Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Implementation Science 1/2011

Open Access 01.12.2011 | Research

Applying psychological theories to evidence-based clinical practice: identifying factors predictive of lumbar spine x-ray for low back pain in UK primary care practice

verfasst von: Jeremy M Grimshaw, Martin P Eccles, Nick Steen, Marie Johnston, Nigel B Pitts, Liz Glidewell, Graeme Maclennan, Ruth Thomas, Debbie Bonetti, Anne Walker

Erschienen in: Implementation Science | Ausgabe 1/2011

Abstract

Background

Psychological models predict behaviour in a wide range of settings. The aim of this study was to explore the usefulness of a range of psychological models to predict the health professional behaviour 'referral for lumbar spine x-ray in patients presenting with low back pain' by UK primary care physicians.

Methods

Psychological measures were collected by postal questionnaire survey from a random sample of primary care physicians in Scotland and north England. The outcome measures were clinical behaviour (referral rates for lumbar spine x-rays), behavioural simulation (lumbar spine x-ray referral decisions based upon scenarios), and behavioural intention (general intention to refer for lumbar spine x-rays in patients with low back pain). Explanatory variables were the constructs within the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB), Social Cognitive Theory (SCT), Common Sense Self-Regulation Model (CS-SRM), Operant Learning Theory (OLT), Implementation Intention (II), Weinstein's Stage Model termed the Precaution Adoption Process (PAP), and knowledge. For each of the outcome measures, a generalised linear model was used to examine the predictive value of each theory individually. Linear regression was used for the intention and simulation outcomes, and negative binomial regression was used for the behaviour outcome. Following this 'theory level' analysis, a 'cross-theoretical construct' analysis was conducted to investigate the combined predictive value of all individual constructs across theories.

Results

Constructs from TPB, SCT, CS-SRM, and OLT predicted behaviour; however, the theoretical models did not fit the data well. When predicting behavioural simulation, the proportion of variance explained by individual theories was TPB 11.6%, SCT 12.1%, OLT 8.1%, and II 1.5% of the variance, and in the cross-theory analysis constructs from TPB, CS-SRM and II explained 16.5% of the variance in simulated behaviours. When predicting intention, the proportion of variance explained by individual theories was TPB 25.0%, SCT 21.5%, CS-SRM 11.3%, OLT 26.3%, PAP 2.6%, and knowledge 2.3%, and in the cross-theory analysis constructs from TPB, SCT, CS-SRM, and OLT explained 33.5% variance in intention. Together these results suggest that physicians' beliefs about consequences and beliefs about capabilities are likely determinants of lumbar spine x-ray referrals.

Conclusions

The study provides evidence that taking a theory-based approach enables the creation of a replicable methodology for identifying factors that predict clinical behaviour. However, a number of conceptual and methodological challenges remain.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1748-5908-6-55) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

Martin Eccles is Co-Editor in Chief of Implementation Science; Jeremy Grimshaw is a member of the editorial board of Implementation Science. All editorial decisions on this article were made by Robbie Foy, Deputy Editor.

Authors' contributions

AW, MPE, JG, MJ, and NP conceived the study. MJ, LS, GM, RT, DB, and MPE contributed to the daily running of the study. MJ and NS oversaw the analysis, which was conducted by GM. All authors commented on sequential drafts of the paper and agreed the final draft.

Background

Healthcare systems and professionals fail to deliver the quality of care to which they aspire. Multiple studies internationally have observed evidence to practice gaps demonstrating that 30 to 40 percent of patients do not get treatments of proven effectiveness, and equally discouraging, up to 25 percent of patients receive unnecessary care that is potentially harmful [13]. Such evidence to practice gaps have significant adverse effects on the health and social welfare of citizens and economic productivity.
Lumbar spine imaging for low back pain in primary care settings is an example of an evidence to practice gap. Low back pain is an extremely common presentation in primary care. However, lumbar spine imaging in patients under 50 years is of limited diagnostic benefit within primary care settings [4]. Globally, clinical guidelines for the management of low back pain do not recommend routine imaging of patients with low back pain [48]. Furthermore, standard lumbar spine x-rays (the most common imaging modality used by UK primary care physicians) are associated with significant ionising radiation dosage. Despite this, lumbar spine x-rays are the fourth most common x-ray request from UK primary care physicians [9], with x-ray referrals continuing at the rate of 7 per 1000 patients per year [10]. We conducted a trial that found that for the majority of primary care physician requests, case note review could not identify appropriate indications for referral [10]. The trial also observed a reduction in lumbar spine x-rays of 20 percent without apparent adverse effects following the introduction of educational messages [10].
Recognition of evidence to practice gaps has led to increased interest in more active strategies to disseminate and implement evidence. Over the past two decades, a considerable body of implementation research has been developed [11]. This research demonstrates that dissemination and implementation interventions can be effective, but provides little information to guide the choice or optimise the components of such complex interventions in practice [12, 13]. The effectiveness of interventions appears to vary across different clinical problems, contexts, and organizations. Our understanding of potential barriers and enablers to dissemination and implementation is limited and hindered by a lack of a 'basic science' relating to determinants of professional and organizational behaviour and potential targets for intervention [14]. The challenge for implementation researchers is to develop and evaluate a theoretical base to support the choice and development of interventions as well as the interpretation of implementation study results [15]. Despite recent increased interest in the potential value of behavioural theory to predict healthcare professional behaviour, relatively few studies have assessed this. A recent review by Godin et al. explored the use of social cognitive models to better understand determinants of health care professionals' intentions and behaviours [16]. They identified 72 studies that provided information on the determinants of intention, but only 16 prospective studies that provided information on the determinants of behaviour.
The current study, one part of the PRIME (PRocess modelling in ImpleMEntation research) study) [17], aimed to investigate the use of a number of psychological theories to explore factors associated with primary care physician lumbar spine x-ray referrals. Previous PRIME studies have used similar methods to explore factors associated with primary care physicians' use of antibiotics for sore throats and general dental practitioners' use of routine intra-oral x-rays and preventive fissure sealants [1820]. Variables were drawn from the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB) [21], Social Cognitive Theory (SCT) [22], Operant Learning Theory (OLT) [23] (http://​www.​bfskinner.​org/​BFSkinner/​Home.​html, Implementation Intentions (II) [24], Common Sense Self-Regulation Model (CS-SRM) [25], and Weinstein's Stage Model termed the Precaution Adoption Process (PAP) [26, 27]. These specific theories, which are described in detail elsewhere [28], were chosen because they predict behaviour but vary in their emphasis. Some focus on motivation, proposing that motivation determines behaviour, and therefore the best predictors of behaviour are factors that predict or determine motivation (e.g., TPB). Some place more emphasis on factors that are necessary to predict behaviour in people who are already motivated to change (e.g., II). Others propose that individuals are at different stages in the progress toward behaviour change, and that predictors of behaviour may be different for individuals at different stages (e.g., PAP). The specific models used in this study were chosen for three additional reasons. First, they have been rigorously evaluated with patients or with healthy individuals. Second, they allow us to examine the influence on clinical behaviour of perceived external factors, such as patient preferences and organisational barriers and facilitators. Third, they all explain behaviour in terms of variables that are amenable to change.
The objective of this study was to identify those theories and the theoretical constructs that predicted clinical behaviour, behavioural simulation (as measured by the decisions made in response to five written clinical scenarios) and behavioural intention for lumbar spine x-ray referral.

Methods

The methods of the study are described in detail elsewhere [1720]. Briefly, this was a predictive study of the theory-based cognitions and clinical behaviours of primary care physicians; in this paper, we report data on primary care physicians' lumbar spine x-ray requests. Study participants were a random sample of primary care physicians selected from a list of all such physicians in selected regions of Scotland (Grampian, Tayside, Lothian) and north England (Durham, Newcastle and South Tees) by a statistician using a list of random sampling numbers. Data on theory-based cognitions (predictor measures) and two interim outcome measures (stated behavioural intention and behavioural simulation) were collected by postal questionnaire survey during the 12-month period to which the behavioural data related. Behavioural data were collected from routine data systems in the hospitals that primary care physicians reported as their referral centres for lumbar spine x-rays. Planned analyses explored the predictive value of theories and theory-based cognitions in explaining variance in the behavioural data.

Predictor measures

Theoretically-derived measures were developed following standard operationalisation protocols wherever possible [21, 2933]. The cognition questions were developed from semi-structured interviews with 18 primary care physicians in Scotland and north England that lasted up to 60 minutes. The interviews use standard elicitation methods and covered physicians' views and experiences about managing patients with low back pain. Responses were used to create the questions measuring constructs. Five knowledge questions were developed by the study team based on issues for which there was good evidence. Table 1 provides a summary of the predictor measures used in this study (see also [28]); the instrument is available as Additional File 1. Unless otherwise stated, all questions were rated on a 7-point scale from 'strongly disagree' to 'strongly agree.' We aimed to include at least three questions per psychological construct.
Table 1
Summary of the explanatory measures
Theory of Planned Behaviour (Ajzen, 1991)
   Constructs (number of questions)
Example Question(s)
Behavioural intention (3)
I intend to refer patients with back pain for an X-ray as part of their management
Attitude: Direct (3); Indirecta (8 behavioural beliefs (bb) multiplied by 8 outcome evaluations (oe). The score was the mean of the summed multiplicatives.)
Direct: In general, the possible harm to the patient of a lumbar spine X-ray is outweighed by its benefits; Indirect: In general, referring patients with back pain for an X-ray would reassure them (bb) x reassuring patients with back pain is (oe: un/important)
Subjective Norm: Indirect (4 normative beliefs (nb) multiplied by 4 motivation to comply (mtc) questions. The score was the mean of the summed multiplicatives).
I feel under pressure from the NHS not to refer patients for an X-ray (nb) x How motivated are you to do what the NHS thinks you should (mtc: very much/not at all)
Perceived Behavioural Control: Direct (4); Indirect/power (14)c
Direct: Whether I refer patients for a lumbar X-ray is entirely up to me. Indirect: Without an X-ray, how confident are you in your ability to treat patients with back pain who expect me to refer them for an X-ray
Social Cognitive Theory (Bandura,1998)
Risk Perception (3)
It is highly likely that patients with back pain will be worse off if I do not refer them for an X-ray.
Outcome Expectancies
Self (2x2), Behaviour (8x8). The score was the mean of the summed multiplicatives.
Self: If I refer a patient with back pain for an X-ray, then I will think of myself as a competent GP x Thinking of myself as a competent GP is (Un/Important) Behaviour: See Attitude (Theory of Planned Behaviour)
Self Efficacy: General: Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale (Schwarzer, 1992) (10: 4 point scale, not at all true/exactly true); Specific (7)
General: I can always manage to solve difficult problems if I try hard enough Specific: How confident are you in your ability to treat back problems without using an X-ray report
Implementation Intention (Gollwitzer, 1993)
Action planning (3)
Currently, my standard method of managing patients with back pain does not include referring them for an X-ray
Operant Learning Theory (Skinner, Blackman, 1974)
Anticipated consequences (3)
If I start routinely referring patients with back pain then, on balance, my life as a GP will be easier in the long run
Evidence of habit (2)
When I see a patient with back pain, I automatically consider referring them for an X-ray
Experienced (rewarding and punishing) consequences (4: more likely to refer (score = 1); less likely (score=-1); unchanged/not sure/never occurred (score = 0)). Scores were summed.
Think about the last time you referred a patient for a lumbar spine X-ray and felt pleased that you had done so. Do you think the result of this episode has made you: Think about the last time you decided not to refer a patient for a lumbar spine X-ray and felt sorry that you had not done so. Do you think the result of this episode has made you:
Common Sense Self-regulation Model d (Leventhal et al., 1984)
Perceived identity (3)
Back pain as seen in general practice is generally of an intense nature
Perceived cause (8)
Back pain is caused by stress or worry
Perceived controllability (7)
What the patient does can determine whether back pain gets better or worse, What I do can determine whether the patient's back pain gets better or worse
Perceived duration (5)
Back pain as seen in general practice is very unpredictable
Perceived consequences (3)
Back pain does not have much effect on a patient's life
Coherence (2)
I have a clear picture or understanding of back pain
Emotional response (4)
Seeing patients with back pain does not worry me
Precaution Adoption Process (Stage model)(Weinstein, 1988; Weinstein, Rothman & Sutton, 1998)
Current stage of change. A single statement is ticked to indicate the behavioural stage
Unmotivated (3): I have not yet thought about changing the number of lumbar X-rays I currently request. It has been a while since I have thought about changing the number of lumbar X-rays I request. Motivated (2): I have thought about it and decided that I will not change the number of lumbar X-rays I request. I have decided that I will request more lumbar X-rays. I have decided that I will request less lumbar X-rays. Action (1): I have already done something about increasing the number of lumbar X-rays I request I have already done something about decreasing the number of lumbar X-rays I request
Other Measures
Knowledge (5) (True/False/Not Sure)
The presence of spondolytic changes on a lumbar spine X-ray correlates well with back pain
Demographic
Post code, gender, time qualified, number of other doctors in practice, trainer status, hours per week, list size
a All indirect measures consist of specific belief questions identified in the preliminary study as salient to the management of low back pain.
b These individuals and groups were identified in the preliminary study as influential in the management of low back pain
c An indirect measure of perceived behavioural control usually would be the sum of a set of multiplicatives (control beliefs x power of each belief to inhibit/enhance behaviour). However, the preliminary study demonstrated that it proved problematic to ask clinicians meaningful questions which used the word 'control' as clinicians tended to describe themselves as having complete control over the final decision to perform the behaviour. Support for measuring perceived behavioural control using only questions as to the ease or difficulty of performing the outcome behaviour was derived from a metanalysis which suggested that perceived ease/difficulty questions were sensitive predictors of behavioural intention and behaviour (Trafimow et al., 2002).
d Illness representation measures were derived from the Revised Illness Perception Questionnaire (Moss-Morris, R., Weinman, J., Petrie, K. J., Horne, R., Cameron, L.D., & Buick, D. 2002)

Outcome measures

Behaviour

The number of lumbar spine x-ray imaging requests made by each primary care physician over 12 months were obtained from the hospitals that the responding primary care physicians identified as their radiology referral centres. At the time of the study, primary care physicians in the United Kingdom did not have open access to other modalities of lumbar imaging (CT and MRI scans). We standardised our behaviour by the number of patients registered with the primary care doctor to reflect differences in workloads of the participating primary care doctors.

Behavioural simulation

Our measure used vignettes to simulate clinical decision-making in specific situations; such measures have been shown to be predictive of behaviour, though less so than general measures of intention [34]. Key elements which may influence primary care physicians' decisions to refer for a lumbar spine x-ray on patients with low back pain were identified from the literature, opinion of the clinical members of the research team, and the interviews with primary care physicians. From this, five clinical scenarios were constructed describing patients presenting in primary care with low back pain. Respondents were asked to decide whether or not they would request a lumbar spine x-ray for each scenario, and decisions to request an x-ray were summed to create a total score out of a possible maximum of five.

Behavioural intention

Three questions assessed primary care physicians' intention to refer patients presenting with low back pain for lumbar spine x-ray:
'When a patient presents with back pain, I have in mind to refer them for X-ray, I intend to refer patients with back pain for an X-ray as part of their management, I aim to refer patients with back pain for an X-ray as part of patient management (rated on a 7-point scale from 'Strongly Disagree' to 'Strongly Agree').'
Responses were summed (range 3 to 21) and scaled so that a low score equated with a low intention to refer for lumbar spine x-ray.

Procedure

Participants were mailed an invitation pack (letter of invitation, questionnaire consisting of psychological and demographic measures, a form requesting consent to allow the research team to access the respondent's referral data, a study newsletter, and a reply paid envelope) by research staff. Initially, 700 primary care physicians were surveyed between July and mid-August 2003. Due to a low initial response rate, a further sample of 400 primary care physicians were surveyed between October and December 2003. Two postal reminders were sent to non-responders at two and four weeks. Behavioural data were collected over a one-year period, from approximately six months before to six months after the assessment of cognitions.

Sample size and statistical analysis

The target sample size of 200 was based on a recommendation by Green [35] to have a minimum of 162 subjects when undertaking multiple regression analysis with 14 predictor variables.
The internal consistency of the measures was tested using Cronbach's alpha. If this was less than 0.6, then questionnaire items were removed from each measure to achieve the highest Cronbach's alpha possible. For constructs with only two questions, a correlation coefficient of 0.25 was used as a cut off.
For each of the three outcome variables, we examined the relationship between predictor and outcome variables within the structure of each of the theories individually. Spearman's correlation (for behaviour outcome) and Pearson Correlation Coefficients (for behavioural simulation and intention outcomes) between the individual constructs and the outcome measures were calculated. Given the distribution of the behavioural data, we used negative binomial regression (NBR) to model the predictive ability of individual theoretical constructs and complete theories. NBR is used to model count exhibiting over dispersion, as in the case of the behaviour outcome data in this study. We reported incidence rate ratios (IRR) from the NRB models. IRRs estimate the change in the rate of the dependent variable associated with changes in the independent variables. NBR does not generate a direct equivalent of an R2 statistic to estimate the proportion of variance in the dependent variable explained by models. However, it is possible to compute a number of different R2 statistics to explore the goodness of fit of the model [36]. The pseudo-R2 we chose to use was McFaddens' adjusted R2 because it penalizes models in the spirit of adjusted R2 in linear regression for adding more variables to a model (see Additional File 2 for further discussion). Linear regression was used for intention and behavioural simulation. For the five 'perceived cause of illness' questions in the CS-SRM, responses were dichotomized into scores of five to seven (indicating agreement that the cause in question was responsible for low back pain) versus anything else (indicating disagreement). These dichotomous variables were then entered as independent variables into the regression models. The relationship between II and intention was not explored as it is a post-intentional theory. For the analysis of the PAP, respondents were dichotomized into two groups (decided to reduce or have already reduced x-rays versus other responses) and the relationship between predictive and outcome variables were examined using regression models. Finally, for predictors p < 0.25 irrespective of whether or not they came from the same theory, we conducted a cross-theoretical construct analyses that examined the relationship between predictive and outcome variables.

Ethics approval

The study was approved by the UK South East Multi-Centre Research Ethics Committee (MREC/03/01/03).

Results

Of the 1,100 primary care physicians approached, 299 (27%) agreed to participate. Most respondents provided usable data on intention (296) and behavioural simulation (297), and we were able to obtain imaging request data from 287 (Figure 1). Numbers included in analyses vary between the outcome measures because complete case analysis was used. For the negative binomial regression analyses, we had complete data from 240 respondents.
Fifty eight percent of the respondents were male. Respondents had been qualified for a mean (SD) of 21 (8) years. They had a median inter-quartile range (IQR) list size of 1,450 registered patients, a median IQR of 4.8 (3.6 to 6.8) partners, and worked a median IQR of 8 (6 to 9) half day sessions a week; 45 (15%) were trainers. Descriptive statistics for the independent variables are provided in Table 2.
Table 2
Descriptive statistics
Theory
Predictive Constructs
N
Alpha
Mean
(SD)
Respondents agreeing with item (%)
Theory of
Attitude direct
2
0.25
4.6
(1.2)
 
Planned
Attitude indirect
4
0.75
18.6
(6.9)
 
Behaviour
Subjective Norm
4
0.68
15.0
(4.8)
 
 
Intention
3
0.69
2.1
(1.0)
 
 
PBC direct
4
0.63
4.5
(1.1)
 
 
PBC power
14
0.91
3.1
(1.0)
 
Social Cognitive Theory
Risk perception
2
0.46
2.2
(1.0)
 
 
Outcome expectancies
6
0.76
13.9
(8.3)
 
 
Self efficacy
14
0.93
3.2
(0.8)
 
 
Generalised self efficacy
10
0.87
2.8
(0.4)
 
Implementation Intention
Action Planning
-
-
2.4
(1.6)
 
Operant Learning Theory
Anticipated consequences
2
0.46
2.2
(1.0)
 
 
Evidence of habitual behaviour
2
0.60
3.3
(1.7)
 
Common Sense
Identity of condition
3
0.49
4.2
(0.8)
 
Self-regulation
Timeline acute
2
0.19
3.4
(0.8)
 
Model
Timeline cyclical
3
0.54
4.4
(0.9)
 
 
Control - by treatment
3
0.66
5.6
(0.8)
 
 
Control - by patient
2
0.85
5.7
(1.0)
 
 
Control - by doctor
2
0.36
5.3
(0.9)
 
 
Cause - stress
1
   
126 (42)
 
Cause - family problems
1
   
117 (39)
 
Cause - poor prior medical care
1
   
66 (22)
 
Cause - patient's own behaviour
1
   
225 (85)
 
Cause - ageing
1
   
217 (73)
 
Cause - bad luck
1
   
140 (47)
 
Cause - overwork
1
   
148 (49)
 
Consequence
2
0.21
4.8
(0.8)
 
 
Emotional Response
4
0.69
5.1
(1.0)
 
 
Coherence
2
0.74
2.7
(1.0)
 
Precaution Adoption Process
     
157 (53)†
Other
Knowledge
5
0.21
3.1
(1.0)
 
*p≤0.05; ** p≤0.01; ***p≤0.001.
Alpha = Cronbach's
Number of respondents who replied 'I have decided that I will request less lumbar X-rays' or 'I have already done something about decreasing the number of lumbar X-rays I request.'

Relationship between the three outcome measures

The three outcome measures were significantly (though weakly) correlated with each other: for behaviour and behavioural simulation, the Spearman's rho statistic was 0.169 (p = 0.004); similarly for behaviour and behavioural intention it was 0.165 (p = 0.005); and for behavioural simulation and behavioural intention the Pearson's r was 0.313 (p < 0.001).

Predicting behaviour

The mean number of lumbar spine x-rays was 5.0 per 1,000 patients registered per year. The results of analyses are shown in Table 3. Individual construct analyses suggested that constructs from TPB (attitudes, intention, and perceived behavioural control), SCT (risk perception, self efficacy), OLT (anticipated consequences) and CS-SRM (cause - aging) significantly predicted the lumbar spine referrals. To aid interpretation of the results, we provide the following example; intention had a mean score of 2.1 (SD 1.0), the IRR was 1.29 -- this suggests that for every point increase in intention (equivalent in this example to one SD), lumbar spine referrals would increase by 29.0%. Theory-level analyses (Table 3) suggested that TPB (perceived behavioural control), SCT (risk perception), OLT (anticipated consequences), CS-SRM (control - by patient, cause - poor prior medical care, cause - patients' own behaviours, cause - aging) predicted behaviour. II, PAP, and knowledge did not predict behaviour. However, the goodness to fit measures suggested that the theoretical models did not predict behaviour data in this dataset (McFadden's pseudo R2 range from 0 to 0.004, see also Additional file 2 for addition goodness to fit measures). In the cross-theoretical construct analysis, constructs from TPB (attitudes) and CS-SRM (coherence, cause - poor prior medical care, control - by patient) were retained in the regression model; again the goodness of fit models performed poorly (Table 4).
Table 3
Predicting behaviour by psychological theory: negative binomial regression analyses
Theory
Predictive Constructs
IRR Individual and p-value
IRR model
Theory of Planned
Intention
1.285
0.008
1.097
 
Behaviour
PBC direct
1.023
0.823
1.175
 
 
PBC power
1.427
< 0.001
1.444**
R2 = 0.004
Social Cognitive Theory
Risk perception
1.444
< 0.001
1.392**
 
 
Outcome expectancies
1.019
0.080
1.001
 
 
Self efficacy
1.363
0.019
1.110
 
 
Generalised self efficacy
0.855
0.564
0.823
R2 = 0.002
Implementation Intention
 
1. 111
0.138
1.111
R2 = 0.000
Operant Learning Theory
Anticipated consequences
1.449
< 0.001
1.413**
 
 
Evidence of habitual behaviour
1.089
0.179
1.017
R2 = 0.004
Common Sense
Identity of condition
0.864
0.278
0.867
 
Self-regulation
Timeline acute
1.08
0.957
1.026
 
Model
Timeline cyclical
1.187
0.196
1.273
 
 
Control - by treatment
1.105
0.970
1.170
 
 
Control - by patient
0.869
0.142
0.725*
 
 
Control - by doctor
0.936
0.524
1.064
 
 
Cause - stress
1.191
0.370
0.519
 
 
Cause - family problems
1.345
0.130
2.526
 
 
Cause - poor prior medical care
1.403
0.134
1.70*
 
 
Cause - patient's own behaviour
0.897
0.581
0.592*
 
 
Cause - ageing
1.609
0.028
1.671*
 
 
Cause - bad luck
0.712
0.080
0.759
 
 
Cause - overwork
0.878
0.502
0.969
 
 
Consequence
1.006
0.902
1.060
 
 
Emotional Response
0.962
0.699
1.005
 
 
Coherence
1.231
0.046
1.171
R2 = 0.000
Precaution Adoption Process
 
0.871
0.599
0.871
R2 = 0.000
Knowledge
 
0.859
0.104
0.859
R2 = 0.000
*p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01; ***p ≤ 0.001.
Alpha = Cronbach's; IRR Individual = incidence rate ratio from a regression model with the single construct independent variable IRR Model = incidence rate ratio from the theoretical model with all constructs included as independent variables. R2 is MacFadden's adjusted R2.
Table 4
Results of the stepwise regression cross-theoretical construct analyses
Predictive Constructs
Entered
    
Outcome: Ordering lumbar spine x-rays
 
IRR
Adj. R 2
  
TPB: Attitude Indirect and Direct; PBC Power; Intention
SCT: Risk Perception; Self Efficacy
Operant learning theory: anticipated consequences; Evidence of habitual behaviour
Implementation Intention
CS-SRM Timeline cyclical; Control - by patient; Cause - family problems, poor prior medical care, ageing, bad luck; Coherence
Knowledge
Coherence
1.122*
   
 
Control - by patient
0.897*
   
 
Attitude Direct
1.017***
   
 
Cause - poor prior medical care
1.848**
0.015†
  
Outcome: Behavioural Simulation
 
Beta
Adj. R 2
df
F
TPB: Attitude Indirect and Direct; PBC Power and PBC Power direct; Intention
SCT: Risk Perception; Outcome expectancy Self Efficacy
Operant learning theory: Anticipated Consequences; Evidence of Habitual Behaviour
Implementation Intention
CS-SRM: Control - by treatment, patient, doctor; Cause - ageing; Coherence; Emotional Response
Precaution Adoption Process
Action Planning
0.272***
   
 
PBC Power
0.252***
   
 
Cause - ageing
0.126*
0.165
3, 277
19.4***
Outcome: Behavioural Intention
 
Beta
Adj. R 2
df
F
TPB: Attitude Indirect and Direct; Subjective Norm; PBC Power and PBC Power direct
SCT: Risk Perception; Outcome expectancy Self Efficacy
Operant learning theory: anticipated consequences; Evidence of Habitual Behaviour
CS-SRM: Control - by treatment, patient and doctor; Cause- stress; Coherence; Emotional Response
Precaution Adoption Process
Knowledge
PBC Power
0.273***
   
 
Evidence of Habitual Behaviour
0.286***
   
 
Outcome expectancy
0.169**
   
 
Control - by treatment
-0.115*
0.335
4, 275
36.1***
*p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01; ***p ≤ 0.001.
PBC = perceived behavioural control; TPB = Theory of Planned Behaviour; SCT = Social Cognitive Theory; CS-SRM = Common Sense Self-Regulation Model;
† McFadden's pseudo R2

Predicting behavioural simulation

In response to the five clinical scenarios, the respondents indicated that they would refer for lumbar spine x-ray in a mean (SD) of 1.5 (1.2) cases. The median number of referrals was 1 with a range of 0 to 3. From Table 5, the individual constructs that predicted behavioural simulation (i.e., what primary care physicians said they would do in response to the specific clinical scenarios) were: TPB (attitudes, social norms, perceived behavioural control, and intention), SCT (risk perception, outcome expectancies, and self efficacy); II; OLT (anticipated consequences, evidence of habitual behaviour); CS-SRM (control - by treatment, control - by patient, control - by doctor, cause - ageing, emotional response treatment). Neither knowledge nor PAP predicted behavioural simulation.
Table 5
Predicting behavioural simulation and intention by psychological theory: correlation and multiple regression analyses
  
Behavioural simulation
Behavioural intention
Theory
Predictive Constructs
r
Beta
R2(adj)
df
F
r
Beta
R2(adj)
df
F
Theory of Planned
Intention
0.313***
0.182**
        
Behaviour
PBC direct
-0.143*
0.018
        
 
PBC power
0.315***
0.236**
.116
3, 282
13.4***
     
 
Attitude direct
     
-0.180**
-0.088
   
 
Attitude indirect
     
0.361***
0.013
   
 
Subjective Norm
     
0.149**
-0.003
   
 
PBC direct
     
-0.320***
-0.068
   
 
PBC power
     
0.487***
0.090***
.250
5, 282
20.1***
Social Cognitive
Risk perception
0.286***
0.204**
   
0.392***
0.226***
   
Theory
Outcome expectancies
0.139*
-0.023
   
0.350***
0.210**
   
 
Self efficacy
0.301***
0.245***
   
0.336***
0.197**
   
 
Generalised self efficacy
-0.036
-0.001
.121
4, 272
10.5***
-0.035
0.022
.215
4, 271
19.8***
Implementation intention
 
.135*
.135*
.015
1, 275
5.1*
     
Operant Learning Theory
Anticipated consequences
0.286***
0.253***
   
0.392***
0.238***
   
 
Evidence of habitual behaviour
0.184**
0.080
.081
2, 287
13.7***
0.470***
0.371***
.263
2, 286
52.3***
Common sense
Identity of condition
-0.043
-0.029
   
0.043
0.081
   
Self regulation model
Timeline acute
0.079
-0.029
   
0.097
0.000
   
 
Timeline cyclical
0.010
0.006
   
-0.020
-0.050
   
 
Control - by treatment
-0.187*
-0.115
   
-0.217**
-0.160**
   
 
Control - by patient
-0.121*
-0.004
   
-0.282**
-0.089
   
 
Control - by doctor
-0.140*
-0.024
   
-0.315**
-0.107
   
 
Cause - stress
-0.104
-0.051
   
-0.119*
-0.190
   
 
Cause - family problems
-0.096
-0.097
   
-0.080
0.084
   
 
Cause - poor prior medical care
0.039
0.100
   
-0.033
0.011
   
 
Cause - patient's own behaviour
0.040
0.074
   
-0.048
0.017
   
 
Cause - ageing
0.145***
0.145*
   
0.073
0.062
   
 
Cause - bad luck
0.053
0.071
   
-0.010
-0.044
   
 
Cause - overwork
-0.032
-0.080
   
0.046
0.052
   
 
Consequence
-0.080
-0.063
   
-0.061
-0.015
   
 
Emotional Response
-0.184***
-0.117
   
0.187**
-0.001
   
 
Coherence
0.089
-0.060
.036
16,268
1.7
-0.249**
-0.142**
.113
16,265
3.2***
Precaution Adoption Process
 
-0.09
-0.09
.005
1, 296
2.5
-0.17**
-0.17**
0.026
1, 294
8.3**
Knowledge
 
-.091
-.091
.005
1, 292
0.1
-.163**
-.148**
.023
1, 292
8.0**
*p = or <0.05; ** p = or <0.01; ***p = or <0.001.
r = Pearson product moment correlation coefficient; Beta = standardised regression coefficients.
The results of the theory-level analyses are shown in Table 5. The TPB explained 11.6% of the variance in behavioural simulation, SCT explained 12.1%, II explained 1.5%, and OLT explained 8.1%. In the cross-theoretical construct analysis, constructs from TPB (perceived behavioural control), II and CS-SRM (cause - ageing) were retained in the regression model, together explaining 16.5% of the variance in the scenario score (Table 4).

Predicting behavioural intention

With the range of possible scores for intention of 1 to 7, the mean (SD) intention score was 2.1 (1.0); the median intention score was 1.6 with a range of 1 to 5.5. The constructs that predicted behavioural intention were: TPB (attitudes, subjective norms, perceived behavioural control); SCT (risk perception, outcome expectancy, self efficacy); OLT (anticipated consequences, evidence of habitual behaviour); CS-SRM (control - treatment, control - patient, control - doctor, cause - stress, emotional response, and coherence); knowledge; and PAP (Table 5).
The results of the theory level analyses are shown in Table 5. The TPB explained 25% of the variance in behavioural intention, SCT 21.5%, OLT 26.3%, CS-SRM 11.3%, knowledge 2.3%, and PAP explained 2.6%. In the cross-theoretical construct analysis, constructs from TPB (perceived behavioural control), OLT (evidence of habitual behaviour, outcome expectancy), CS-SRM (control - treatment) were retained in the regression model, together explaining 33.5% of the variance in intention (Table 4).

Discussion

We have successfully developed and applied psychological theory-based questionnaires that have, in the context of ordering of lumbar spine x-rays in the management of patients with low back pain been able to predict two proxies for behaviour (behavioural simulation and intention) and (to a lesser extent) behaviour.

Overall interpretation

Low back pain is a frequent presenting problem in primary care settings. However, the use of x-rays in clinical management of low back pain is relatively infrequent. In the theory level analysis predicting clinical behaviour, constructs relating to beliefs about consequences (SCT (risk perception) and CS-SRM (cause - poor prior medical treatment, cause - patient's own behaviour and cause-ageing, control - patient) and beliefs about capabilities (TPB (perceived behavioural control)) all significantly predicted behaviour. Looking across our two other outcome measures, there are also suggestions that beliefs about consequences (attitudes, outcome expectancies, risk perception, anticipated consequences) and beliefs about capabilities (PBC, self efficacy) may be important. In addition, II predicted behavioural simulation and OLT (evidence of habitual behaviour) predicted intention. The theories individually explained a significant proportion of the variance in behavioural simulation and intention, but overall were poorly predictive of behaviour. Together, these findings suggest both beliefs about consequences and beliefs about capabilities are likely determinants of lumbar spine x-ray requests.
This is a correlational study, so the causative aspects of the theories and their constructs remain untested in this population; but it is promising for the utility of applying psychological theory to changing clinical behaviour that the constructs are acting as the theories expect. These results suggest that an intervention that specifically targets predictive elements should have the greatest likelihood of success in influencing the implementation of this evidence-based practice.
The PRIME study has evaluated the predictive value of a range of theories across different behaviours (prescribing antibiotics for upper respiratory tract infections, or URTIs, taking dental radiographs, placing preventive fissure sealants), target professional groups (primary care doctors, dentists), and contexts [17, 19, 20, 37]; we have demonstrated that different constructs predicted different proportions of the variance in the intention and behaviour. This raises the question of how best to identify relevant theories specific to different behaviours and clinical groups. One option would be to undertake preliminary work to identify the key construct domains that are likely to influence the target behaviours, and use them to specify potentially relevant theories [38, 39].

Strengths and weaknesses

Operationalising our behaviour of interest in the surveys that reflected the available behavioural data was challenging. Our behaviour of interest was managing patients with low back pain without referral for lumbar spine x-ray. However, we could only get behavioural data on the number of lumbar spine x-ray referrals ordered by primary care physicians. In general, we tried to word the survey questions to correspond to the available behavioural data (e.g., 'when a patient presents with back pain, I have in mind to refer them for X-ray'). However, we found it difficult to frame some questions that corresponded to the behavioural data and clinically sensible. As a result the final questionnaire, included some questions worded in terms of doing the behaviour (e.g., in general, referring patients with back pain for an X-ray would...) and some worded in terms of not doing the behaviour (e.g., without an x-ray, how confident are you in your ability to...). This raises the issue of whether doing and not doing a behaviour are two sides of the same behaviour, or whether they represent linked but alternate behaviours. If the latter, the predictive ability of our survey instrument would be likely to be reduced.
Operationalising the constructs with theoretical fidelity was also challenging. A number of the models (OLT, II, CS-SRM) had not been operationalised in this way prior to the PRIME studies. OLT and II are usually used as intervention methods to change behaviour. However, both predicted behavioural simulation, and OLT predicted intention and behaviour. Since we undertook this study, some of the models have been adapted or enhanced, and different approaches to measurement have been developed -- for example, the post intentional action-coping planning enhancements of the TPB [40, 41] and Verplanken's Self Reported Habit Index [42].
The CS-SRM pattern of results mirrored the overall picture of beliefs about consequences and capabilities being important. However, they did not predict behaviour, behavioural simulation, and intention particularly well. The model has previously been used mainly to refer to an individual's perceptions of their clinical condition; we used it to measure a clinician's perception of the condition in general. We had difficulty operationalising this model, and further work is needed to explore the utility of this theory to predict clinician behaviour.
There is a stepwise decrease in the proportion of variance across our dependent variables from intention to behavioural simulation (to behaviour) (Tables 2 and 4) as found in previous PRIME studies. Godin's review [16] of the predictive value of social cognitive models on professional behaviour showed a similar pattern, with social cognitive models explaining means of 13% of the variance in objectively measured behaviour (from 11 studies), 44% of self-reported behaviour (from four studies), and 59% of intention (from 72 studies). Our results are each lower than Godin's average figures, but all are within the range reported by other studies. However, our explanation of behaviour is at the very lowest limit of the reported range. In the previous PRIME studies, we have been able to explain 16% of the variance in general dental practitioners' use of dental radiographs [37] and 6% of primary care physicians prescribing of antibiotics for patients who present with an URTI [20]. This suggests that our operationalisation of the models was likely to have been good, and raises the question of why the models did not work as well for ordering lumbar spine x-rays by primary care physicians.
We can identify three potential explanations. Firstly, there was poor correspondence between the behaviour specified in the survey and the measured behaviour as mentioned above. This highlights the importance of clear and consistent framing of the questions and concordance with the measured behaviour. In the previous PRIME papers, the behaviours specified in the surveys and the measured behaviours were: dental radiographs (survey - use of intra-oral radiographs in patient management, data - the number of intra oral radiographs taken per course of treatment (good concordance)) [18], and antibiotics (survey - prescribing an antibiotic for patients presenting with URTIs, managing patients without an antibiotic, data - number of likely URTI relevant antibiotic prescriptions per 100 patients registered (weak concordance)) [20].
Second, there was potentially excess observational error (noise) in our behaviour measure. X-ray-ordering data was chosen because it was available from routine data sources, and was therefore inexpensive to collect. Low back pain was chosen because it was more likely that a request for an investigation would be attributed to the primary care doctor who issued it. Despite this, anecdotally we believe that there may be errors in the attribution of x-rays to doctors, with radiology departments reporting that requests could be reported to the correct practice but attributed to the wrong primary care doctor. In addition, we attempted to standardise our behaviour by the number of patients registered with the primary care doctor to reflect differences in workloads of the participating primary care doctors. We only had data on the total number of patients and number of primary care doctors in each practice, and so calculated an average list size per primary care doctor within each practice. This is a relatively crude standardisation approach that does not take account of likely variations of workload within practices (not all primary care doctors in the same practice will have the same workload) and variations in presentation of the target condition (not all primary care doctors will have same rate of presentation of low back pain). In the previous PRIME studies, these issues were likely to have been more problematic in the antibiotic study rather than the fissure sealant study (where data were abstracted from a claims database). These issues reflect some of the challenges of using routine data to measure behaviour relating to the level of clinical detail available (we could not estimate the number of patients each primary care doctor saw presenting with back pain) and problems of attribution of clinical actions to specific primary care doctors. In future studies of this kind, it will be important to invest more in the measurement of the behavioural data. These issues are likely to be less problematic in population-based large administrative database facilities where there may be detailed understanding of the content of the available data and their limitations. Alternatively it could be possible to collect behavioural data directly.
Thirdly, we used a different analytical approach to analyse the behavioural data. Previous PRIME studies have used multiple regression analyses and used the adjusted R2 statistic from ordinary least squares (OLS) regression to quantify the proportion of variance explained by the models. In the current study, when we conducted multiple regression analyses of behavioural simulation and intention, we observed similar magnitude R2 statistics for behavioural simulation and intention models. However given the distribution of the lumbar spine x-ray data, we had to use negative binomial regression for the behavioural analysis. A direct equivalent to the adjusted R2 statistic does not exist for negative binomial regression. There are several pseudo R2 statistics that mimic OLS R2 in the sense that they can range over the scale 0 to 1 with higher values indicating a better fit of models to data. We present the results for various goodness to fit models that suggest that, in general, the resulting models overall were poorly predictive of the behavioural data. However these pseudo R2 values cannot be used to compare the performance of competing theoretical models across different data sets, making comparisons of proportion variation explained with previous PRIME study surveys qualitative only. To explore the likely comparability of these results with previous PRIME studies, we undertook an OLS regression of square root transformed behavioural data and observed an R2 statistic of 0.05, which is at the lower end of the observed R2 statistics from previous PRIME studies. Together, we believe these data suggest that the models may be performing similarly to those in previous PRIME studies and the analytical approach required due to the negative binomial distribution is obscuring this.
Our final response rate was not high compared to what would be expected for a postal questionnaire survey to healthcare professionals. Following the report by Cummings et al. that up to 1995, response rates of surveys of healthcare professionals remained constant at approximately 60% [43], Cook et al. demonstrated that by 2005 response rates in surveys of healthcare professionals had slightly declined to an average of 57.5% [44]. Given this, we cannot exclude the possibility of selection bias in respondents and should be cautious about generalising from our respondents to the population of UK primary care physicians. However, this may be less of an issue at this exploratory stage of using these methods, as the purpose of the study was theory testing and an exploration of the predictive ability of theories to explain variations in behaviour. Our aim was not to generate data that was representative, but to receive our pre-specified number of responses from a population who had a range of behaviour, reported a range of behavioural simulation and intention, and who reported a range of cognitions. The study achieved this aim.

Conclusions

This study provides evidence that psychological models may be useful in understanding and predicting clinical behaviour. Taking a theory-based approach enables the creation of a replicable methodology for identifying factors that predict clinical behaviour. However, there remain conceptual challenges in operationalising a number of the models and a range of methodological challenges in terms of instrument development and measurement of behaviour that have to be surmounted before these methods could be regarded as routine.

Acknowledgements

The development of this study was supported by the UK Medical Research Council Health Services Research Collaboration. It was funded by a grant from the UK Medical Research Council (G0001325). The Health Services Research Unit is funded by the Chief Scientist Office of the Scottish Executive. Ruth Thomas was funded by the Wellcome Trust (GR063790MA). Jeremy Grimshaw holds a Canada Research Chair in Health Knowledge Transfer and Uptake. The views expressed in this paper are those of the authors and may not be shared by the funding bodies. We would also like to thank Jill Francis and participating primary care physicians for their contribution to this study.
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Competing interests

Martin Eccles is Co-Editor in Chief of Implementation Science; Jeremy Grimshaw is a member of the editorial board of Implementation Science. All editorial decisions on this article were made by Robbie Foy, Deputy Editor.

Authors' contributions

AW, MPE, JG, MJ, and NP conceived the study. MJ, LS, GM, RT, DB, and MPE contributed to the daily running of the study. MJ and NS oversaw the analysis, which was conducted by GM. All authors commented on sequential drafts of the paper and agreed the final draft.
Anhänge

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Seddon ME, Marshall MN, Campbell SM, Roland MO: Systematic review of studies of quality of clinical care in general practice in the UK, Australia and New Zealand. Qual Health Care. 2001, 10: 152-158. 10.1136/qhc.0100152.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Seddon ME, Marshall MN, Campbell SM, Roland MO: Systematic review of studies of quality of clinical care in general practice in the UK, Australia and New Zealand. Qual Health Care. 2001, 10: 152-158. 10.1136/qhc.0100152.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Grol R: Successes and failures in the implementation of evidence-based guidelines for clinical practice. Med Care. 2001, 39: II46-II54.CrossRefPubMed Grol R: Successes and failures in the implementation of evidence-based guidelines for clinical practice. Med Care. 2001, 39: II46-II54.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat McGlynn EA, Asch SM, Adams J, Keesey J, Hicks J, DeCristofaro A: The quality of health care delivered to adults in the United States. N Engl J Med. 2003, 348: 2635-2645. 10.1056/NEJMsa022615.CrossRefPubMed McGlynn EA, Asch SM, Adams J, Keesey J, Hicks J, DeCristofaro A: The quality of health care delivered to adults in the United States. N Engl J Med. 2003, 348: 2635-2645. 10.1056/NEJMsa022615.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Jarvik JG, Deyo RA: Diagnostic evaluation of low back pain with emphasis on imaging. Ann Intern Med. 2002, 137: 586-597.CrossRefPubMed Jarvik JG, Deyo RA: Diagnostic evaluation of low back pain with emphasis on imaging. Ann Intern Med. 2002, 137: 586-597.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Royal College of Radiologists: Making the best use of a Department of Radiology. 1998, London: Royal College of Radiologists Royal College of Radiologists: Making the best use of a Department of Radiology. 1998, London: Royal College of Radiologists
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Australian Acute Musculoskeletal Pain Guidelines Group (AAMPGG): Evidence-based management of acute musculoskeleta pain. 2003, Brisbane: Australian Academic Press Australian Acute Musculoskeletal Pain Guidelines Group (AAMPGG): Evidence-based management of acute musculoskeleta pain. 2003, Brisbane: Australian Academic Press
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Savigny P, Kuntze S, Watson P, Underwood M, Ritchie G, Cotterell M: Low back pain: early management of persistent non-specific low back pain. 2009, London: National Collaborating Centre for Primary Care and Royal College of General Practitioners Savigny P, Kuntze S, Watson P, Underwood M, Ritchie G, Cotterell M: Low back pain: early management of persistent non-specific low back pain. 2009, London: National Collaborating Centre for Primary Care and Royal College of General Practitioners
8.
Zurück zum Zitat National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence: NICE clinical guideline 88 Low back pain early management of persistent non-specific low back oain. 2009, London: National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence: NICE clinical guideline 88 Low back pain early management of persistent non-specific low back oain. 2009, London: National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Matowe L, Ramsay CR, Grimshaw JM, Gilbert FJ, Macleod MJ, Needham G: Effects of mailed dissemination of the Royal College of Radiologists' guidelines on general practitioner referrals for radiography: a time series analysis. Clin Radiol. 2002, 57: 575-578. 10.1053/crad.2001.0894.CrossRefPubMed Matowe L, Ramsay CR, Grimshaw JM, Gilbert FJ, Macleod MJ, Needham G: Effects of mailed dissemination of the Royal College of Radiologists' guidelines on general practitioner referrals for radiography: a time series analysis. Clin Radiol. 2002, 57: 575-578. 10.1053/crad.2001.0894.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Eccles M, Steen N, Grimshaw J, Thomas L, McNamee P, Soutter J: Effect of audit and feedback, and reminder messages on primary-care radiology referrals: a randomised trial. Lancet. 2001, 357: 1406-1409. 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)04564-5.CrossRefPubMed Eccles M, Steen N, Grimshaw J, Thomas L, McNamee P, Soutter J: Effect of audit and feedback, and reminder messages on primary-care radiology referrals: a randomised trial. Lancet. 2001, 357: 1406-1409. 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)04564-5.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Grimshaw JM, Shirran L, Thomas R, Mowatt G, Fraser C, Bero L: Changing provider behavior: an overview of systematic reviews of interventions. Med Care. 2001, 39: II2-45.CrossRefPubMed Grimshaw JM, Shirran L, Thomas R, Mowatt G, Fraser C, Bero L: Changing provider behavior: an overview of systematic reviews of interventions. Med Care. 2001, 39: II2-45.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Grimshaw JM, Thomas RE, Maclennan G, Fraser C, Ramsay CR, Vale L: Effectiveness and efficiency of guideline dissemination and implementation strategies. Health Technol Assess. 2004, 8: iii-72.CrossRefPubMed Grimshaw JM, Thomas RE, Maclennan G, Fraser C, Ramsay CR, Vale L: Effectiveness and efficiency of guideline dissemination and implementation strategies. Health Technol Assess. 2004, 8: iii-72.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Foy R, Eccles MP, Jamtvedt G, Young J, Grimshaw JM, Baker R: What do we know about how to do audit and feedback? Pitfalls in applying evidence from a systematic review. BMC Health Serv Res. 2005, 5: 50-10.1186/1472-6963-5-50.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Foy R, Eccles MP, Jamtvedt G, Young J, Grimshaw JM, Baker R: What do we know about how to do audit and feedback? Pitfalls in applying evidence from a systematic review. BMC Health Serv Res. 2005, 5: 50-10.1186/1472-6963-5-50.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Eccles M, Grimshaw J, Walker A, Johnston M, Pitts N: Changing the behavior of healthcare professionals: the use of theory in promoting the uptake of research findings. J Clin Epidemiol. 2005, 58: 107-112. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2004.09.002.CrossRefPubMed Eccles M, Grimshaw J, Walker A, Johnston M, Pitts N: Changing the behavior of healthcare professionals: the use of theory in promoting the uptake of research findings. J Clin Epidemiol. 2005, 58: 107-112. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2004.09.002.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat The Improved Clinical Effectiveness through Behavioural Research Group (ICEBeRG): Designing theoretically-informed implementation interventions. Implement Sci. 2006, 1: 4-CrossRef The Improved Clinical Effectiveness through Behavioural Research Group (ICEBeRG): Designing theoretically-informed implementation interventions. Implement Sci. 2006, 1: 4-CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Godin G, Belanger-Gravel A, Eccles M, Grimshaw J: Healthcare professionals' intentions and behaviours: A systematic review of studies based on social cognitive theories. Implement Sci. 2008, 3: 36-10.1186/1748-5908-3-36.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Godin G, Belanger-Gravel A, Eccles M, Grimshaw J: Healthcare professionals' intentions and behaviours: A systematic review of studies based on social cognitive theories. Implement Sci. 2008, 3: 36-10.1186/1748-5908-3-36.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Walker AE, Grimshaw J, Johnston M, Pitts N, Steen N, Eccles M: PRIME--PRocess modelling in ImpleMEntation research: selecting a theoretical basis for interventions to change clinical practice. BMC Health Serv Res. 2003, 3: 22-10.1186/1472-6963-3-22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Walker AE, Grimshaw J, Johnston M, Pitts N, Steen N, Eccles M: PRIME--PRocess modelling in ImpleMEntation research: selecting a theoretical basis for interventions to change clinical practice. BMC Health Serv Res. 2003, 3: 22-10.1186/1472-6963-3-22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Bonetti D, Pitts NB, Eccles M, Grimshaw J, Johnston M, Steen N: Applying psychological theory to evidence-based clinical practice: identifying factors predictive of taking intra-oral radiographs. Soc Sci Med. 2006, 63: 1889-1899. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.04.005.CrossRefPubMed Bonetti D, Pitts NB, Eccles M, Grimshaw J, Johnston M, Steen N: Applying psychological theory to evidence-based clinical practice: identifying factors predictive of taking intra-oral radiographs. Soc Sci Med. 2006, 63: 1889-1899. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.04.005.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Bonetti D, Johnston M, Clarkson JE, Grimshaw J, Pitts NB, Eccles M: Applying psychological theories to evidence-based clinical practice: identifying factors predictive of placing preventive fissure sealants. Implement Sci. 2010, 5: 25-10.1186/1748-5908-5-25.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Bonetti D, Johnston M, Clarkson JE, Grimshaw J, Pitts NB, Eccles M: Applying psychological theories to evidence-based clinical practice: identifying factors predictive of placing preventive fissure sealants. Implement Sci. 2010, 5: 25-10.1186/1748-5908-5-25.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Eccles MP, Grimshaw JM, Johnston M, Steen N, Pitts NB, Thomas R: Applying psychological theories to evidence-based clinical practice: Identifying factors predictive of managing upper respiratory tract infections without antibiotics. Implement Sci. 2007, 2: 26-10.1186/1748-5908-2-26.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Eccles MP, Grimshaw JM, Johnston M, Steen N, Pitts NB, Thomas R: Applying psychological theories to evidence-based clinical practice: Identifying factors predictive of managing upper respiratory tract infections without antibiotics. Implement Sci. 2007, 2: 26-10.1186/1748-5908-2-26.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Ajzen I: The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 1991, 50: 179-211. 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T.CrossRef Ajzen I: The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 1991, 50: 179-211. 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Bandura A: Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol Rev. 1977, 84: 191-215.CrossRefPubMed Bandura A: Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol Rev. 1977, 84: 191-215.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Blackman D: Operant conditioning an experimental analysis of behaviour. 1974, London: Methuen Blackman D: Operant conditioning an experimental analysis of behaviour. 1974, London: Methuen
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Gollwitzer PM: Goal achievement: the role of intentions. European Review of Social Psychology. Edited by: Stroebe W, Hewstone M. 1993, Chichester: Wiley Gollwitzer PM: Goal achievement: the role of intentions. European Review of Social Psychology. Edited by: Stroebe W, Hewstone M. 1993, Chichester: Wiley
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Leventhal H, Nerenz D, Steele DJ: Handbook of psychology and health. 1984, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Leventhal H, Nerenz D, Steele DJ: Handbook of psychology and health. 1984, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Weinstein ND, Rothman A, Sutton SR: Stage theories of health behaviour: conceptual and methodological issues. Health Psychol. 1998, 17: 290-299.CrossRefPubMed Weinstein ND, Rothman A, Sutton SR: Stage theories of health behaviour: conceptual and methodological issues. Health Psychol. 1998, 17: 290-299.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Walker AE, Grimshaw J, Johnston M, Pitts N, Steen N, Eccles M: PRIME--PRocess modelling in ImpleMEntation research: selecting a theoretical basis for interventions to change clinical practice. BMC Health Serv Res. 2003, 3: 22-10.1186/1472-6963-3-22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Walker AE, Grimshaw J, Johnston M, Pitts N, Steen N, Eccles M: PRIME--PRocess modelling in ImpleMEntation research: selecting a theoretical basis for interventions to change clinical practice. BMC Health Serv Res. 2003, 3: 22-10.1186/1472-6963-3-22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Conner M, Sparks P: The theory of planned behaviour and health behaviours. Predicting health behaviour Research and practice with social cognition models. Edited by: Conner M, Norman P. 1996, Buckingham: Open University Press, 121-162. 1 Conner M, Sparks P: The theory of planned behaviour and health behaviours. Predicting health behaviour Research and practice with social cognition models. Edited by: Conner M, Norman P. 1996, Buckingham: Open University Press, 121-162. 1
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Francis JJ, Eccles M, Johnston M, Walker A, Grimshaw J, Foy R: Constructing questionnaires based on the theory of planned behaviour A manual for health services researchers. 2004 Francis JJ, Eccles M, Johnston M, Walker A, Grimshaw J, Foy R: Constructing questionnaires based on the theory of planned behaviour A manual for health services researchers. 2004
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Bandura A: Self-efficacy the exercise of control. 1997, New York: Freeman Bandura A: Self-efficacy the exercise of control. 1997, New York: Freeman
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Bandura A: Health promotion from the persepctive of social cognitive theory. Understanding and changing Health Behaviour from Health Beliefs to Self-Regulation. Edited by: Norman P, Abraham C, Conner M. 2000, Amsterdam: Harwood Bandura A: Health promotion from the persepctive of social cognitive theory. Understanding and changing Health Behaviour from Health Beliefs to Self-Regulation. Edited by: Norman P, Abraham C, Conner M. 2000, Amsterdam: Harwood
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Moss-Morris R, Weinman J, Petrie KJ, Horne R, Cameron LD, Buick D: The revised illness perception questionnaire (IPQ-R). Psychol Health. 2002, 17: 1-16. 10.1080/08870440290001494.CrossRef Moss-Morris R, Weinman J, Petrie KJ, Horne R, Cameron LD, Buick D: The revised illness perception questionnaire (IPQ-R). Psychol Health. 2002, 17: 1-16. 10.1080/08870440290001494.CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Hrisos S, Eccles MP, Francis JJ, Dickinson HO, Kaner EF, Beyer F: Are there valid proxy measures of clinical behaviour? A systematic review. Implement Sci. 2009, 4: 37-10.1186/1748-5908-4-37.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hrisos S, Eccles MP, Francis JJ, Dickinson HO, Kaner EF, Beyer F: Are there valid proxy measures of clinical behaviour? A systematic review. Implement Sci. 2009, 4: 37-10.1186/1748-5908-4-37.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Green SB: How many subjects does it take to do a regression analysis?. Multivariate Behavioural Research. 1991, 26: 499-510. 10.1207/s15327906mbr2603_7.CrossRef Green SB: How many subjects does it take to do a regression analysis?. Multivariate Behavioural Research. 1991, 26: 499-510. 10.1207/s15327906mbr2603_7.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Freese J, Long JS: Regression models for categorical dependent variables using Stata. 2006, College Station: Stata Press Freese J, Long JS: Regression models for categorical dependent variables using Stata. 2006, College Station: Stata Press
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Bonetti D, Pitts NB, Eccles M, Grimshaw J, Steen N, Glidewell L: Applying psychological theory to evidence-based clinical practice: identifying factors predictive of taking intra-oral radiographs. Soc Sci Med. 2006, 63: 1889-1899. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.04.005.CrossRefPubMed Bonetti D, Pitts NB, Eccles M, Grimshaw J, Steen N, Glidewell L: Applying psychological theory to evidence-based clinical practice: identifying factors predictive of taking intra-oral radiographs. Soc Sci Med. 2006, 63: 1889-1899. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.04.005.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Francis JJ, Tinmouth A, Stanworth SJ, Grimshaw JM, Johnston M, Hyde C: Using theories of behaviour to understand transfusion prescribing in three clinical contexts in two countries: development work for an implementation trial. Implement Sci. 2009, 4: 70-10.1186/1748-5908-4-70.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Francis JJ, Tinmouth A, Stanworth SJ, Grimshaw JM, Johnston M, Hyde C: Using theories of behaviour to understand transfusion prescribing in three clinical contexts in two countries: development work for an implementation trial. Implement Sci. 2009, 4: 70-10.1186/1748-5908-4-70.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Francis JJ, Stockton C, Eccles MP, Johnston M, Cuthbertson BH, Grimshaw JM: Evidence-based selection of theories for designing behaviour change interventions: using methods based on theoretical construct domains to understand clinicians' blood transfusion behaviour. Br J Health Psychol. 2009, 14: 625-646. 10.1348/135910708X397025.CrossRefPubMed Francis JJ, Stockton C, Eccles MP, Johnston M, Cuthbertson BH, Grimshaw JM: Evidence-based selection of theories for designing behaviour change interventions: using methods based on theoretical construct domains to understand clinicians' blood transfusion behaviour. Br J Health Psychol. 2009, 14: 625-646. 10.1348/135910708X397025.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Sniehotta FF, Scholz U, Schwarzer R: Action plans and coping plans for physical exercise: A longitudinal intervention study in cardiac rehabilitation. Br J Health Psychol. 2006, 11: 23-37. 10.1348/135910705X43804.CrossRefPubMed Sniehotta FF, Scholz U, Schwarzer R: Action plans and coping plans for physical exercise: A longitudinal intervention study in cardiac rehabilitation. Br J Health Psychol. 2006, 11: 23-37. 10.1348/135910705X43804.CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Sniehotta FF, Schwarzer R, Scholz U, Schuz B: Action planning and coping planning for long term lifestyle change: theory and assessment. European Journal of Social Psychology. 2005, 35: 565-576. 10.1002/ejsp.258.CrossRef Sniehotta FF, Schwarzer R, Scholz U, Schuz B: Action planning and coping planning for long term lifestyle change: theory and assessment. European Journal of Social Psychology. 2005, 35: 565-576. 10.1002/ejsp.258.CrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Verplanken B, Orbell S: Reflections on past behavior: a self-report index of habit strength. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2003, 33: 1313-1330. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01951.x.CrossRef Verplanken B, Orbell S: Reflections on past behavior: a self-report index of habit strength. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2003, 33: 1313-1330. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01951.x.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Cummings SM, Savitz LA, Konrad TR: Reported response rates to mailed physician questionnaires. Health Serv Res. 2001, 35: 1347-1355.PubMedPubMedCentral Cummings SM, Savitz LA, Konrad TR: Reported response rates to mailed physician questionnaires. Health Serv Res. 2001, 35: 1347-1355.PubMedPubMedCentral
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Cook JV, Dickinson HO, Eccles MP: Response rates in postal surveys of healthcare professionals between 1996 and 2005: an observational study. BMC Health Serv Res. 2009, 9: 160-10.1186/1472-6963-9-160.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Cook JV, Dickinson HO, Eccles MP: Response rates in postal surveys of healthcare professionals between 1996 and 2005: an observational study. BMC Health Serv Res. 2009, 9: 160-10.1186/1472-6963-9-160.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
Applying psychological theories to evidence-based clinical practice: identifying factors predictive of lumbar spine x-ray for low back pain in UK primary care practice
verfasst von
Jeremy M Grimshaw
Martin P Eccles
Nick Steen
Marie Johnston
Nigel B Pitts
Liz Glidewell
Graeme Maclennan
Ruth Thomas
Debbie Bonetti
Anne Walker
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2011
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Implementation Science / Ausgabe 1/2011
Elektronische ISSN: 1748-5908
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1748-5908-6-55

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2011

Implementation Science 1/2011 Zur Ausgabe