Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Public Health 1/2022

Open Access 01.12.2022 | Research

Modeling the effects of physical activity, education, health, and subjective wealth on happiness based on Indonesian national survey data

verfasst von: Bhina Patria

Erschienen in: BMC Public Health | Ausgabe 1/2022

Abstract

Background

Studies on physical activity’s psychological benefits are generally fewer than those on its physiological benefits, and these limited studies have mostly investigated its impact on cognitive functions. Studies exclusively investigating physical activity’s effects on happiness are rare. This study aims to investigate the effect of physical activity on psychological functions, especially on happiness.

Methods

Analysis was based on a large field of nationally representative Indonesian adult data. Data were compiled based on face-to-face interviews with 12,051 adults. Participants provided measures of physical activity, subjective health, and happiness, and responses were recorded with computer-assisted personal interviewing (CAPI) software. Demographic data, including gender, subjective wealth, education, and age, were also included in the analysis. Structural equation modeling (SEM) was conducted to determine the relationship between physical activity, health, subjective wealth, and happiness.

Results

The tested model of the association between physical activity, health, subjective wealth, and happiness indicated a good fit, based on χ2 (1, n = 12,051) = 48.733, p = .001, RMSEA = .063, and CFI = .97. Path analysis results showed that health conditions mediated the effects of physical activity on happiness. The result also showed positive effects of education level and subjective wealth on happiness.

Conclusion

This study provides evidence that engagement in physical activity has a positive impact on happiness. Indonesian adults should engage in more active lifestyles since more than one-third of Indonesians did not get enough physical activity.
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Background

Studies on the physiological benefits of physical activity outnumber those of its psychological benefits [1]. Various studies show that regular physical activity has multiple physiological benefits. The intensity of physical activity contributes to lipoprotein profile, carbohydrate metabolism, lower blood pressure, and weight loss [2]. Physical activity also offers protection against cancers of the colon, breast endometrium, pancreas, prostate, lung, and ovary [36]. Other studies reported that physical activity helped control type II [79] and type I diabetes [1013]. Blair [14] even concluded that low cardiorespiratory fitness (CRF) was the highest cause of death than other factors—i.e., high blood pressure, smoking, high cholesterol, diabetes, and obesity.
Studies that reported the psychological effects of physical activity were mostly concentrated on cognitive functions. The effects of regular activity were observed across a variety of cognitive processes. However, the highest were found in the executive control process—i.e., planning, scheduling, working memory, interference control, and task coordination [1517].
In aging adults, regular physical activity maintains cognitive condition and was associated with the decreasing risk of poor cognition and early cognitive decline [1820]. Weuve and colleagues [19] studied the effects of long-term regular physical activity, mainly walking, on cognitive functions of women aged 70–81 (N = 18,766). Results showed that higher levels of regular physical activity were associated with better cognitive performance. Higher cardiorespiratory fitness was also associated with less memory decline across lifespan [19, 21].
Physical activity was also proven to reduce depression [2225], reduce anxiety [23, 26, 27], and protect against stress [28]. A randomized experiment by Babyak and colleagues [22] showed that exercise was as effective as pharmacotherapy for depression. Furthermore, after 10 months, the exercise group had significantly lower relapse rates than the medication group [22].
The present study investigates the effect of physical activity on psychological functions, especially on subjective wellbeing or happiness. The terms subjective wellbeing and happiness are used interchangeably in the literature [29]. This study uses the term happiness because wellbeing usually consists of objective variables such as income and health [2931]. Happiness in this study refers to the overall evaluation of life or life-satisfaction [32], rather than positive emotional states like contentment, joy, and excitement.
Studies examining the effects of physical activity exclusively on happiness are rare [3336]. The World Database of Happiness [37], an online register of research on happiness, listed few studies that associated physical activity with happiness. A study on adolescents’ samples concluded that regular exercise was associated with psychological wellbeing and a lower propensity for eating disorder behaviors [38]. Another study on older adults found that physical exercise programs reduced pain intensity [39]. Participants also reported significant improvement in psychological wellbeing—feeling happier, less lonely, more life satisfaction, and less depression [39]. A study found that the intensity of physical activity was positively related to daily positive affect, which is a known determinant of happiness [40].
Why does physical activity affect happiness? Argyle [29] suggested it is partly due to social interaction with others. Physical activity can increase the opportunity for social relationships, which are noted to have a powerful effect on happiness [29]. Nevertheless, physical activity’s effect on happiness was also found in solitary exercise. Literature has long noted the “runner’s high” phenomena, which is suspected to be caused by the release of endorphins after strenuous physical activity [41]. A study measured the activity of the brain before and after strenuous physical activity. The results showed that a release of endogenous opioids occurred in the frontolimbic brain regions after running, and the level of euphoria was significantly increased [42].
The present study also addressed the effects of other variables related to happiness, such as economic status, education, and health.

Subjective wealth and happiness

The association between income and happiness has been the most enduring debate in the literature on subjective wellbeing [4345]. Studies showed positive and negative effects of wealth on happiness. Earlier studies reported various levels of correlation between income and happiness—in some cases, no relation at all [29, 44, 46]. Studies favoring the positive effects of wealth argued that higher income boosts purchasing power, expands affordable goods, and increases consumption, which leads to improved wellbeing [47]. Experimental studies showed that the increase in income significantly affects the level of happiness. For example, unconditional cash transfers from government or NGO increased recipients’ happiness in Zambia [48] and Kenya [49]. A study using a multilevel model with data from World Values Survey (N = 64,923, k = 81 nations) showed consistent results [50].
In support of the adverse effects of wealth on happiness, a review study noted that income only accounted for 4% of the variance of subjective wellbeing [51]. Higher-income is related to less daily sadness, but not to daily happiness [52]. More recent studies depicted time- and money-spending behaviors as variables that should be accounted for in the relationship between wealth and happiness [48, 5357].
This study used subjective wealth as a proxy for objective wealth. Previous studies showed that subjective wealth is associated with objective wealth [58]. There is also a consensus among researchers that subjective wealth is a predictor of general happiness [58, 59].

Education and happiness

The direct relationship between education and happiness is still unclear, though several studies found correlations between them [6064]. Jongbloed [64] stated that higher education is significantly associated with happiness. One possible explanation is that higher education is associated with longer and healthier lives, successful marriages, higher quality of interpersonal relationships, and better opportunities on the labor market [62, 65]. Another study stated that non-monetary factors also play a role in the relationship between education and happiness, e.g., interpersonal networking and degree of cosmopolitanism. Better-educated people have broader social networks and involvement with the wider world, which is associated with happiness [66].
Nevertheless, an increasing number of studies established an insignificant or declined relationship between higher education and happiness [29, 62]. One study pointed out that income and occupation moderated the association between education and happiness [67]. When income and occupation were controlled, education had a negative effect on happiness [67]. A longitudinal study found similar results; participants with only secondary education (non-vocational) were healthier, happier, and wealthier when compared to other groups [68].
Education’s correlation with happiness seems to be affected by the country’s overall welfare, with high correlations in developing nations and low correlation in rich ones [69]. This is not because education breeds dissatisfaction but possibly due to scarcity of employment that matches the level of education or the fading of earlier advantages in the process of social equalizing [69].

Health and happiness

The hypothesis that health affects happiness is widely accepted. One study found that self-rated health correlated significantly with subjective wellbeing [70]. A recent study based on Asian samples also concluded that self-rated health greatly affected happiness, especially when physical health problems occurred [71]. Another study, based on a community sample of older adults, concluded that health status is one of the most influential predictors of happiness [72]. However, disease severity has little effect on happiness. People with cancer can be happier than people with allergies. A higher relationship was found between happiness and the degree to which disease disrupts daily functioning [72].
Based on the context that physical activity’s psychological benefits are limited and mostly related to cognitive functions, there is a need to examine the effect of physical activity on other psychological functions, i.e., happiness. The present study proposed a model to investigate the association between physical activity and happiness. Based on the aforementioned studies, the model includes other related variables—subjective wealth, health, and education. The analysis also controlled for age and gender. Figure 1 shows the hypothesized relationship model between physical activity, subjective wealth, education, health, and happiness.
$${\displaystyle \begin{array}{c}{\mathcal{y}}_1=\upalpha +{\upgamma \mathcal{x}}_1+{\upgamma \mathcal{x}}_2+{\upgamma \mathcal{x}}_3+{\upzeta}_1\\ {}{\mathcal{y}}_2=\upalpha +{\upgamma \mathcal{x}}_1+{\upgamma \mathcal{x}}_2+{\upgamma \mathcal{x}}_3+{\mathcal{y}}_2\uppsi +{\upzeta}_2\end{array}}$$
Where 𝓎 is the endogenous variable (i.e., 𝓎1: health, 𝓎2: happiness), α is the intercept, γ is the regression coefficient, 𝓍 is the exogenous variable (i.e., 𝓍1: physical activity, 𝓍2: education, 𝓍3: subjective wealth), ψ is the residual variance of the endogenous variable, and ζ is the residual or error in the equation [73].

Materials and methods

This study is a quantitative study using a structural equation modeling (SEM). SEM is a multivariate technique combining aspects of factor analysis and multiple regression that enables the researcher to simultaneously examine a series of interrelated dependence relationships among variables [74]. The data were from a longitudinal socioeconomic and health survey in Indonesia, the Indonesian Family Life Survey (IFLS). IFLS has been conducted five times, in 1993, 1998, 2000, 2007, and 2014–2015. IFLS sampling scheme stratified on provinces and urban/rural location and then randomly sampled within the strata. The sampling method considered the cultural and socioeconomic diversity in Indonesia and represent four most populated islands in Indonesia—Java, Sumatra, Kalimantan, and Sulawesi—containing 83% of the population. The first IFLS sampling frame were based on 1993 SUSENAS (National Socioeconomic Survey), which was based on 1990 census [75].
The IFLS surveys were reviewed and approved by IRBs in the United States and in Indonesia at Universitas Gadjah Mada (UGM). The ethical clearance number from RAND’s Human Subjects Protection Committee (RAND’s IRB) was s0064-06-01-CR01.

Participants

From the total dataset, 12,051 participants were selected for the analysis. The gender composition was 47.4% male (n = 5711) and 52.6% female (n = 6340). Participant inclusion was based on: (1) ages 18 to 65, (2) completion the necessary variables in the questionnaire, and (3) not having a chronic disease (i.e., asthma, cancer or malignant tumor, memory-related disease, high-cholesterol).

Variables

Physical activities

The IFLS (Indonesian Family Life Survey) questionnaire includes physical activity variables in Book IIIB Section KK (Health condition). There are nine questions in the section measuring activities type and their duration. Participants were asked to quantify their activity in the previous 7 days. The levels of physical activity were vigorous activities, moderate physical effort, and walking [76]. Vigorous activities were described as those that made participants breathe much harder than usual, such as heavy lifting, digging, plowing, aerobics, fast bicycling, and cycling with loads. Moderate physical activities were those that made participants breathe somewhat harder than usual, such as carrying light loads, bicycling at a regular pace, and mopping the floor. Walking included daily walking at work, at home, to travel from place to place, or in any other context related to recreation, sport, exercise, or leisure. The intensity of physical activities was measured by the duration per day (less than 30 minutes to more than 4 hours) and the number of days performed in the previous 7 days.
Physical activity data were converted to metabolic equivalent value (MET) according to the Compendium of Physical Activities [77]. One MET is equal to the energy spent when a person sits still. The MET for walking is from two to eight, depending on speed and obstacles. In this study, vigorous physical activities were categorized as multiple household tasks (vigorous) in the Compendium of Physical Activities, which equals four MET. Moderate physical activities were categorized as multiple household tasks (medium), which equals 3.5 MET, while walking was categorized as equal to 2.5 MET. The participants’ METs were afterward converted to MET minutes (MET × 60 seconds). The participants’ average MET minutes were 69.6 (SD = 55.5).

Subjective wealth

Question SW01 from Book IIIA of the IFLS questionnaire measured the subjective wealth of the participants, who were asked to rate their overall wealth from 1 ‘Poorest’ to 6 ‘Richest’. The average response was 2.86 (SD = .8).

Education

Participants were asked about their highest level of education (question DL6 Book IIIA). Among them, 6.6% had at least a bachelor’s degree, and 5.9% had a diploma degree (3 years of vocational education after high school). Table 1 shows the complete statistics of participants’ education levels.
Table 1
Descriptive statistics
Variables
N = 12,051
Age, mean (SD)
34.57 (11.3)
Gender, n (%)
 Male
5711 (47.4)
 Female
6340 (52.6)
Education, n (%)
  ≤ High school
10,777 (89.4)
 Diploma (vocational degree)
405 (5.9)
 Bachelor degree
772 (6.4)
 Master degree
19 (0.2)
Subjective health, n (%)
 Unhealthy
13 (.1)
 Somewhat unhealthy
1362 (11.3)
 Somewhat healthy
9298 (77.2)
 Very healthy
1378 (11.4)
Physical activity, mean (SD)
69.60 (55.5)
Happiness, mean (SD)
2.98 (.4)
Happiness, n (%)
 Very unhappy
29 (.2)
 Unhappy
851 (7.1)
 Happy
10,457 (86.8)
 Very happy
714 (5.9)
Subjective wealth, mean (SD)
2.86(.8)
Subjective wealth, n (%)
 Perceived income ladder 1 & 2
3360 (27.9)
 Perceived income ladder 3 & 4
8554 (71.0)
 Perceived income ladder 5 & 6
137 (1.1)

Health

Participants’ responses to question KK01 in Book IIIB were used to indicate health. Participants were asked about the general condition of their health. More than 88.6% stated that they were somewhat healthy or very healthy. Table 1 presents the complete statistics of the participants’ responses.

Happiness

Participants’ responses to question SW12 of the IFLS Book IIIA were used to indicate happiness. Participants were asked to rate their happiness. The scale ranged from 1 ‘Very unhappy’ to 4 ‘Very happy.’ Participants’ average self-rated happiness was 2.98 (SD = .4). More than 91% of the participants were happy or very happy.

Statistical analysis

In the present study, the model was tested using structural equation modeling or analysis of covariance structure. To assess the fitness of the model, it is necessary to report fit statistics such as χ2 value and degrees of freedom; the CFI (comparative fit index) or TLI (Tucker-Lewis Index); and RMSEA (root mean square error of approximation) [74, 78]. The threshold of fit indices was based on the recommendation of Hair et al. [74]. For a model with less than 12 observed variables and n more than 250, the suggested values are CFI ≥ .97 and RMSEA < .07 [74].

Results

When the hypothesized model was fitted to the data, the following fit indices resulted: χ2 (1, N = 12,051) = 48.733, p = .001, RMSEA = .063, CFI = .970. This fulfilled the requirements for a good model fit by Hair and colleagues [74]. Figure 2 depicts the structural model.
The analysis was controlled for participants’ gender. Studies show that gender difference is relevant in happiness studies. Table 2 shows the fit statistics of the final model by gender.
Table 2
Fit statistics of the model by gender and field of study
 
N
χ2
df
RMSEA
CFI
Male
5711
14.557
1
.049
.985
Female
6340
28,250
1
.066
.964
Total data
12,051
48,733
1
.063
.970
As shown in Table 3, all standardized regression weights are positively significant except for the path from physical activity to happiness which is negatively significant. Table 3 and Fig. 2 show the mediating effects of health in the relationship between physical activity and happiness. The highest effect was found in the path from subjective wealth to happiness (β = .13), followed by health to happiness (β = .095), and education to happiness (β = .095). Education, subjective wealth, and physical activity positively affect health (β = .049, β = .048, and β = .025, respectively). The value above happiness (.04) in Fig. 2 is the square of multiple correlations and indicates the variability in happiness that is explained by other variables in the model. This means the variables in the model explained only 4% of the variability in happiness.
Table 3
Standardized regression weight of the predictors on happiness
 
β
SE
CR
p*
Subjective wealth - > Health
.048
.006
5.133
.000
Physical activity - > Health
.025
.000
2.736
.006
Education - > Health
.048
.004
5.111
.000
Subjective wealth - > Happiness
.130
.004
14.263
.000
Health - > Happiness
.095
.007
10.678
.000
Education - > Happiness
.092
.003
9.911
.000
Physical activity - > Happiness
−.020
.000
−2.259
.024
Note. * one-tailed. SE Standard error, CR Critical ratio

Discussion

Based on data from a large survey in Indonesia, the present study examined the relationship between physical activity, subjective wealth, health, education, and happiness. Based on the structural model, the overall analysis showed that the hypothesized model is a good fit for the dataset.
Further analysis found that physical activity did not directly affect happiness. Physical activity has a negative effect on physical activity. However, health conditions mediated the relationship between physical activity and happiness. This result is consistent with prior studies on the mediating effects of health on happiness [33, 79, 80]. Few studies have investigated the mediating effect of health in general populations [79]; most focused on older populations [80, 81]. The present study makes a noteworthy contribution since the dataset was based on a large sample from the general population of a developing country.
A recent review highlighted the mediating effects of health on the relationship between physical activity and happiness [33]. Engaging in physical activity contributes to the perception of good health, thus potentially improving happiness [79]. Other studies also showed an indirect association between physical activity and happiness, mediated by health and social functioning [80]. A possible explanation is that less physically active people, such as physically limited or ill older adults, face difficulty socializing with others. This leads to poor social functioning related to low levels of happiness [80].
The negative effect of physical activity on happiness may partly be explained by the participants’ diverse physical activity levels. A high level of physical activity is often associated with a blue-collar occupation [82], lower socioeconomic status, and a low level of happiness. Further analysis of the model by controlling the level of physical activity—including only participants who have recommended physical activity [83, 84]—supported this argument. After controlling the level of physical activity, the model showed a non-significant direct effect of physical activity on happiness.
Another contribution of the present study is the use of a structural model rarely used by previous studies investigating physical activity and happiness. Structural equation modeling is the appropriate method for evaluating a series of simultaneous hypotheses about the impacts of latent and manifest variables on other variables while taking measurement errors into account [74, 85]. Therefore, the present study’s structural modeling gives a better understanding of the relationships between physical activity, education, subjective wealth, health, and happiness.
In the present study, the highest regression coefficient was in the path from subjective wealth to happiness. This finding supports prior studies regarding subjective wealth’s positive effects on happiness [43, 45, 50]. Classic economic theories posit that higher-income boosts purchasing power, expands affordable goods, and increases consumption, leading to improved wellbeing [47]. One study showed that wealth is positively and significantly associated with happiness in low-income and high-income countries; however, higher effects were found in the former than in the latter [50]. This relates to the ceiling effect on the relationship between income and happiness—i.e., income satiation. After reaching a certain level, income no longer affects happiness [57].
The structural model also showed a significant positive effect of education on happiness. These results support previous research on the association between education and happiness [60, 61, 6365, 86]. However, an increasing number of studies depict an insignificant relationship between education and happiness [6769]. One possible explanation for this is that most of these studies use life satisfaction as a proxy for happiness [86], which is inaccurate because it only measures the evaluative dimension of happiness, excluding the hedonic and eudaimonic dimensions [86].
Several limitations must be noted regarding the present study. First, most variables were based on subjective self-report, which is open to biases. Responses might be affected by social norms [29]; e.g., participants might under-report their wealth because humbleness is valued under Indonesian social norms. The second is that, evengthough SEM previously used to indicate a causality evidence [87], the cross-sectional design of this study prohibiting evidence for directionality relations. Future studies should consider using objective measurements—i.e., for health and wealth. The use of latent variables should also be considered. Including variables such as religiosity and social relationships would be interesting, especially in the Indonesian context.

Conclusion

This study suggests that health mediates physical activity’s effects on happiness. Active individuals would have better health compared to their sedentary counterparts. Health condition, as suggested by the model, was one of the biggest predictors of happiness. The fact that physical activity positively affects happiness should also motivate Indonesians to engage in more active lifestyles. This is important since the national health survey revealed that more than one-third (33.5%) of Indonesians did not get enough physical activity [88].

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Declarations

The research methods were performed in accordance with the guidelines and regulations of The Declaration of Helsinki. The IFLS surveys were reviewed and approved by IRBs in the United States and in Indonesia at Universitas Gadjah Mada (UGM). The ethical clearance number from RAND’s Human Subjects Protection Committee (RAND’s IRB) was s0064-06-01-CR01. All requirements for consent were met and approved by those IRBs. The data used in this study were anonymized before its use.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The author declares no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Brannon L, Feist J, Updegraff J. Health psychology: an introduction to behavior and health. Wadsworth: Cengage Learning; 2014. Brannon L, Feist J, Updegraff J. Health psychology: an introduction to behavior and health. Wadsworth: Cengage Learning; 2014.
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Blair SN, Kampert JB, Kohl HW III, et al. Influences of cardiorespiratory fitness and other precursors on cardiovascular disease and all-cause mortality in men and women. JAMA. 1996;276(3):205–10.PubMedCrossRef Blair SN, Kampert JB, Kohl HW III, et al. Influences of cardiorespiratory fitness and other precursors on cardiovascular disease and all-cause mortality in men and women. JAMA. 1996;276(3):205–10.PubMedCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Miles L. Physical activity and the prevention of cancer: a review of recent findings. Nutr Bull. 2007;32(3):250–82.CrossRef Miles L. Physical activity and the prevention of cancer: a review of recent findings. Nutr Bull. 2007;32(3):250–82.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Kruk J, Czerniak U. Physical activity and its relation to cancer risk: updating the evidence. Asian Pac J Cancer Prev. 2013;14(7):3993–4003.PubMedCrossRef Kruk J, Czerniak U. Physical activity and its relation to cancer risk: updating the evidence. Asian Pac J Cancer Prev. 2013;14(7):3993–4003.PubMedCrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Rezende LFM, Sa TH, Markozannes G, Rey-Lopez JP, Lee IM, Tsilidis KK, et al. Physical activity and cancer: an umbrella review of the literature including 22 major anatomical sites and 770 000 cancer cases. Br J Sports Med. 2018;52(13):826–33.PubMedCrossRef Rezende LFM, Sa TH, Markozannes G, Rey-Lopez JP, Lee IM, Tsilidis KK, et al. Physical activity and cancer: an umbrella review of the literature including 22 major anatomical sites and 770 000 cancer cases. Br J Sports Med. 2018;52(13):826–33.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Araiza P, Hewes H, Gashetewa C, Vella CA, Burge MR. Efficacy of a pedometer-based physical activity program on parameters of diabetes control in type 2 diabetes mellitus. Metab Clin Exp. 2006;55(10):1382–7.PubMedCrossRef Araiza P, Hewes H, Gashetewa C, Vella CA, Burge MR. Efficacy of a pedometer-based physical activity program on parameters of diabetes control in type 2 diabetes mellitus. Metab Clin Exp. 2006;55(10):1382–7.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Way KL, Hackett DA, Baker MK, Johnson NA. The effect of regular exercise on insulin sensitivity in type 2 diabetes mellitus: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Diabetes Metab J. 2016;40(4):253–71.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Way KL, Hackett DA, Baker MK, Johnson NA. The effect of regular exercise on insulin sensitivity in type 2 diabetes mellitus: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Diabetes Metab J. 2016;40(4):253–71.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Asano RY, Sales MM, Browne RA, Moraes JF, Coelho Junior HJ, Moraes MR, et al. Acute effects of physical exercise in type 2 diabetes: a review. World J Diabetes. 2014;5(5):659–65.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Asano RY, Sales MM, Browne RA, Moraes JF, Coelho Junior HJ, Moraes MR, et al. Acute effects of physical exercise in type 2 diabetes: a review. World J Diabetes. 2014;5(5):659–65.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Conn VS, Hafdahl AR, LeMaster J, Ruppar T, Cochran J, Nielsen P. Meta-analysis of health behavior change interventions in type 1 diabetes. Am J Health Behav. 2008;32(3):315–29.PubMedCrossRef Conn VS, Hafdahl AR, LeMaster J, Ruppar T, Cochran J, Nielsen P. Meta-analysis of health behavior change interventions in type 1 diabetes. Am J Health Behav. 2008;32(3):315–29.PubMedCrossRef
11.
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Burr JF, Shephard RJ, Riddell MC. Physical activity in type 1 diabetes mellitus: assessing risks for physical activity clearance and prescription. Can Fam Physician. 2012;58(5):533–5.PubMedPubMedCentral Burr JF, Shephard RJ, Riddell MC. Physical activity in type 1 diabetes mellitus: assessing risks for physical activity clearance and prescription. Can Fam Physician. 2012;58(5):533–5.PubMedPubMedCentral
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Colberg SR, Sigal RJ, Yardley JE, Riddell MC, Dunstan DW, Dempsey PC, et al. Physical activity/exercise and diabetes: a position statement of the American Diabetes Association. Diabetes Care. 2016;39(11):2065–79.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Colberg SR, Sigal RJ, Yardley JE, Riddell MC, Dunstan DW, Dempsey PC, et al. Physical activity/exercise and diabetes: a position statement of the American Diabetes Association. Diabetes Care. 2016;39(11):2065–79.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Blair SN. Physical inactivity: the biggest public health problem of the 21st century. Br J Sports Med. 2009;43(1):1–2.PubMed Blair SN. Physical inactivity: the biggest public health problem of the 21st century. Br J Sports Med. 2009;43(1):1–2.PubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Kramer AF, Colcombe SJ, McAuley E, Scalf PE, Erickson KI. Fitness, aging and neurocognitive function. Neurobiol Aging. 2005;26(1, Supplement):124–7.PubMedCrossRef Kramer AF, Colcombe SJ, McAuley E, Scalf PE, Erickson KI. Fitness, aging and neurocognitive function. Neurobiol Aging. 2005;26(1, Supplement):124–7.PubMedCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Mandolesi L, Polverino A, Montuori S, Foti F, Ferraioli G, Sorrentino P, et al. Effects of physical exercise on cognitive functioning and wellbeing: biological and psychological benefits. Front Psychol. 2018;9:509.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mandolesi L, Polverino A, Montuori S, Foti F, Ferraioli G, Sorrentino P, et al. Effects of physical exercise on cognitive functioning and wellbeing: biological and psychological benefits. Front Psychol. 2018;9:509.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Cox EP, O'Dwyer N, Cook R, Vetter M, Cheng HL, Rooney K, et al. Relationship between physical activity and cognitive function in apparently healthy young to middle-aged adults: a systematic review. J Sci Med Sport. 2016;19(8):616–28.PubMedCrossRef Cox EP, O'Dwyer N, Cook R, Vetter M, Cheng HL, Rooney K, et al. Relationship between physical activity and cognitive function in apparently healthy young to middle-aged adults: a systematic review. J Sci Med Sport. 2016;19(8):616–28.PubMedCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P, McAuley E, Cohen NJ, et al. Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(9):3316–21.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P, McAuley E, Cohen NJ, et al. Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(9):3316–21.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Weuve J, Kang JH, Manson JE, Breteler MM, Ware JH, Grodstein F. Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women. JAMA. 2004;292(12):1454–61.PubMedCrossRef Weuve J, Kang JH, Manson JE, Breteler MM, Ware JH, Grodstein F. Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women. JAMA. 2004;292(12):1454–61.PubMedCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Lautenschlager NT, Cox KL, Flicker L, Foster JK, van Bockxmeer FM, Xiao J, et al. Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease: a randomized trial. JAMA. 2008;300(9):1027–37.PubMedCrossRef Lautenschlager NT, Cox KL, Flicker L, Foster JK, van Bockxmeer FM, Xiao J, et al. Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease: a randomized trial. JAMA. 2008;300(9):1027–37.PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Wendell CR, Gunstad J, Waldstein SR, Wright JG, Ferrucci L, Zonderman AB. Cardiorespiratory fitness and accelerated cognitive decline with aging. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69(4):455–62.PubMedCrossRef Wendell CR, Gunstad J, Waldstein SR, Wright JG, Ferrucci L, Zonderman AB. Cardiorespiratory fitness and accelerated cognitive decline with aging. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69(4):455–62.PubMedCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Babyak M, Blumenthal JA, Herman S, Khatri P, Doraiswamy M, Moore K, et al. Exercise treatment for major depression: maintenance of therapeutic benefit at 10 months. Psychosom Med. 2000;62(5):633–8.PubMedCrossRef Babyak M, Blumenthal JA, Herman S, Khatri P, Doraiswamy M, Moore K, et al. Exercise treatment for major depression: maintenance of therapeutic benefit at 10 months. Psychosom Med. 2000;62(5):633–8.PubMedCrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Martinsen EW. Physical activity in the prevention and treatment of anxiety and depression. Nord J Psychiatry. 2008;62(s47):25–9.PubMedCrossRef Martinsen EW. Physical activity in the prevention and treatment of anxiety and depression. Nord J Psychiatry. 2008;62(s47):25–9.PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Mayor S. Physical activity reduces risk of depression at any age, finds study. BMJ. 2018;361:k1808.PubMedCrossRef Mayor S. Physical activity reduces risk of depression at any age, finds study. BMJ. 2018;361:k1808.PubMedCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Stonerock GL, Hoffman BM, Smith PJ, Blumenthal JA. Exercise as treatment for anxiety: systematic review and analysis. Ann Behav Med. 2015;49(4):542–56.PubMedCrossRef Stonerock GL, Hoffman BM, Smith PJ, Blumenthal JA. Exercise as treatment for anxiety: systematic review and analysis. Ann Behav Med. 2015;49(4):542–56.PubMedCrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Paxton R, Motl R, Aylward A, Nigg C. Physical activity and quality of life—the complementary influence of self-efficacy for physical activity and mental health difficulties. Int J Behav Med. 2010;17(4):255–63.PubMedCrossRef Paxton R, Motl R, Aylward A, Nigg C. Physical activity and quality of life—the complementary influence of self-efficacy for physical activity and mental health difficulties. Int J Behav Med. 2010;17(4):255–63.PubMedCrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Argyle M. The psychology of happiness. Sussex: Routledge; 2001. Argyle M. The psychology of happiness. Sussex: Routledge; 2001.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Diener E, Lucas RE, Schimmack U, Helliwell JF. Defining well-being: Well-Being for Public Policy: Oxford University Press; 2009. Diener E, Lucas RE, Schimmack U, Helliwell JF. Defining well-being: Well-Being for Public Policy: Oxford University Press; 2009.
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Helliwell JF, Putnam RD. The social context of well-being. In: Huppert FA, Baylis N, Keverne B, editors. The science of well-being: Oxford University Press; 2005. Helliwell JF, Putnam RD. The social context of well-being. In: Huppert FA, Baylis N, Keverne B, editors. The science of well-being: Oxford University Press; 2005.
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Marks N, Shah H. A well-being manifesto for a flourishing society. In: Huppert FA, Baylis N, Keverne B, editors. The science of well-being: Oxford University Press; 2005. Marks N, Shah H. A well-being manifesto for a flourishing society. In: Huppert FA, Baylis N, Keverne B, editors. The science of well-being: Oxford University Press; 2005.
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang Z, Chen W. A systematic review of the relationship between physical activity and happiness. J Happiness Stud. 2018;20:1305–22. Zhang Z, Chen W. A systematic review of the relationship between physical activity and happiness. J Happiness Stud. 2018;20:1305–22.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Richards J, Jiang X, Kelly P, Chau J, Bauman A, Ding D. Don't worry, be happy: cross-sectional associations between physical activity and happiness in 15 European countries. BMC Public Health. 2015;15:53.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Richards J, Jiang X, Kelly P, Chau J, Bauman A, Ding D. Don't worry, be happy: cross-sectional associations between physical activity and happiness in 15 European countries. BMC Public Health. 2015;15:53.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Harris MA. The relationship between physical inactivity and mental wellbeing: findings from a gamification-based community-wide physical activity intervention. Health Psychol Open. 2018;5(1):2055102917753853.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Harris MA. The relationship between physical inactivity and mental wellbeing: findings from a gamification-based community-wide physical activity intervention. Health Psychol Open. 2018;5(1):2055102917753853.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Lapa TY. Physical activity levels and psychological well-being: a case study of university students. Procedia Soc Behav Sci. 2015;186:739–43.CrossRef Lapa TY. Physical activity levels and psychological well-being: a case study of university students. Procedia Soc Behav Sci. 2015;186:739–43.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Veenhoven R. World database of happiness. Soc Indic Res. 1995;34(3):299–313.CrossRef Veenhoven R. World database of happiness. Soc Indic Res. 1995;34(3):299–313.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Gomes R, Gonçalves S, Costa J. Exercise, eating disordered behaviors and psychological well-being: a study with Portuguese adolescents. Rev Latinoam Psicol. 2015;47(1):66–74. Gomes R, Gonçalves S, Costa J. Exercise, eating disordered behaviors and psychological well-being: a study with Portuguese adolescents. Rev Latinoam Psicol. 2015;47(1):66–74.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Tse MMY, Tang SK, Wan VTC, Vong SKS. The effectiveness of physical exercise training in pain, mobility, and psychological well-being of older persons living in nursing homes. Pain Manag Nurs. 2014;15(4):778–88.PubMedCrossRef Tse MMY, Tang SK, Wan VTC, Vong SKS. The effectiveness of physical exercise training in pain, mobility, and psychological well-being of older persons living in nursing homes. Pain Manag Nurs. 2014;15(4):778–88.PubMedCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Fortier MS, Guerin E, Williams T, Strachan S. Should I exercise or sleep to feel better? A daily analysis with physically active working mothers. Ment Health Phys Act. 2015;8(0):56–61.CrossRef Fortier MS, Guerin E, Williams T, Strachan S. Should I exercise or sleep to feel better? A daily analysis with physically active working mothers. Ment Health Phys Act. 2015;8(0):56–61.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Dishman RK, O'Connor PJ. Lessons in exercise neurobiology: the case of endorphins. Ment Health Phys Act. 2009;2(1):4–9.CrossRef Dishman RK, O'Connor PJ. Lessons in exercise neurobiology: the case of endorphins. Ment Health Phys Act. 2009;2(1):4–9.CrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Boecker H, Sprenger T, Spilker ME, Henriksen G, Koppenhoefer M, Wagner KJ, et al. The runner’s high: opioidergic mechanisms in the human brain. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(11):2523–31.PubMedCrossRef Boecker H, Sprenger T, Spilker ME, Henriksen G, Koppenhoefer M, Wagner KJ, et al. The runner’s high: opioidergic mechanisms in the human brain. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(11):2523–31.PubMedCrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Lucas RE, Schimmack U. Income and well-being: how big is the gap between the rich and the poor? J Res Pers. 2009;43(1):75–8.CrossRef Lucas RE, Schimmack U. Income and well-being: how big is the gap between the rich and the poor? J Res Pers. 2009;43(1):75–8.CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Piff PK, Moskowitz JP. Wealth, poverty, and happiness: social class is differentially associated with positive emotions. Emotion. 2018;18(6):902–5.PubMedCrossRef Piff PK, Moskowitz JP. Wealth, poverty, and happiness: social class is differentially associated with positive emotions. Emotion. 2018;18(6):902–5.PubMedCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Gere J, Schimmack U. Benefits of income: associations with life satisfaction among earners and homemakers. Personal Individ Differ. 2017;119:92–5.CrossRef Gere J, Schimmack U. Benefits of income: associations with life satisfaction among earners and homemakers. Personal Individ Differ. 2017;119:92–5.CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Kahneman D, Krueger AB, Schkade D, Schwarz N, Stone AA. Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing illusion. Science. 2006;312(5782):1908–10.PubMedCrossRef Kahneman D, Krueger AB, Schkade D, Schwarz N, Stone AA. Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing illusion. Science. 2006;312(5782):1908–10.PubMedCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Howell R, Howell CJ. The relation of economic status to subjective well-being in developing countries: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2008;134(4):536–60.PubMedCrossRef Howell R, Howell CJ. The relation of economic status to subjective well-being in developing countries: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2008;134(4):536–60.PubMedCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Natali L, Handa S, Peterman A, Seidenfeld D, Tembo G. Zambia cash transfer evaluation T. does money buy happiness? Evidence from an unconditional cash transfer in Zambia. SSM Popul Health. 2018;4:225–35.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Natali L, Handa S, Peterman A, Seidenfeld D, Tembo G. Zambia cash transfer evaluation T. does money buy happiness? Evidence from an unconditional cash transfer in Zambia. SSM Popul Health. 2018;4:225–35.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Haushofer J, Shapiro J. The short-term impact of unconditional cash transfers to the poor: experimental evidence from Kenya*. Q J Econ. 2016;131(4):1973–2042.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Haushofer J, Shapiro J. The short-term impact of unconditional cash transfers to the poor: experimental evidence from Kenya*. Q J Econ. 2016;131(4):1973–2042.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Sarracino F. Determinants of subjective well-being in high and low-income countries: do happiness equations differ across countries? 2013. p. 51–66. Sarracino F. Determinants of subjective well-being in high and low-income countries: do happiness equations differ across countries? 2013. p. 51–66.
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Lucas RE, Dyrenforth PS. Does the existence of social relationships matter for subjective well-being? In: Finkel EJ, Vohs KD, editors. Self and relationships: connecting intrapersonal and interpersonal processes. New York: Guilford Press; 2006. p. 254–73. Lucas RE, Dyrenforth PS. Does the existence of social relationships matter for subjective well-being? In: Finkel EJ, Vohs KD, editors. Self and relationships: connecting intrapersonal and interpersonal processes. New York: Guilford Press; 2006. p. 254–73.
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Kushlev K, Dunn EW, Lucas RE. Higher income is associated with less daily sadness but not more daily happiness. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2015;6(5):483–9.CrossRef Kushlev K, Dunn EW, Lucas RE. Higher income is associated with less daily sadness but not more daily happiness. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2015;6(5):483–9.CrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Aaker JL, Rudd M, Mogilner C. If money does not make you happy, consider time. J Consum Psychol. 2011;21(2):126–30.CrossRef Aaker JL, Rudd M, Mogilner C. If money does not make you happy, consider time. J Consum Psychol. 2011;21(2):126–30.CrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Dunn EW, Gilbert DT, Wilson TD. If money doesn't make you happy, then you probably aren't spending it right. J Consum Psychol. 2011;21(2):115–25.CrossRef Dunn EW, Gilbert DT, Wilson TD. If money doesn't make you happy, then you probably aren't spending it right. J Consum Psychol. 2011;21(2):115–25.CrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaun DE. Income and happiness: earning and spending as sources of discontent. J Socio-Econ. 2005;34(2):161–77.CrossRef Kaun DE. Income and happiness: earning and spending as sources of discontent. J Socio-Econ. 2005;34(2):161–77.CrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Boyce CJ, Daly M, Hounkpatin HO, Wood AM. Money may buy happiness, but often so little that it Doesn't matter. Psychol Sci. 2017;28(4):544–6.PubMedCrossRef Boyce CJ, Daly M, Hounkpatin HO, Wood AM. Money may buy happiness, but often so little that it Doesn't matter. Psychol Sci. 2017;28(4):544–6.PubMedCrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Jebb AT, Tay L, Diener E, Oishi S. Happiness, income satiation and turning points around the world. Nat Hum Behav. 2018;2(1):33–8.PubMedCrossRef Jebb AT, Tay L, Diener E, Oishi S. Happiness, income satiation and turning points around the world. Nat Hum Behav. 2018;2(1):33–8.PubMedCrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Gasiorowska A. The relationship between objective and subjective wealth is moderated by financial control and mediated by money anxiety. J Econ Psychol. 2014;43:64–74.CrossRef Gasiorowska A. The relationship between objective and subjective wealth is moderated by financial control and mediated by money anxiety. J Econ Psychol. 2014;43:64–74.CrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Joo SH, Grable JE. An exploratory framework of the determinants of financial satisfaction. J Fam Econ Iss. 2004;25(1):25–50.CrossRef Joo SH, Grable JE. An exploratory framework of the determinants of financial satisfaction. J Fam Econ Iss. 2004;25(1):25–50.CrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Nikolaev B, Rusakov P. Education and happiness: an alternative hypothesis. Appl Econ Lett. 2015;23(12):827–30.CrossRef Nikolaev B, Rusakov P. Education and happiness: an alternative hypothesis. Appl Econ Lett. 2015;23(12):827–30.CrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Swiatek L. Education for sustainable happiness and well-being. High Educ Res Dev. 2017;36(6):1307–9.CrossRef Swiatek L. Education for sustainable happiness and well-being. High Educ Res Dev. 2017;36(6):1307–9.CrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Nikolaev B. Does higher education increase hedonic and Eudaimonic happiness? J Happiness Stud. 2016;19:483–504. Nikolaev B. Does higher education increase hedonic and Eudaimonic happiness? J Happiness Stud. 2016;19:483–504.
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruiu G, Ruiu ML. The complex relationship between education and happiness: the case of highly educated individuals in Italy. J Happiness Stud. 2018;20:2631–53. Ruiu G, Ruiu ML. The complex relationship between education and happiness: the case of highly educated individuals in Italy. J Happiness Stud. 2018;20:2631–53.
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Jongbloed J. Higher education for happiness? Investigating the impact of education on the hedonic and eudaimonic well-being of Europeans. Eur Educ Res J. 2018;17(5):733–54.CrossRef Jongbloed J. Higher education for happiness? Investigating the impact of education on the hedonic and eudaimonic well-being of Europeans. Eur Educ Res J. 2018;17(5):733–54.CrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Oreopoulos P, Salvanes KG. Priceless: the nonpecuniary benefits of schooling. J Econ Perspect. 2011;25(1):159–84.CrossRef Oreopoulos P, Salvanes KG. Priceless: the nonpecuniary benefits of schooling. J Econ Perspect. 2011;25(1):159–84.CrossRef
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen W-c. How education enhances happiness: comparison of mediating factors in four east Asian countries. Soc Indic Res. 2012;106(1):117–31.CrossRef Chen W-c. How education enhances happiness: comparison of mediating factors in four east Asian countries. Soc Indic Res. 2012;106(1):117–31.CrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Clark AE, Oswald AJ. Satisfaction and comparison income. J Public Econ. 1996;61(3):359–81.CrossRef Clark AE, Oswald AJ. Satisfaction and comparison income. J Public Econ. 1996;61(3):359–81.CrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Hartog J, Oosterbeek H. Health, wealth and happiness: why pursue a higher education? Econ Educ Rev. 1998;17(3):245–56.CrossRef Hartog J, Oosterbeek H. Health, wealth and happiness: why pursue a higher education? Econ Educ Rev. 1998;17(3):245–56.CrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Veenhoven R. Developments in satisfaction-research. Soc Indic Res. 1996;37(1):1–46.CrossRef Veenhoven R. Developments in satisfaction-research. Soc Indic Res. 1996;37(1):1–46.CrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Okun MA, George LK. Physician- and self-ratings of health, neuroticism and subjective well-being among men and women. Personal Individ Differ. 1984;5(5):533–9.CrossRef Okun MA, George LK. Physician- and self-ratings of health, neuroticism and subjective well-being among men and women. Personal Individ Differ. 1984;5(5):533–9.CrossRef
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Park JH, Lee KS. Self-rated health and its determinants in Japan and South Korea. Public Health. 2013;127(9):834–43.PubMedCrossRef Park JH, Lee KS. Self-rated health and its determinants in Japan and South Korea. Public Health. 2013;127(9):834–43.PubMedCrossRef
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Angner E, Ghandhi J, Purvis KW, Amante D, Allison J. Daily functioning, health status, and happiness in older adults. J Happiness Stud. 2013;14:1563–74.CrossRef Angner E, Ghandhi J, Purvis KW, Amante D, Allison J. Daily functioning, health status, and happiness in older adults. J Happiness Stud. 2013;14:1563–74.CrossRef
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Jöreskog KG, Olsson UH, Wallentin FY. Multivariate analysis with LISREL: Springer International Publishing; 2016.CrossRef Jöreskog KG, Olsson UH, Wallentin FY. Multivariate analysis with LISREL: Springer International Publishing; 2016.CrossRef
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Hair JF, Black WC, Babin BJ, Anderson RE. Multivariate data analysis: Pearson Education Limited; 2014. p. 734. Hair JF, Black WC, Babin BJ, Anderson RE. Multivariate data analysis: Pearson Education Limited; 2014. p. 734.
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Strauss J, Witoelar F, Sikoki B. The fifth wave of the Indonesia family life survey: overview and field report: RAND Corporation; 2016. Strauss J, Witoelar F, Sikoki B. The fifth wave of the Indonesia family life survey: overview and field report: RAND Corporation; 2016.
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Strauss J, Witoelar F, Sikoki B, Wattie AM. The fourth wave of the Indonesia family life survey (IFLS4): overview and field report; 2009. Strauss J, Witoelar F, Sikoki B, Wattie AM. The fourth wave of the Indonesia family life survey (IFLS4): overview and field report; 2009.
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Ainsworth BE, Haskell WL, Leon AS, Jacobs DR Jr, Montoye HJ, Sallis JF, et al. Compendium of physical activities: classification of energy costs of human physical activities. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1993;25(1):71–80. Ainsworth BE, Haskell WL, Leon AS, Jacobs DR Jr, Montoye HJ, Sallis JF, et al. Compendium of physical activities: classification of energy costs of human physical activities. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1993;25(1):71–80.
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Brown TA. Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. 2nd ed. New York: The Guilford Press; 2015. p. 462. Brown TA. Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. 2nd ed. New York: The Guilford Press; 2015. p. 462.
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Ross A, Cloutier S, Searle M. The association between leisure time physical activity and happiness: testing the indirect role of health perception. J Community Psychol. 2019;47(5):1169–83.PubMedCrossRef Ross A, Cloutier S, Searle M. The association between leisure time physical activity and happiness: testing the indirect role of health perception. J Community Psychol. 2019;47(5):1169–83.PubMedCrossRef
80.
Zurück zum Zitat de Souto BP. Direct and indirect relationships between physical activity and happiness levels among older adults: a cross-sectional study. Aging Ment Health. 2014;18(7):861–8.CrossRef de Souto BP. Direct and indirect relationships between physical activity and happiness levels among older adults: a cross-sectional study. Aging Ment Health. 2014;18(7):861–8.CrossRef
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Lera-López F, Ollo-López A, Sánchez-Santos JM. How does physical activity make you feel better? The mediational role of perceived health. Appl Res Qual Life. 2017;12(3):511–31.CrossRef Lera-López F, Ollo-López A, Sánchez-Santos JM. How does physical activity make you feel better? The mediational role of perceived health. Appl Res Qual Life. 2017;12(3):511–31.CrossRef
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Korshøj M, Gupta N, Mortensen OS, Jørgensen MB, Holtermann A. Intensity of occupational physical activity in blue-collar workers: do self-reported rating and device-worn measurements agree? Eur J Appl Physiol. 2022;22(5):1293–301.CrossRef Korshøj M, Gupta N, Mortensen OS, Jørgensen MB, Holtermann A. Intensity of occupational physical activity in blue-collar workers: do self-reported rating and device-worn measurements agree? Eur J Appl Physiol. 2022;22(5):1293–301.CrossRef
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Jakicic JM, Otto AD. Physical activity considerations for the treatment and prevention of obesity. Am J Clin Nutr. 2005;82(1):226S–9S.PubMedCrossRef Jakicic JM, Otto AD. Physical activity considerations for the treatment and prevention of obesity. Am J Clin Nutr. 2005;82(1):226S–9S.PubMedCrossRef
84.
Zurück zum Zitat WHO. Global recommendations on physical activity for health. Switzerland: WHO; 2010. WHO. Global recommendations on physical activity for health. Switzerland: WHO; 2010.
85.
Zurück zum Zitat Lee S-Y. Structural equation modeling: a Bayesian approach. Sussex: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2007.CrossRef Lee S-Y. Structural equation modeling: a Bayesian approach. Sussex: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2007.CrossRef
86.
Zurück zum Zitat Nikolaev B. Does higher education increase hedonic and Eudaimonic happiness? J Happiness Stud. 2018;19(2):483–504. Nikolaev B. Does higher education increase hedonic and Eudaimonic happiness? J Happiness Stud. 2018;19(2):483–504.
87.
Zurück zum Zitat Bullock HE, Harlow LL, Mulaik SA. Causation issues in structural equation modeling research. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. 1994;1(3):253–67.CrossRef Bullock HE, Harlow LL, Mulaik SA. Causation issues in structural equation modeling research. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. 1994;1(3):253–67.CrossRef
88.
Zurück zum Zitat MoH. Laporan nasional Riskesdas 2018. Jakarta: Badan Penelitian dan Pengembangan Kesehatan Kementrian Kesehatan RI; 2018. MoH. Laporan nasional Riskesdas 2018. Jakarta: Badan Penelitian dan Pengembangan Kesehatan Kementrian Kesehatan RI; 2018.
Metadaten
Titel
Modeling the effects of physical activity, education, health, and subjective wealth on happiness based on Indonesian national survey data
verfasst von
Bhina Patria
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2022
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Public Health / Ausgabe 1/2022
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2458
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-13371-x

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2022

BMC Public Health 1/2022 Zur Ausgabe