds ratios of physical child punishment in households affected by IPV declined slightly as more variables were added but remained about 1.5 to 2 (p < 0.001) in all countries even after adjusting for all factors in the full model.
Table 12
Odds ratios of physical child punishment in households affected by intimate partner violence
IPV Timeframe
Colombia
Mexico
Peru
OR
(95% CI)
p-value
OR
(95% CI)
p-value
OR
(95% CI)
p-value
 
Unadjusted odds ratios
Never
Ref
Ref
Ref
Past year
1.83
(1.58–2.12)
 < 0.001
3.67
(3.26–4.12)
 < 0.001
2.35
(1.95–2.84)
 < 0.001
Before past year
1.72
(1.41–2.11)
 < 0.001
2.62
(2.35–2.93)
 < 0.001
1.79
(1.53–2.10)
 < 0.001
 
Adjusted for age, education, wealth and residence only
Never
Ref
Ref
Ref
Past year
1.81
(1.56–2.09)
 < 0.001
3.68
(3.27–4.14)
 < 0.001
2.31
(1.90–2.80)
 < 0.001
Before past year
1.71
(1.40–2.09)
 < 0.001
2.54
(2.27–2.83)
 < 0.001
1.82
(1.55–2.14)
 < 0.001
 
Reduced model adjusted for factors available from all three countries
Never
Ref
Ref
Ref
Past year
1.66
(1.40–1.97)
 < 0.001
2.38
(2.08–2.72)
 < 0.001
2.08
(1.63–2.65)
 < 0.001
Before past year
1.56
(1.30–1.86)
 < 0.001
1.94
(1.72–2.19)
 < 0.001
1.65
(1.39–1.96)
 < 0.001
 
Full model adjusted for all factors
Never
Ref
Ref
Ref
Past year
1.56
(1.31–1.85)
 < 0.001
2.03
(1.77–2.32)
 < 0.001
1.94
(1.50–2.52)
 < 0.001
Before past year
1.46
(1.22–1.74)
 < 0.001
1.72
(1.52–1.94)
 < 0.001
1.58
(1.32–1.90)
 < 0.001
CI confidence interval, IPV intimate partner violence, OR odds ratio, Ref reference category

Discussion

This is the first multi-country study from LAC to investigate correlates of co-occurring physical child punishment and physical IPV in the same household using national, population-based datasets. In all three countries, co-occurrence was significantly associated with women’s lower education, urban residence, child marriage/early motherhood, 2+ children in the household, a child aged 2–5, a partner who tried to isolate her from family or friends, and a history of violence (caregiver violence, exposure to IPV or both) in her own childhood. Significant associations were also found with partner’s excess drinking (Mexico and Peru), women’s agreement with physical child punishment (Peru) and partner’s history of childhood violence (Colombia and Mexico). These findings generally harmonize with previous research on violent discipline, IPV and their intersection [21, 23, 25, 58, 64, 65, 6971, 74, 79]. Correlations with lower education, partner’s drinking, social isolation and support for violence against children are consistent with some, though not all previous studies of co-occurrence in LMICs [25] and high-income settings [21].
Conversely, collaborative partnership dynamics, including shared household decision-making (all countries) and shared household chores (Colombia and Mexico) were significantly protective. There is a dearth of quantitative studies from LMICs examining co-occurrence and partnership dynamics; however, these findings echo qualitative research from Uganda linking co-occurrence with patriarchal family structures that normalize women’s subordinate position and encourage violence to enforce power structures within families [46].
The study did not find consistent evidence that socio-demographic characteristics (age, education, wealth, residence or ethnicity) were shared risk factors for physical violence in the household across the three countries. In fact, indigenous ethnicity and urban residence tended to be protective for physical child punishment but a risk factor for IPV, though associations were not always significant. This finding echoes the separate literatures on IPV [70] and violent discipline, including evidence suggesting that poverty may not be a universal correlate of household violence [29, 40, 41, 100, 101]. Similarly, child marriage/early motherhood was significantly associated with IPV (only) in all countries and with co-occurrence in Mexico and Peru (consistent with the IPV literature [80]), but not with physical child punishment (only) in any country.
The study found evidence that partner’s excess drinking was a significant shared risk factor for all three patterns of violence in Mexico. In Peru it was a shared risk factor after excluding attitudes towards physical child punishment, with which it covaried. These findings support a large literature theorizing that harmful use of alcohol may be a policy-relevant risk factor for both IPV and violence against children [23, 6466].
A partner who tried to isolate her from family or friends was a shared risk factor for all patterns of violence except physical child punishment (only) in Colombia, while collaborative partnership dynamics, such as shared financial decision-making (all countries) and shared domestic chores (Colombia and Mexico) were protective. These findings are consistent with theories articulated by major international frameworks for violence prevention that greater gender equality and empowerment of women within households may protect women and children from violence [82, 83]. They also echo evidence from the IPV literature that joint decision-making may be protective for IPV against women even more than any female decision-making [63, 65, 87].
This study did not find consistent evidence that social norms justifying violence were shared risk factors, after controlling for other factors. Only Peru measured agreement with physical child punishment, which was a significant risk factor for physical child punishment (only) and co-occurrence (supporting previous research on violent discipline [29, 36, 59, 61]), but not IPV (only). Support for wife-beating in Colombia and Peru was not significantly correlated with any pattern of violence except co-occurrence in Peru. This contrasts with other research [31, 35], including a previous analysis of the same Colombia dataset that found a significant association between support for wife-beating and beating children under five with objects [55]. This study replicated that finding by replacing ‘any physical child punishment’ with ‘beating with objects’, highlighting how differences in operational definitions may change findings, and comparisons should be made with caution. It is also noteworthy that support for wife-beating in these surveys was extremely low: only 1.4% of women in Peru and 3.4% of women in Colombia agreed with any reason for wife-beating, compared with 41.1% of women across 49 LMICs in a 2018 analysis [102]. Moreover, it is possible that community or male support for wife-beating may elevate the risk of violence even if female support does not, but that was beyond the scope of this study. The strongest evidence for shared risk factors was for caregiver histories of violence in childhood, providing support for ‘intergenerational transmission of violence’ theories [23, 67, 103]. Histories of almost all patterns of violence (caregiver violence, exposure to IPV and/or both) in childhood among women (all countries) and their partners (Colombia and Mexico) were significant correlates of all patterns of violence in the current household, with a couple of exceptions in Colombia.
Finally, this study found that associations between physical child punishment and IPV could not be explained by shared risk factors alone. In all countries, physical IPV (past year and before past year) remained a significant correlate for physical child punishment, even after controlling for all other factors. Understanding pathways by which IPV increases the risk of physical child punishment was beyond the scope of this study; however, these findings are consistent with the theory that partner violence may “spill over” into caregiver-child relationships due to stress, social learning and/or normalization of violence [21, 27, 46, 49].

Limitations

This study had many limitations. Survivor reports of IPV and caregiver reports of violent discipline may underestimate prevalence due to social stigma or fear of reprisal. In contrast to the MICS [28], these surveys measured a small number of disciplinary acts in diverse ways, without asking about a specific child or timeframe. Nonetheless, the estimate of physical child punishment in Mexico produced by this analysis (39.6%, 95% CI: 38.9–40.4) was only somewhat lower than the Mexico 2015 MICS estimate of 43.7% [104], even though MICS asked about eight acts of physical punishment and ENDIREH asked about only one.
Another limitation was that while key sociodemographic variables (e.g. age, education, wealth, etc.) were highly comparable across surveys, other variables were not. As a result, models varied by country, and comparisons should be made with caution. In addition, this analysis was limited to physical violence, in contrast to studies that define child maltreatment and IPV to include sexual and emotional as well as physical abuse [21].
Another important limitation is that the ecological framework posits that societal, community, household and individual factors may contribute to violence [82, 83], but this analysis was limited to select household and individual variables based on data availability and a quest for comparability. Other correlates worthy of research include levels of armed/criminal violence and socio-economic disadvantages in the community [42, 5458], inequitable gender norms endorsed by male partners [23, 31, 62], maternal anxiety, depression and harmful use of alcohol/drugs [26, 42, 48, 105, 106], and histories of unplanned or unwanted pregnancies [106]. Finally, an analysis of how correlates varied according to whether the woman, her partner or both physically punished the children was beyond the scope of this study but that is a worthy question for future research.

Conclusions

This study highlights important intersections between violent discipline of children and IPV against women, consistent with calls for greater investment in coordinated responses to both forms of violence [107]. The study findings are consistent with several theories of change articulated by key international violence prevention frameworks [82, 83], including the theory that physical child punishment and IPV against women may share risk and protective factors and that more equitable gender relations, empowerment of women and collaborative partnerships may protect women and children from violence. These theories suggest a need for strategies to improve women’s access to education, to prevent child marriage/early motherhood and to promote more collaborative, gender equitable partnerships. This study also produced findings consistent with the theory that violence between partners may ‘spill over’ into caregiver-child relationships, regardless of shared risk factors, suggesting that to be effective, programmes aiming to reduce violent discipline of children may also need to address IPV against women in the household. Finally, evidence of intergenerational transmission suggests that long run prevention of violence against women may not be possible without reducing violent discipline of children, and vice versa. Generally, this study highlights a need for research, programmes and policies to pay greater attention to intersections between violence against children and violence against women.

Acknowledgements

Thanks are due to the thousands of women and girls who provided data for this study and to survey research teams in Colombia, Mexico and Peru, and to Gwyther Rees who reviewed an early version of this manuscript. 

Declarations

This research was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Demographic and Health Surveys were approved by the Institutional Review Board of ICF (previously known as Macro International), approval ID number FWA00000845. In Mexico, the Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía established a specialized national committee comprised of a wide array of government, academic, civil society and international organizations and advocates with expertise in violence research, prevention and services in order to provide scientific and ethical review for all stages of ENDIREH 2016 survey design and implementation.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

Authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
9.
Zurück zum Zitat World Health Organization. Violence against women prevalence estimates, 2018: global, regional and national prevalence estimates for intimate partner violence against women and global and regional prevalence estimates for non-partner sexual violence against women. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2021. https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/9789240022256. World Health Organization. Violence against women prevalence estimates, 2018: global, regional and national prevalence estimates for intimate partner violence against women and global and regional prevalence estimates for non-partner sexual violence against women. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2021. https://​www.​who.​int/​publications/​i/​item/​9789240022256.
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Pearson I, Page S, Zimmerman C, Meinck F, Gennari F, Guedes A, et al. The co-occurrence of intimate partner violence and violence against children: A systematic review on associated factors in low- and middle-income countries. Trauma Violence Abuse. 2022:15248380221082943 https://doi.org/10.1177/15248380221082943. Pearson I, Page S, Zimmerman C, Meinck F, Gennari F, Guedes A, et al. The co-occurrence of intimate partner violence and violence against children: A systematic review on associated factors in low- and middle-income countries. Trauma Violence Abuse. 2022:15248380221082943 https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​1524838022108294​3.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Gao Y, Mi X, Wang Y, Zou S, Zhou H. Association between household crowding and violent discipline and neglect of children: Analysis of Multiple Indicator Cluster Surveys in 26 low- and middle-income countries. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(4):1685. Gao Y, Mi X, Wang Y, Zou S, Zhou H. Association between household crowding and violent discipline and neglect of children: Analysis of Multiple Indicator Cluster Surveys in 26 low- and middle-income countries. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(4):1685.
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Nazar A, Salvatierra B, Salazar S, Solís R. [Violencia física contra adolescentes y estructura familiar: magnitudes, expresiones y desigualdades.] Physical violence against adolescents and family structure: Scope, expressions and inequalities. Estudios Demográficos y Urbanos. 2018;33(2):365–400. https://doi.org/10.24201/edu.v33i2.1650.CrossRef Nazar A, Salvatierra B, Salazar S, Solís R. [Violencia física contra adolescentes y estructura familiar: magnitudes, expresiones y desigualdades.] Physical violence against adolescents and family structure: Scope, expressions and inequalities. Estudios Demográficos y Urbanos. 2018;33(2):365–400. https://​doi.​org/​10.​24201/​edu.​v33i2.​1650.CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Straus MA. Prevalence, societal causes, and trends in corporal punishment by parents in world perspective. Law Contemp Probl. 2010;73(2):1–30. Straus MA. Prevalence, societal causes, and trends in corporal punishment by parents in world perspective. Law Contemp Probl. 2010;73(2):1–30.
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Alampay L, Godwin J, Lansford J, Oburu P, Bornstein M, Chang L, et al. Change in caregivers’ attitudes and use of corporal punishment following a legal ban: a multi-country longitudinal comparison. Child Maltreatment. 2021:107755952110364 https://doi.org/10.1177/10775595211036401. Alampay L, Godwin J, Lansford J, Oburu P, Bornstein M, Chang L, et al. Change in caregivers’ attitudes and use of corporal punishment following a legal ban: a multi-country longitudinal comparison. Child Maltreatment. 2021:107755952110364 https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​1077559521103640​1.
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Avila-Burgos L, Valdez-Santiago R, Barroso-Quiab A, Hijar M, Rojas R, Del Rio-Zolezzi A. Prevalence and factors associated with intimate partner abuse in female users of public health services in Mexico: a comparative analyses. Rev Invest Clin. 2014;66(1):45–58.PubMed Avila-Burgos L, Valdez-Santiago R, Barroso-Quiab A, Hijar M, Rojas R, Del Rio-Zolezzi A. Prevalence and factors associated with intimate partner abuse in female users of public health services in Mexico: a comparative analyses. Rev Invest Clin. 2014;66(1):45–58.PubMed
88.
Zurück zum Zitat Ranganathan M, Knight L, Abramsky T, Muvhango L, Polzer Ngwato T, Mbobelatsi M, et al. Associations between women's economic and social empowerment and intimate partner violence: findings from a microfinance plus program in rural North West Province, South Africa. J Interpers Violence. 2019:886260519836952 https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260519836952 Ranganathan M, Knight L, Abramsky T, Muvhango L, Polzer Ngwato T, Mbobelatsi M, et al. Associations between women's economic and social empowerment and intimate partner violence: findings from a microfinance plus program in rural North West Province, South Africa. J Interpers Violence. 2019:886260519836952 https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​0886260519836952​
96.
Zurück zum Zitat United Nations Committee on the Rights of the Child. General comment No. 8 (2006): The right of the child to protection from corporal punishment and other cruel or degrading forms of punishment. Geneva: United Nations Committee on the Rights of the Child; 2007. https://www.refworld.org/docid/460bc7772.html. United Nations Committee on the Rights of the Child. General comment No. 8 (2006): The right of the child to protection from corporal punishment and other cruel or degrading forms of punishment. Geneva: United Nations Committee on the Rights of the Child; 2007. https://​www.​refworld.​org/​docid/​460bc7772.​html.
103.
104.
Zurück zum Zitat Instituto Nacional de Salud Pública, UNICEF Mexico. Encuesta Nacional de Niños, Niñas y Mujeres 2015 - Encuesta de Indicadores Múltiples por Conglomerados 2015, Informe Final. Mexico City: Instituto Nacional de Salud Pública and UNICEF Mexico; 2016. https://mics.unicef.org/surveys. Instituto Nacional de Salud Pública, UNICEF Mexico. Encuesta Nacional de Niños, Niñas y Mujeres 2015 - Encuesta de Indicadores Múltiples por Conglomerados 2015, Informe Final. Mexico City: Instituto Nacional de Salud Pública and UNICEF Mexico; 2016. https://​mics.​unicef.​org/​surveys.
Metadaten
Titel
Correlates of co-occurring physical child punishment and physical intimate partner violence in Colombia, Mexico and Peru
verfasst von
Sarah Bott
Ana P. Ruiz-Celis
Jennifer Adams Mendoza
Alessandra Guedes
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2022
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Public Health / Ausgabe 1/2022
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2458
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-14453-6

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2022

BMC Public Health 1/2022 Zur Ausgabe