Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Medicine 1/2011

Open Access 01.12.2011 | Research article

Best practice for motor imagery: a systematic literature review on motor imagery training elements in five different disciplines

verfasst von: Corina Schuster, Roger Hilfiker, Oliver Amft, Anne Scheidhauer, Brian Andrews, Jenny Butler, Udo Kischka, Thierry Ettlin

Erschienen in: BMC Medicine | Ausgabe 1/2011

insite
INHALT
download
DOWNLOAD
print
DRUCKEN
insite
SUCHEN

Abstract

Background

The literature suggests a beneficial effect of motor imagery (MI) if combined with physical practice, but detailed descriptions of MI training session (MITS) elements and temporal parameters are lacking. The aim of this review was to identify the characteristics of a successful MITS and compare these for different disciplines, MI session types, task focus, age, gender and MI modification during intervention.

Methods

An extended systematic literature search using 24 databases was performed for five disciplines: Education, Medicine, Music, Psychology and Sports. References that described an MI intervention that focused on motor skills, performance or strength improvement were included. Information describing 17 MITS elements was extracted based on the PETTLEP (physical, environment, timing, task, learning, emotion, perspective) approach. Seven elements describing the MITS temporal parameters were calculated: study duration, intervention duration, MITS duration, total MITS count, MITS per week, MI trials per MITS and total MI training time.

Results

Both independent reviewers found 96% congruity, which was tested on a random sample of 20% of all references. After selection, 133 studies reporting 141 MI interventions were included. The locations of the MITS and position of the participants during MI were task-specific. Participants received acoustic detailed MI instructions, which were mostly standardised and live. During MI practice, participants kept their eyes closed. MI training was performed from an internal perspective with a kinaesthetic mode. Changes in MI content, duration and dosage were reported in 31 MI interventions. Familiarisation sessions before the start of the MI intervention were mentioned in 17 reports. MI interventions focused with decreasing relevance on motor-, cognitive- and strength-focused tasks. Average study intervention lasted 34 days, with participants practicing MI on average three times per week for 17 minutes, with 34 MI trials. Average total MI time was 178 minutes including 13 MITS. Reporting rate varied between 25.5% and 95.5%.

Conclusions

MITS elements of successful interventions were individual, supervised and non-directed sessions, added after physical practice. Successful design characteristics were dominant in the Psychology literature, in interventions focusing on motor and strength-related tasks, in interventions with participants aged 20 to 29 years old, and in MI interventions including participants of both genders. Systematic searching of the MI literature was constrained by the lack of a defined MeSH term.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1741-7015-9-75) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Authors' contributions

CS, BA, JB, UK and TE made substantial contributions to conception and design of the review. CS, RH, OAM, AS made substantial contributions to acquisition of data, and analysis and interpretation of data. CS, OAM, BA, JB, UK, TE were involved in drafting the manuscript and critically revising it. All authors have given final approval of the manuscript.
Abkürzungen
CCT
clinical controlled trial
CS
case series
fMRI
functional magnetic resonance imaging
MI
motor imagery
MITS
Motor Imagery Training Session
MP
mental practice
PP
physical practice
PETTLEP
physical, environment, timing, task, learning, emotion, perspective
PEDro
Physiotherapy Evidence Database
RCT
randomised controlled trial
SCRD
single-case research design

Introduction

In sports psychology, there is evidence that mental practice (MP) can accelerate learning and improve motor skills. In their extensive meta-analysis in 1983, Feltz and Landers included single-group interventions with pre- and post-tests (tests before and after the interventions), and studies with multiple groups to compare an MP group versus controls [1]. They summarised 60 studies regardless of their quality and methods. Analysis of effect sizes showed that performing MP is not as good as physical practice (PP) but is better than doing no practice at all. In their revised meta-analysis in 1988, they replicated the previous results [2].
MP can be considered an umbrella term that includes various mental training interventions. In recent years, researchers have started to use the term 'motor imagery' (MI) to specifically address the imagination of moving specific body parts.
Over the past two decades, the publication of MP literature has increased tremendously, from 122 publications up to 1980 to a total of 20,011 publications in 2009 (PubMed search on 12 April 2010 with the search term 'mental practice'). The MI technique has been adopted in other research areas (education, medicine, music and sports), where the beneficial effect of MI added to PP has been confirmed, and 27 reviews summarise the research findings in those fields [127]. Despite the different review foci (for example, history and development of MI, theoretical concepts of MI functioning and effectiveness evaluation), all reviews attribute a beneficial effect to MI when added to PP. In some reviews, the methodological procedure lacked a systematic approach.

Aim of the current systematic literature review

None of the published reviews have analysed the design of the MI training session (MITS) to determine successful MI intervention techniques, such as the position of the person during MI, the number of MI trials, and the instruction mode and type. However, the MITS design is of vital importance for researchers and clinicians planning to implement MI interventions adapted to participant health status, age and gender. In this systematic literature review, we extracted and analysed 17 MITS elements based on the PETTLEP (physical, environment, timing, task, learning, emotion, perspective) approach. Furthermore, we analysed seven temporal parameters, including duration times and number of repetitions. In total, we analysed five disciplines in which MI represents an important training strategy.

Imagery models and frameworks in the education and psychology literature

Hall described the cognitive processes and neural basis of MI in a review on educational literature, based on a MEDLINE search [7]., and proposed a six-stage procedure for explicit learning of surgical skills: task definition, prior learning, mental rehearsal, reflection, problem solving and reality check.
In psychology, various tasks, participant groups and reporting statistics have been considered for MI. Driskell et al. summarised the effects of MP and determined under which conditions MI was most effective [13]. They defined five conditions of interest: 1) type of task, 2) retention interval, 3) experience level of trainees, 4) length of practice and 5) type of control group. The results of their meta-analysis showed a positive effect of MI when the following criteria were met: examination mainly of the cognitive aspects of the task performance, short retention interval, participants being novices to the task, and the MI session being about 20 minutes or shorter. They reported a non-significant trend for larger effects of MI compared with a non-treatment group and with an equivalent control treatment group.

Imagery models and frameworks in the sports literature

In the sports psychology literature, six imagery models and frameworks were reviewed by Guillot and Collet [26]. The models included a four-component scheme originally designed by Martin et al., who described how MI influences cognitive, affective and behavioural outcomes [20]. The six-stage model from Munroe et al. was also evaluated, including the well known 'W' questions (where, when and why do athletes use MI, and what do they imagine?) [28]. This qualitative method includes the type (visual, kinaesthetic) and perspective (internal, external) of MI. MacIntyre and Moran extended the framework of Munroe et al. by adding the question: 'How should MI be executed and used by athletes?' [29], and they described a multimodal model that includes definition, outcome and importance of MI. Holmes and Collins introduced the PETTLEP framework, building on findings in functional neuroscientific research literature and experience in sport psychology [30]. PETTLEP aims to facilitate designing MI interventions for athletes, and comprises seven components (physical, environment, task, timing, learning, emotion and perspective). These components describe the physical position of the individual, the environment that has to be imagined, the task involved, the timing or duration of the imagery, the learning or changes involved during imagery, the emotions that are associated with the task, and imagery perspective. By contrast, the three-step model described by Watt et al. focused on MI ability and two image-generation approaches: 1) vividness, control, duration, ease, and speed; and 2) visual sensory methods [31]. The recent framework proposed by Guillot and Collet aimed to combine key components from previously described models. Their Motor Imagery Integrative Model in Sport (MIIMS) includes four MI outcomes: 1) motor learning and performance; 2) motivation, self-confidence and anxiety; 3) strategies and problem-solving; and 4) injury rehabilitation. The scheme aimed to combine different imagery types (visual, kinaesthetic, olfactory, tactile and auditory) to create a complete mental version of the movement [26].

Motor imagery in medicine

MI research from sports psychology has been used in medicine, particularly in neurological rehabilitation [1113]. Literature reviews have evaluated the overall beneficial effect of MI [5, 9, 11], but none has described the MITS elements or temporal parameters. In this review, we analysed the MITS elements and temporal parameters that have been successfully used in different disciplines: Education, Medicine, Music, Psychology and Sports (in this review, we use the term 'Sports' for all studies that include athletes as participants and the term 'Psychology' for all studies including healthy participants who are not athletes).

Methods

Search terms and search strategy

Search terms were identified by a previous search of databases (including PubMed) and internet search engines (including Google and Google Scholar). Additionally, each searched database was checked for pre-defined MeSH terms, and where available, these terms were integrated into the search strategy. The following terms were used: 'mental imagery', 'mental practice', 'mental rehearsal', 'mental movements', 'eidetic imagery', 'visual imagery', 'guided imagery', 'motor imagery' and 'mental training'. The Appendix provides the complete search strategy for Scopus. The Scopus search strategy was adapted to individual databases and trial registers to account for specific vocabulary and syntax rules. No restrictions were made regarding year of publication, study design or age of the study population.

Study identification

Table 1 provides an overview of all databases, trial and dissertation registers, and conference proceedings searched. Database retrievals were imported into a reference management software package (EndNote; Thomson Reuters, Carlsbad, CA, USA). In total, 21,739 references were retrieved in February 2007. The literature search was repeated in June 2010 for references published between spring 2007 and 2010 in the largest databases of each discipline: the Education Resources Information Center (ERIC), Scopus, Répertoire International de Littérature Musicale (RILM), PsycINFO and SPORTDiscus. This search resulted in 5,741 additional references.
Table 1
Overview of searched databases, trial and dissertation registers and conference proceedings, and the number of references found
Number
Discipline
Database
Searched time period
References found, n
1
Education
Academic Search Premier
1975 to Feb 2007
1040
2
Medicine
AMED
1985 to Feb 2007
623
3
Education
ASSIA
1987 to March 2007
353
4
 
AEI
1979 to Feb 2007
84
5
 
BEI
1975 to Feb 2007
18
6
Medicine
BNI
1985 to Feb 2007
54
7
 
CINAHL
1982 to Feb 2007
1606
8
 
Cochrane Library
1948/1995 to march 2007
363
9
 
Digital dissertations
1930 to March 2007
30
10
 
DIMDI
1967 to March 2007
130
11
Sports
EMAERALD
1965 to March 2007
134
12
Education
ERIC
1966 to Feb 2007
795
13
Medicine
GMS meetings
2002 to March 2007
1
14
 
ISI Proceedings
1990 to March 2007
241
15
Music
JSTOR
1665 (1800) to Feb 2007
200
16
Psychology
PsycINFO
1887 to Feb 2007
4588
17
Music
RILM
1967 to March 2007
180
18
Medicine
Scopus
1996 to Feb 2007
2550
19
Sports
SPORTDiscus
1800 to Feb 2007
4023
20
Sports
SPORLIT, SPOFOR, SPORMED
1974 to Jan 2007
589
21
Medicine
ClinicalTrials.gov
1997 to March 2007
12
22
 
ISRCTN
1998 March 2007
2
23
 
National Research Register
2000 to March 2007
16
24
 
Web of Science
1970 to March 2007
2837
25
 
Zetoc
1993 to March 2007
1270
Total
   
21,739
AMED = Allied and Complementary Medicine, ASSIA = Applied Social Sciences Index and Abstracts, AEI = Australian Education International, BEI = British Education Index, BNI = British Nursing Index, CINAHL = Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature, DIMDI = German Institute for Medical Documentation and Information, ERIC = Educational Resources Information Center, GMS = General Medical Services, ISI = Web of Knowledge, JSTOR = Journal STORage, RILM = Répertoire international de Littérature Musicale, SPORLIT = Sporlit(eratur), SPORFOR = Sporfor(schung), SPORMED = Spormed(ia), ISRCTN = International Standard Randomised Controlled Trial Number

Study selection criteria

The references were selected for review inclusion based on the following criteria:
  • Any design of quantitative intervention studies with a focus on imagining movements.
  • Studies that included healthy volunteers, students, children, professionals, athletes or patients from any discipline.
  • Study intervention that focused on motor skill, performance or strength improvement.
The following exclusion criteria were used:
  • Mental practice not related to movements (audition, odour, any kind of visual imagery with static pictures).
  • Mental practice based on a computer-animated technique (virtual reality).
  • Mental practice used during a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) session.
  • Mental practice carried out during hypnosis or psychotherapy (guided imagery, eidetic imagery).
  • Mental practice used as mental rotation or diagnostic tool.
  • Suggested frameworks without participant evidence or experience.
  • Publication language other than English or German.

Selection process

During the manual selection process (Figure 1), articles were evaluated based on title, abstract or keywords. Two of the authors (CS and RH) reviewed the articles; CS evaluated all references, and RH evaluated a randomly selected proportion (20%) of the initial number of references selected from each discipline. Full texts were ordered if no decision could be made based on the available information. If one reviewer could not reach a decision for a reference, this reference was discussed by both reviewers, and if both reviewers had not been able to agree on a decision (which was not the case in this investigation) a third reviewer (JB) would have been consulted.
Owing to the large number of references, EndNote search options were used to eliminate studies based on the exclusion criteria.
To confirm the selection congruency between both independent reviewers, the inter-rater congruency was calculated. Reviewer agreement ranged between 78% and 100% (average 96%) for the five disciplines. Because some studies reported more than one MI intervention, the total exceeded the number of included studies. Each MI intervention was subsequently analysed as an independent investigation.

Data extraction

Information on study methods, MITS elements and temporal parameters were extracted by three researches (RB, AS, CS) and checked for accuracy (CS). Table 2 summarises all extracted information. Figure 2 illustrates the temporal parameters and the MITS terminology.
Table 2
Overview of extracted MITSa elements
Number
MITS element
MITS element description and categories
PETTLEP category
Dominant category found in successful MIb interventions
1
Position
Describes the position of the individual during MI practice as task-specific or not task-specific.
Physical
Task-specific
2
Location
Describes the location of MITS as task-specific or not task-specific.
Environment
Task-specific
3
Focus
Each task consists of different parts. Focus of the intervention classifies the main focus of task-related activities that had to be imagined: motor, cognitive or strength.
Task
Motor-focused activities
4
Order
Describes temporal order of MI and PPc trials. MI trials could have been performed before, between, after or simultaneously with PP.
Timing
MI after PP
5
Integration
Describes whether MI practice has been added to PP or embedded d into PP.
 
Added
6
MI instructions medium
MI instructions can be provided differently through one or more media types. Media type was scored as acoustic, written or visual. More than one media type could be assigned.
Learning
Acoustic
7
Instruction mode
In addition to the instruction medium, the mode was classified as live or pre-recorded (for example, using tape or video).
 
Live
8
Supervision
MITS could have been supervised or not supervised by an instructor present during the session.
 
Supervised
9
Directedness
MITS could have been directed d or non-directed when stepwise guidance was present or not.
 
Non-directed
10
Instruction type
The description of MI instructions varied. Instructions could cover detailed descriptions for each part of the task that had to be imagined, simple keywords, or coarse (broad) overall MI instructions.
 
Detailed
11
Instruction individuali-sation
MI instructions could have been individualised to the participant's problems with the task that had to be imagined (tailored), or could have been the same for each participant (standardised).
 
Standardised
12
Familiari-sation
Describe whether study participants had received an MI familiarisation session before the MI intervention began.
 
No familiarisation
13
Change
Indicated whether modification of content, duration or dosage of the MI training occurred, to facilitate the learning process during the MI intervention period.
  
14
MI session
MITS could have been classified as group sessions with more than one person participating in a MITS or as individual sessions with one participant only.
Emotion
Individual
15
Eyes
During the MI, the participant's eyes could have been closed or open. In some interventions, participants started with one condition and changed to the other after one or several MITS.
 
Closed
16
Perspective
During the MI, participants could have imagined the task from an internal or external perspective. In some interventions, participants started with one condition and changed to the other after one or several MITS.
Perspective
Internal
17
Mode
During the MI, participants could have used a kinaesthetic or visual mode. In some MI interventions, participants started with one condition and changed to the other during one or after several MITS.
 
Kinaesthetic
aMotor imagery training session.
bMotor imagery.
cPhysical practice.
dUsed in MI interventions with no change or negative results, and differing from successful MI interventions.

Study quality rating

Two rating lists were used because studies with different quantitative designs were included. The Physiotherapy Evidence Database (PEDro) list was used to evaluate randomised controlled trials (RCTs) (maximum of ten points) and non-RCTs (maximum of eight points) [32]. For case series or single cases experimental designs, the 11-point Single Case Experimental Design (SCED) scale was used [33]. All studies were rated by CS based on detailed rating guidelines. Studies received one point for each fulfilled methodological criterion on the respective rating list. The higher the achieved score, the better the study quality.

Data analysis

Raw information was extracted into Excel (Microsoft Corp., Redmond, WA, USA). After coding and classification, MI intervention data was imported into statistical analysis software packages (SPSS versions 16 and 17 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA), MATLAB version 2009b (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA)) for frequency analyses, frequency and mean comparison tests and visualisation. MI intervention data was not pooled or analysed for significant differences because of the variability in experimental settings and missing information in MI intervention descriptions. The heterogeneity between MI interventions was also present in standard deviations of temporal parameters. All MI interventions were classified into two categories: positive change (129 MI interventions, 91.5%), and no or negative change from pre- to post-test (12 MI interventions, 8.5%). MITS elements and temporal parameters of studies with positive change were summarised under the term 'average positive MI intervention' and used for comparison in three analyses.
First, trend analyses were performed to identify MITS elements for MI interventions with positive results versus no changes or negative results. Further analyses aimed to identify changing trends in MITS element frequencies in MI interventions with positive results for five different disciplines, integration approaches, MI training focus, session type, age and gender groups and MI intervention modifications. Secondly, the χ2 was used to test for significant differences between actual and expected observation frequencies for each MITS element. The tests were performed if 20% of the expected frequency showed an amount of 5 or more [34]. Thirdly, for temporal parameters, normal distribution was tested using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, and variance homogeneity was confirmed by the Levene test. Depending on the test results, group means were compared using Student t-test or Mann-Whitney U-test. The tests were used if at least five observations were available to estimate the statistic. For all temporal parameters, group means were compared against the average positive MI intervention.
For all statistical tests p ≤ 0.05 was considered significant.

Results

The bar charts of plot A (Figure 3; Figure 4; Figure 5; Figure 6; Figure 7; Figure 8; Figure 9; Figure 10; Figure 11; Figure 12; Figure 13; Figure 14; Figure 15) show the frequencies of MI interventions that reported details on MITS elements. For each MITS element, one or more categories were considered; for example, for the MITS element 'session', the categories 'group' and 'individual' were analysed. The categories of MITS elements added up to 100% if an element was reported for all MI interventions in the respective analysis. Relevant trends in MITS elements, as reported in the text below, were marked in plots A to F in the same figures. For temporal parameters, bars show mean and positive SD.

Study characteristics

In total, 133 studies were included in the analysis, reporting 141 MI interventions in five disciplines: Education (9 Interventions), Medicine (37), Music (5), Psychology (79) and Sports (11). For the studies published between 1941 and 2010, there were peaks in 1989/1990 (8 publications), in 2004 (18) and 2007 to 2009 (38). In Medicine, MI publications first appeared in 2000, with a steady increase until 2010. These studies originated from Europe, Australia/New Zealand, the Americas, Asia, and the Middle East.
The study designs comprised 91 randomised controlled trials (RCTs), 22 controlled clinical trials (CCTs), 15 case series (CSs) and 13 single-case research designs (SCRDs). Study quality was rated on a 10-point scale for RCTs (4 to 9), an 8-point scale for CCTs (3 to 6), an 11-point scale for CSs (4 to 11), and on an 11-point scale for SCRDs (7 to 10).
On average, RCTs and CCTs scored 6 on the 10-point PEDro scale, whereas CSs and SCRDs scored 6 and 8, respectively, on the 11-point SCED scale (on both, higher scores indicate better quality). Examples of MI instructions were available for 29 MI interventions, and changes in MI content during the MI intervention period were reported in 31 MI interventions. An overview of essential study characteristics is provided for each discipline separately (Table 3, Table 4, Table 5, Table 6 and Table 7).
Table 3
Overview of extracted descriptive study data for the discipline Education
Reference
First author
Year
Countrya
Language
Study duration, days
Intervention duration, days
Study design
Study groups
Number of participants
Participants
Gender
Age, years
Body part
Training task
Focus
Measurement events
Resultsb
Quality rating
                 
Relative change
Absolute change
 
[93]
Bucher, L
1993
USA
English
999
999
RCT
3
108
Nursing students
NSt
Range 19 to 21
Upper limb
Remove sterile gloves
M
1 (post-test)
5/10
[94]
Doheny, MO
1993
USA
English
1
1
RCT
4
95
Nursing students
Both
Mean = 21, range 18 to 40
Upper limb
Intramuscular injection
M
2 (pre-post test)
NSt
5/10
[95]
Immenroth, M
2005
DE
English
2
1
RCT
3
98
Surgeons
NSt
Mean ± SD = 32 ± 4
Upper limb
Laparoscopic cholecystectomy
M
2 (pre-post test)
9/10
[96]
Komesu
2009
USA
English
999
1
RCT
2
68
Surgeons
NSt
NSt
Upper limb
Surgical cystoscopy
C
1 (post-test)
8/10
[97]
Sanders, CW
2004
USA
English
21
21
RCT
3
65
Medical students
NSt
Students
Upper limb
Basic surgical procedures
M
2 (pre-post test)
7/10
[98]
Sanders
2008
USA
English
15
2
RCT
2
64
Medical students
NSt
NSt
Upper limb
Basic surgical procedures
C
3 (post-tests, FU)
9/10
[99]
Stig, LC
1989
UK
English
1
1
RCT
2
35
Chiropractic students
Both
Mean = 23, range 19 to 40
Upper limb
Chiropractic adjustment skill
M
2 (pre-post test)
6/10
[100]
Welk, A
2007
DE
English
999
999
RCT
2
41
Dentistry students
Both
Mean = 23
Upper limb
Preparation of tooth crown
C
2 (pre-post test)
8/10
[60]
Wright, CJ
2008
UK
English
999
28
RCT
2
56
Students
Both
University Students
Upper limb
Measuring blood pressure, antiseptic dressing task
C
2 (pre-post test)
8/10
a Countries: AU = Australia, BE = Belgium, BR = Brazil, CA = Canada, DE = Germany, ES = Spain, FR = France, G = German, GR = Greece, HK = Hong Kong, IL = Israel, IR = Iran, IT = Italy, KR = South Korea, NL = The Netherlands, NZ = New Zealand, PT = Portugal, SE = Sweden, UK = United Kingdom, USA = United States of America.
bTwo elements were used to describe the study results: relative and absolute change: relative change evaluates the MI group results versus results of other study groups, while absolute change indicates the change of the MI group from pre- to post-test. ↗, →, ↘ = indicate trends of the study results from pre- to post-test (↗ positive change, → - no change, ↘ - negative change, ≈ = no precise numbers of measurement events stated in the publication)
Abbreviations: BL = Baseline, C = cognitive, CG = control group, CRPS1 = complex regional pain syndrome type 1, int. = Intervention, M = motor, N/A = not applicable, NK, not known;,NSt = not stated, S = strength
Table 4
Overview of extracted descriptive study data for the discipline Medicine
Reference
First author
Year
Country
Language
Study duration, days
Intervention duration, days
Study design
Study groups
Number of participants
Participants
Gender
Age, years
Body part
Training task
Focus
Measurement events
Results
Quality rating
                 
Relative change
Absolute change
 
[84]
Bovend'Eerdt, TJH
2009
UK
E
999
56
RCT
2
11
Stroke, MS, TBI
Both
Mean ± SD = 50 ± 14
Whole body
Muscle stretching
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[58]
Bovend'Eerdt, TJH
2010
UK
E
126
35
RCT
2
30
Stroke, TBI, MS
Both
Mean ± SD = 50 ± 14
Lower limb
ADL tasks
M
3 (pre-post-test, FU)
8/10
[101]
Cramer, SC
2007
USA
E
9
7
CS
N/A
20
SCI
NSt
Mean ± SD = 31 ± 4
Tongue, foot
Tapping
M
2 (pre-post-test)
N/A
9/11
[102]
Crosbie, J
2004
UK
E
35
14
SCRD
N/A
10
Stroke
Both
Range 45 to 81
Upper limb
Reaching, grasping
M
10 (BL, during int., FU)
N/A
10/11
[59]
Dickstein, R
2004
IL
E
42
42
SCRD
N/A
1
Stroke
Male
69
Lower limb
Walking
M
5 (BL, midterm, post-test, FU)
N/A
9/11
[103]
Dijkerman, R
2004
UK
E
28
28
CCT
3
20
Stroke
Both
Mean ± SD = 64 ± 9
Upper limb
Reaching, grasping
M
2 (pre-post-test)
5/10
[104]
Dunsky, A
2006
IL
E
77
42
SCRD
N/A
4
Stroke
Male
Mean = 58, (64, 57, 63, 47)
Lower limb
Walking
M
5 (BL, midterm, post-test, FU)
N/A
9/11
[105]
Dunsky, A
2008
IL
E
77
21
CS
N/A
17
Stroke
Both
Mean = 58
Lower limb
Walking
M
6 (BL, pre-test, during int., post-test, FU)
N/A
11/11
[38]
Guillot, A
2009
FR
E
14
999
RCT
2
14
hand burn
Both
Mean ± SD = 47 ± 14, range 27 to 74
Upper limb
Wrist + finger movements
M
≈ 6 (pre-test, during int., post-test)
7/10
[106]
Gustin, SM
2008
AU
E
15
7
CS
N/A
15
SCI
Male
Mean = 47, range 26 to 67
Lower limb
Plantarflexion, dorsiflexion
M
2 (pre-post-test)
N/A
8/11
[107]
Hewett, T
2007
USA
E
56
42
SCRD
N/A
5
Stroke
Both
Mean = 53 ± 5, range 38 to 76
Upper limb
Reaching, grasping
M
2 (pre-post-test)
N/A
7/11
[108]
Jackson, PL
2004
CA
E
35
21
SCRD
N/A
1
Hemorrhage- related lesion
Male
38
Lower limb
Foot serial response time task
M
2 (pre-post-test)
N/A
8/11
[109]
Liu, K
2004
HK
E
21
21
RCT
2
46
Stroke
Both
Mean = 72
Whole body
ADL tasks
M
3 (pre-post-test, FU)
7/10
[110]
Liu, K
2004
HK
E
49
14
SCRD
N/A
2
Stroke
Both
65, 66
Whole body
ADL tasks
M
3 (pre-post-test, FU)
N/A
7/11
[111]
Liu, KPY
2009
HK
E
999
2
RCT
2
33
Stroke
Both
Mean = 70 ± 8
Whole body
ADL tasks
M
2 (pre-post-test)
8/10
[62]
Malouin, F
2004
CA
E
2
1
CS
N/A
12
Stroke
Both
Mean = 53 ± 12
Lower limb
Symmetrical load standing up + sitting down
M
3 (pre-post-test, FU)
N/A
9/11
[63]
Malouin, F
2009
CA
E
42
21
RCT
3
12
Stroke
Both
Mean = 61 ± 8, range 53 to 75
Lower limb
Symmetrical load standing up + sitting down
M
3 (pre-post-test, FU)
8/10
[112]
McCarthy, M
2002
UK
E
999
999
SCRD
N/A
2
CVA, TBI
Male
64, 36
 
Neglect
M
3 (pre-test, during int., post-test)
N/A
9/11
[113]
Moseley, GL
2004
AU
E
210
14
RCT
2
13
CRPS1 after wrist fracture
Both
Mean ± SD = 37 ± 15
Upper limb
Hand + finger movements
M
5 (pre-test, during int., post-test, FU)
7/10
[114]
Moseley, GL
2005
AU
E
126
14
RCT
3
20
CRPS1 after wrist fracture
Both
Mean = 34 ± 8
Upper limb
Hand + finger movements
M
5 (pre-test, during int., post-test, FU)
7/10
[47]
Moseley, GL
2006
AU
E
84
14
RCT
2
51
Phantom limb, CRPS1
Both
37
Upper limb
Hand + finger movements
M
3 (pre-post-test, FU)
7/10
[86]
Moseley, GL
2008
Western Europe + AU
E
1
1
CCT
2
37
CRPS1, no-CRPS1 pain
Both
Mean ± SD = 41 ± 14
Upper limb
Hand + finger movements
M
2 (pre-post-test)
5/10
[115]
Mueller, K
2007
DE
E
98
28
RCT
3
17
Stroke
Both
Mean ± SD = 62 ± 10
Upper limb
Finger+hand movements
M
8 (BL, during int., post-test, FU)
6/10
[116]
Page, SJ
2000
USA
E
28
28
RCT
2
16
Stroke
Male
Mean = 63
Upper limb
Weightbearing + functional task
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[65]
Page, SJ
2001
USA
E
56
42
SCRD
N/A
1
Stroke
Male
56
Upper limb
Whole arm movements
M
3 (BL, post-test)
N/A
7/11
[64]
Page, SJ
2001
USA
E
56
42
RCT
2
13
Stroke
Both
Mean ± SD = 65, range 64 to 79
Upper limb
Whole arm movements
M
3 (BL, post-test)
7/10
[117]
Page, SJ
2005
USA
E
56
42
RCT
2
11
Stroke
Both
Mean = 62 ± 5, range 53 to 71
Upper limb
Hand ADL tasks
M
3 (BL, post-test)
8/10
[118]
Page, SJ
2007
USA
E
999
72
CS
N/A
4
Stroke
Both
Mean = 63, range 49 to 73
Upper limb
Hand ADL tasks
M
2 (pre-post-test)
N/A
10/11
[119]
Page, SJ
2007
USA
E
63
42
RCT
2
32
Stroke
NSt
Mean ± SD = 60 ± 14
Upper limb
Hand ADL tasks
M
3 (BL, post-test)
8/10
[120]
Page, SJ
2009
USA
E
91
70
CS
N/A
10
Stroke
Both
Mean = 57 ± 12, range 37 to 69
Upper limb
Whole arm ADL tasks
M
3 (BL, post-test)
N/A
9/11
[121]
Page, SJ
2009
USA
E
168
70
RCT
2
10
Stroke
Both
Mean ± SD = 61 ± 3, range 48 to 79
Upper limb
Whole arm ADL tasks
M
4 (BL, post-test, FU)
8/10
[122]
Riccio, I
2010
IT
E
42
21
RCT
2
36
Stroke
Both
Mean = 60 ± 12
Upper limb
Whole arm ADL tasks
M
3 (pre-test, first + second study part)
8/10
[123]
Simmons, L
2008
UK
E
999
10
CS
CS
10
Stroke
Both
Mean = 68 ± 14
Upper limb
Whole arm movements
M
3 (pre-post-test, FU)
N/A
7/11
[66]
Stenekes, MW
2009
NL
E
84
42
RCT
2
25
Surgery for carpal tunnel syndrome
Both
Mean ± SD = 34 ± 11
Upper limb
Passive bending + straightening wrist + fingers
M
3 (pre-post-test, FU)
7/10
[124]
Stevens, JA
2003
USA
E
128
28
SCRD
N/A
2
Stroke
Both
76; 63
Upper limb
Wrist movements, object manipulation
M
4 (pre-test, during int., post-test, FU)
N/A
7/11
[125]
Tamir, R
2007
IL
E
84
84
RCT
2
23
Parkinson disease
Both
Mean ± SD = 67 ± 10
Whole body
ADL tasks
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[126]
Yoo, EY
2006
KR
E
10
999
SCRD
N/A
3
Stroke
Male
Mean = 57, (46, 70, 56)
Lower limb
Symmetrical weightbearing
M
21 (BL, during int., post-test, FU)
N/A
9/11
a Countries: AU = Australia, BE = Belgium, BR = Brazil, CA = Canada, DE = Germany, ES = Spain, FR = France, G = German, GR = Greece, HK = Hong Kong, IL = Israel, IR = Iran, IT = Italy, KR = South Korea, NL = The Netherlands, NZ = New Zealand, PT = Portugal, SE = Sweden, UK = United Kingdom, USA = United States of America.
bTwo elements were used to describe the study results: relative and absolute change: relative change evaluates the MI group results versus results of other study groups, while absolute change indicates the change of the MI group from pre- to post-test. ↗, →, ↘ = indicate trends of the study results from pre- to post-test (↗ positive change, → - no change, ↘ - negative change, ≈ = no precise numbers of measurement events stated in the publication)
Abbreviations: BL = Baseline, C = cognitive, CG = control group, CRPS1 = complex regional pain syndrome type 1, int. = Intervention, M = motor, MS = multiple sclerosis; N/A = not applicable, NK, not known;,NSt = not stated, S = strength, SCI = spinal cord injury, TBI = traumatic brain injury
Table 5
Overview of extracted descriptive study data for the discipline Music
Reference
First author
Year
Country
Language
Study duration, days
Intervention duration, days
Study design
Study groups
Number of participants
Participants
Gender
Age, years
Body part
Training task
Focus
Measurement events
Results
Study rating
                 
Relative change
Absolute change
 
[127]
Coffman, DD
1990
USA
E
1
1
CCT
8
2
Musicians
Both
Mean = 23, range 18 to 58
Upper limb
Piano-playing performance
M
2 (pre-post-test)
5/10
[128]
Ross, SL
1985
USA
E
1
1
RCT
5
51
Trombonists
Both
Mean = 22, range 18 to 29
Upper limb
Trombone-playing performance
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[129]
Rubin-Rabson, G
1941
USA
E
168
999
CCT
3
13
Piano teachers
NSt
range 21 to 25
Upper limb
Piano-playing performance, memorising new études
M
3 (during int., post-test)
4/10
[130]
Sonnen-schein, I
1990
DE
G
3
3
CS
N/A
20
Piano players
Both
Mean = 33, range 14 to 51
Upper limb
Piano-playing performance
M
2 (pre-post-test)
N/A
4/11
[131]
Theiler, T
1995
USA
E
1
1
CCT
4
14
Music students: guitar majors, voice majors
NSt.
Range 19 to 29
Upper limb
Guitar-playing + vocal performances
M
2 (pre-post-test)
5/10
Table 6
Overview of extracted descriptive study data for the discipline Psychology
Reference
First author
Year
Country
Language
Study duration, days
Intervention duration, days
Study design
Study groups
Number of participants
Participants
Gender
Age, years
Body part
Training task
Focus
Measurement events
Results
Study rating
                 
Relative change
Absolute change
 
[132]
Allami, N
2008
FR
E
1
1
RCT
5
25
Students
NSt
Mean = 29, range 20 to 37
Upper limb
Grasping task
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[133]
Alves, J
1999
PT
E
28
28
CCT
4
64
Pupils
Both
Range 15 to 17
Whole body
Volleyball service
M
2 (pre-post-test)
3/10
[134]
Andre, C
1986
USA
E
5
5
CCT
3
66
Students
Male
Mean = 21 ± 3
Whole body
Frisbee disc gold putting/throwing
M
2 (pre-post-test)
4/10
[135]
Chevalier, H
1986
CA/FR
E
1
1
RCT
5
30
Students
Both
Undergraduate students
Upper limb
Moving computer mouse
C
1 (post-test only)
6/10
[136]
Clark, LV
1960
USA
E
28
21
CCT
2
144
Pupils
Male
High-school pupils
Whole body
Pacific coast one-hand foul shot
M
2 (pre-post-test)
5/10
[137]
Clegg, BC
2004
USA
E
1
1
SCRD
N/A
28
Older adults, students
Both
Old: mean = 74, range 62 to 88; young: mean = 22, range 18 to 26
Upper limb
Movement with stylus
M
1 (post-test only)
N/A
8/11
[138]
Corbin, CB
1967
USA
E
28
21
RCT
3
30
Pupils
Male
High-school pupils
Whole body
Wand-juggling skill
M
2× BL, post-test, FU 1 day
5/10
[139]
Cornwall, MW
1991
USA
E
4
4
RCT
2
24
Females
Female
Mean = 23, range 21 to 25
Lower limb
Strength of quadriceps muscle
S
2 (pre-post-test)
6/10
[140]
Decety, J
1991
USA
E
1
1
RCT
2
20
Students
Both
Mean = 23 ± 2
Lower limb
Walking on beam
M
5 (during int., post-test)
6/10
[41]
Etnier, J
1996
USA
E
1
1
RCT
9
153
Students
Both
Mean ± SD = 23 ± 4
Whole body
Basketball shooting
M
3 (pre-test, during int., post-test)
6/10
[141]
Gassner, K
2007
DE
G
999
21
RCT
2
36
Students
Both
Mean = 24
Lower limb
Walking with knee prosthesis
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[142]
Gordon, S
1994
AU
E
21
21
RCT
3
64
High-school pupils
NSt
High-school pupils
Whole body
Cricket outswing
M
6 (pre-test, during int., post-test)
6/10
[143]
Gray, SW
1990
USA
E
21
14.0
RCT
2
24
Males
Male
Mean = 22, range 18 to 26
Whole body
Forehand and backhand racquetball skills
M
2 (pre-post-test)
6/10
[144]
Hellwing, W
1976
DE
G
14
14
CCT
2
72
Pupils
Male
Mean = 12, range 11 to 13
Whole body
Fosbury flop
M
1 (post-test)
4/10
[145]
Hemayattalab, R
2009
IR
E
38
24
RCT
5
40
Mentally retarded children
NSt
Mean = 14, range 12 to 15
Whole body
Basketball free throw
M
3 (pre-post-test, FU)
6/10
[146]
Herrero, J
2004
ES
E
7
7
CCT
2
27
Students
Female
Mean ± SD = 20 ± 0.1
Upper limb
Bench-press
S
2 (pre-post-test)
6/10
[56]
Isaac, AR
1992
NZ
E
126
126
CCT
2
70
Students
NSt
NSt
Whole body
Three trampoline skills
M
6 (after 1, 6, 7, 12, 13, 18 weeks)
6/10
[147]
Jaehme, W
1978
DE
G
21
14
RCT
3
48
Pupils
Male
Mean = 16
Whole body
Crawl swimming
M
2 (pre-post-test)
5/10
[42]
Jarus, T
2000
IL
E
1
1
RCT
2
89
Children, adults
Both
Children: mean ± SD = 10 ± 1; adults: 28 ± 5; older adults: 67 ± 2
Upper limb
Two-arm coordination task
C
6 (during int., FU)
6/10
[148]
Jones, JG
1965
AU
E
14
14
RCT
2
71
Students
Male
Students
Whole body
Hock-swing upstart
M
2 (during int., post-test)
7/10
[149]
Kelsey, IB
1961
CA
E
22
2
RCT
3
36
Students
Male
University students
Trunk, lower limb
Endurance abdominal + thigh-flexor muscles
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[150]
Kohl, RM
1980
USA
E
1
1
RCT
3
60
Students
NSt
Mean = 21
Upper limb
Pursuit rotor task
C
28 (during int., post-test)
5/10
[150]
Kohl, RM
1980
USA
E
1
1
RCT
3
60
Students
Male
Mean = 20
Upper limb
Pursuit rotor task
C
36 (during int., post-test)
5/10
[150]
Kohl, RM
1980
USA
E
1
1
RCT
6
108
Pupils
Male
Mean = 17
Upper limb
Pursuit rotor task
C
NSt
5/10
[151]
Kornspan, AS
2004
USA
E
5
4
RCT
4
40
Students
Both
Mean = 20
Whole body
Golf putting
M
3 (pre-post-tes)t
6/10
[152]
Kremer, P
2009
AU
E
1
1
RCT
4
209
Students
Both
Mean ± SD = 21 ± 3
Whole body
Dart throwing with non-preferred hand
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[153]
Krigolson, O
2006
CA
E
1
1
CCT
6
42
Healthy participants
NSt
Range 18 to 32
Lower limb
Walking along walkway
M
20 (during int., post-test)
4/10
[57]
Lejeune, M
1994
BE
E
7
4
CCT
4
40
University students + staff
Both
Mean = 22, range 19 to 27
Whole body
Counterattack forehand and backhand (table tennis)
M
3 (Pre-post-test, FU)
5/10
[154]
Linden, CA
1989
USA
E
14
8
RCT
2
23
Healthy participants
Female
Mean = 79, range 67 to 90
Whole body
Walking balance, equilibrium reactions
M
3 (pre-test, during int., post-test)
8/10
[45]
Lutz, R
2001
USA
E
1
1
RCT
5
120
Students
Both
Undergraduate students
Whole body
Golf putting
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[155]
Maring, JR
1990
USA
E
1
1
RCT
2
26
University students + staff
Both
Mean = 30; range 22 to 40
Upper limb
Tossing a ping-pong ball to target
M
2 (pre-post-test)
6/10
[67]
Martin, KA
1995
CA
E
6
6
RCT
3
39
Students
Both
Mean ± SD = 27 ± 6
Whole body
Golf putting
M
2 (pre-post-test)
6/10
[156]
McAleney, P
1990
USA
E
999
21
RCT
2
20
Students
Both
Mean = 19, range 18 to 20
Whole body
Tennis shooting skills
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[157]
Minas, SC
1978
UK
E
1
1
RCT
4
32
Students
Both
Undergraduate students
Whole body
Throwing performance
M
2 (pre-post-test)
6/10
[68]
O, J
2008
CA
E
999
1
RCT
5
97
Healthy students
Both
Mean ± SD = 18 ± 2
Whole body
Dribbling a soccer ball
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[158]
Papaxanthis, PC
2002
FR
E
1
1
RCT
2
16
Students
Both
Mean = 21, range 19 to 23
Lower + upper limb
Walking + writing task
M
5 (during int., post-test)
6/10
[159]
Phipps, SJ
1969
USA
E
21
21
RCT
2
72
Students
Male
University students
Whole body
Hock swing, jump-foot, soccer hitch kick
M
2 (pre-post-test)
6/10
[160]
Ranganathan, VK
2004
USA
E
231
84
RCT
4
30
Healthy participants
Both
Mean ± SD = 30 ± 5
Upper limb
Muscle strength of little finger abduction, elbow flexion
S
18 (BL, during int., FU)
6/10
[35]
Rapp, G
1973
DE
G
14
9
RCT
3
58
Pre-school children
Both
Mean = 6
Whole body
Skipping
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[161]
Rawlings, E
1972
USA
E
11
10
RCT
3
24
Students
Female
Undergraduate students
Upper limb
Rotary pursuit tracking
C
10 (pre-test, during int., post-test)
6/10
[161]
RawlingsE
1972
USA
E
10
9
RCT
2
20
Students
Male
Students
Upper limb
Rotary pursuit tracking
C
10 (pre-test, during int., post-test)
5/10
[69]
Reiser, M
2005
DE
G
28
28
RCT
3
34
Students
Both
Mean ± SD = 24 ± 2, range 20 to 27
Upper limb
Bench-press
S
4 (pre-test, during int., post-test)
6/10
[83]
Rodrigues, EC
2010
BR
E
1
1
CS
N/A
18
Students
Both
Mean = 26, range 19 to 33
Lower limb
Plantar flexion
M
2 (pre-post-test)
9/11
[162]
Ryan, E
1981
USA
E
1
1
RCT
3
39
Students
Male
Undergraduate students
Upper limb + whole body
'Dial-a-maze' pattern, stabilometer performance
M
2 (pre-post-test)
5/10
[163]
Ryan, E
1982
USA
E
1
1
RCT
6
80
Traffic officers
Male
Mean = 36, range 23 to 57
Whole body
Stabilometer performance
M
4 (pre-test, during int., post-test)
7/10
[164]
Shackell, EM
2007
CA
E
21
10
RCT
3
30
Students
Male
Mean ± SD = 20 ± 2
Lower limb
Strength-training of hip flexor muscle
S
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[165]
Sidaway, B
2005
USA
E
28
28
RCT
3
24
Students
Both
Mean = 23, range 19 to 26
Lower limb
Ankle dorsiflexor torque
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[166]
Singer, RN
1970
USA
E
35
28
RCT
5
65
Students
Female
College students
Upper limb
Learning a pursuit rotor task
C
2 (pre-post-test)
5/10
[167]
Smith, LE
1962
USA
E
1
1
CCT
6
60
Students
Male
Mean = 20, range 17 to 27
Upper limb
Hand-eye coordination task; punchboard learning task
M
2 (pre-post-test)
4/10
[72]
Smith, D
2001
UK
E
21
21
RCT
2
27
Students
Both
Mean ± SD = 20 ± 3
Whole body
Landing hockey penalty
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[71]
Smith, D
2004
UK
E
49
49
RCT
3
19
University students + staff
Male
Mean ± SD = 30 ± 8
Upper limb
Strength of abductor digiti minimi
S
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[71]
Smith, D
2004
UK
E
1
1
RCT
4
24
University students + staff
Both
Mean ± SD = 29 ± 8
Upper limb
Barrier knock-down task
C
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[168]
Smyth, MM
1975
UK
E
1
1
RCT
7
70
Students
Both
Undergraduate and postgraduate students
Upper limb
Mirror drawing of a star
C
2 (pre-post-test)
5/10
[168]
Smyth, MM
1975
UK
E
1
1
RCT
7
71
Students
Both
Undergraduate and postgraduate students
Upper limb
Pursuit rotor task
C
1 (post-test)
5/10
[169]
Start, KB
1960
AU
E
9
9
CS
N/A
35
Pupils
Male
12
Whole body
Basketball throw
M
2 (pre-post-test)
N/A
6/11
[170]
Start, KB
1964
AU
E
7
6
CS
N/A
21
Students
Male
Mean = 20, range 18 to 21
Whole body
Single leg upstart on high-bar
M
1 (post-test)
N/A
7/11
[87]
Start, KB
1964
AU
E
7
6
CS
N/A
44
Students
Male
Mean = 19, range 18 to 25
Whole body
Single leg upstart on high-bar
M
1 (post-test)
N/A
7/11
[88]
Start, KB
1964
AU
E
14
6
CS
N/A
32
Students
Male
Mean = 20, range 18 to 21
Whole body
Single leg upstart on high-bar
M
1 (post-test)
N/A
6/11
[171]
Stebbins, RJ
1968
USA
E
42
21
RCT
5
93
Students
Male
College students
Whole body
Throwing ball into target
M
8-18 (pre-test, during int., post-test)
5/10
[172]
Surburg, PR
1968
USA
E
63
56
CCT
7
183
Students
Male
Junior college students
Whole body
Tennis forehand drive
M
2 (pre-post-test)
5/10
[36]
Taktek, K
2004
CA
E
1
1
CCT
4
64
Children
Both
Mean = 9, range 8 to 10
Upper limb
Pushing play vehicle
C
2 (pre-post-test)
4/10
[73]
Toussaint, L
2010
FR
E
3
2
RCT
8
80
Students
Both
Mean ± SD = 23 ± 3
Lower limb
Knee joint angles
M
3 (pre-post-test, FU)
6/10
[173]
Tunney, N
2006
USA
E
2
2
RCT
2
19
Older adults
Both
Mean = 76, range 66 to 89
Lower limb
Walking with quad cane + climbing four stairs
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[174]
Twining, W
1949
USA
E
22
20
RCT
3
36
Students
Male
College students
Whole body
Throwing rings at target
M
2 (pre-post-test)
-1
4/10
[175]
van Gyn, GH
1990
CA
E
42
42
RCT
4
40
Students
Both
Undergraduate students
Lower limb
Power + sprint performance on ergometer
M
2 (pre-post-test)
6/10
[176]
Vandell, RA
1943
USA
E
20
18
RCT
3
36
Pupils and college students
Male
Junior, senior high-school and college students
Whole body
Throwing darts at target, basketball free throws
M
2 (pre-post-test)
5/10
[37]
Vergeer, I
2006
UK
E
28
28
RCT
3
36
University staff
Both
Mean ± SD = 41 ± 10
Lower limb
Flexibility around hip joint
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[74]
Wakefield, CJ
2009
UK
E
999
28
RCT
4
32
Students
Female
University students
Whole body
Netball shooting performance
C
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[75]
White, KD
1979
AU
E
9
8
CCT
4
24
High-school pupils + university students
NSt
Mean = 19, range 13 to 27
Whole body
Action-reaction swimming start
M
2 (pre-post-test)
4/10
[177]
Whiteley, G
1966
UK
E
84
84
CCT
4
88
Pupils
Male
Mean = 11
Whole body
Neck spring, head spring, short-arm overswing
M
1 (post-test)
3/10
[76]
Williams, JG
2004
UK
E
21
21
RCT
3
24
Undergraduate students
Both
Mean ± SD = 21 ± 2
Lower limb
Rom hip flexion
M
6 (pre-test, during int., post-test, FU)
7/10
[178]
Wohldamm, EL
2007
USA
E
84
2
CCT
4
80
Students
NSt
Undergraduate and postgraduate students
Upper limb
Number typing task
M
2 (post-test)
4/10
[178]
Wohldamm, EL
2007
USA
E
1
1
CCT
4
108
Students
NSt
Undergraduate and postgraduate students
Upper limb
Number typing task
M
3 (pre-test, FU)
5/10
[179]
Woolfolk, RL
1985
USA
E
1
1
RCT
6
48
Students
Male
Undergraduate college students
Whole body
Putt golf balls into cup
M
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[180]
Woolfolk, RL
1985
USA
E
7
6
RCT
3
30
Students
Both
College students
Whole body
Golf backswing and putting stroke
M
2 (pre-post test)
5/10
[77]
Wright, CJ
2009
UK
E
999
42
RCT
5
50
Students
NSt
Mean ± SD = 21 ± 4
Upper limb
Biceps curl task
S
2 (pre-post-test)
7/10
[78]
Yaguez, L
1998
DE
E
1
1
CCT
2
58
Volunteers
Both
Mean ± SD = 35 ± 11, range 22 to 73
Upper limb
Ideogram drawing
C
3 (pre-test, during int., post-test)
6/10
[78]
Yaguez, L
1998
DE
G
1
1
CCT
2
52
Volunteers
Both
Mean = 30, range 22 to 49
Upper limb
Connecting circles
C
3 (pre-test, during int., post-test)
6/10
[181]
Zecker, SG
1982
USA
E
1
1
RCT
4
40
Students
Both
Undergraduate college students
Whole body
Tossing beanbag to target
M
2 (pre-post-test)
5/10
Table 7
Overview of extracted descriptive study data for the discipline Sports
Reference
First author
Year
Country
Language
Study duration, days
Intervention duration, days
Study design
Study groups
Number of participants
Participants
Gender
Age, years
Body part
Training task
Focus
Measurement events
Results
Study rating
                 
Relative change
Absolute change
 
[182]
Casby, A
1998
UK
E
84
46
SCRD
N/A
4
Expert swimmers
Both
Mean = 16, range 16 to 17
Whole body
Freestyle swimming turn
M
50 (pre-test, during int., post-test)
N/A
8/11
[54]
Grouios, G
1992
GR
E
14
7
RCT
5
100
Top level athletes
Male
Mean = 22, range 18 to 25
Upper limb
Pushing button
C
2 (pre-post test)
7/10
[183]
Guillot, A
2009
FR
E
56
42
CS
N/A
10
Basketball players
Female
Mean = 23
Whole body
Three strategic basketball tactics
C
2 (pre-post test)
N/A
8/11
[55]
Guillot, A
2010
FR
E
999
35
RCT
2
21
Synchronised swimmers
Female
Mean = 15, SD 2
Whole body
Stretching exercises
M
2 (pre-post test)
7/10
[184]
Olsson, CJ
2008
SE
E
999
42
RCT
2
19
High-jump athletes
Both
Mean = 19 ± 3, range 16 to 29
Whole body
High jump
M
2 (pre-post test)
6/10
[79]
Robin, N
2007
FR
E
70
56
CCT
3
30
Tennis players
NSt
Mean = 19 ± 3
Whole body
Tennis service return
M
2 (pre-post test)
6/10
[185]
Shambrook, CJ
1996
UK
E
84
84
SCRD
N/A
4
Basketball players
Female
Mean = 20, SD 2
Whole body
Basketball free throw
M
26 (pre-test, during int., post-test)
N/A
9/11
[80]
Smith, D
2007
UK
E
999
42
RCT
4
48
University hockey players
Both
Mean = 20 ± 3
Whole body
Field hockey penalty flicks
C
2 (pre-post test)
7/10
[80]
Smith, D
2007
UK
E
999
42
RCT
4
40
Junior gymnasts
Female
Mean = 10 ± 2, range 7 to 14
Whole body
Full turning straight jump
C
2 (pre-post test)
8/10
[81]
Smith, D
2008
UK
E
999
42
RCT
4
32
Golf players
Male
NSt
Whole body
Hitting golf ball out of bunker
M
2 (pre-post test)
7/10
[186]
Ziemainz, H
2003
DE
G
14
2
RCT
3
27
Triathletes
Both
Mean = 16, range 15 to 17
Whole body
Changing between triathlon-specific sports
M
3 (pre-post test, FU)
5/10

Comparison of MI interventions with positive results versus no change or negative results: how should a successful MI intervention be implemented?

The MITS elements for all MI interventions were compared (Figure 3A). Frequency analyses of MI interventions with positive results revealed a number of key MITS elements present in a successful intervention design: MI was performed in individual sessions and added after PP; MI sessions were supervised and not directed; locations of MITS and the position of the participants during MI were both task-specific; participants received acoustic and detailed MI instructions, which were mainly standardised and live; during MI practice, participants kept their eyes closed.; the perspective used during MI practice was chosen from an internal view combined with a kinaesthetic MI mode; and MI interventions were mainly investigated with motor-focused tasks.
Only 17 reports mentioned an MITS for familiarisation before the MI intervention began. The reporting rate of all MITS elements ranged between 26% for the description of closed or open eyes to 95% for MI instruction individualisation. The most frequently reported MITS elements in successful MI interventions are listed in Table 2.
MI interventions with no change or with negative results predominantly used directed MITS. If MI integration was reported, MITS were embedded between or performed simultaneously with physical trials. Owing to the lack of reporting, the ordering of MI and PP could not be identified in 90% of all MI interventions. Only two MI interventions mentioned an MITS for familiarisation before MI intervention began. For the subsequent analyses only successful MI interventions with positive results were considered.
MI interventions with positive results had almost twice the duration of MI interventions with no change or negative results: study duration (34 days), MI intervention duration (21 days), total MITS count (13), (the number 13 stands for the number of MITS in MI interventions with positive results)MITS duration (17 minutes) and total MI time (178 minutes). By contrast, MI interventions with no change or negative results had a larger number of MITS per week (3) and a larger number of MI trials per MITS (34).

Comparison of positive MI interventions in five different disciplines: how do different disciplines use MI?

In the described analyses, only positive MI interventions were considered. The two disciplines with the youngest participants were Psychology (aged up to 9 years) and Sports (10 to 19 years). Most MI interventions were carried out with students (20 to 29 years) in Education, Psychology, Music, and Sports. Participants aged 50 and older were included only for Medicine and Psychology. Gender-specific investigations were carried out in Medicine, Psychology and Sports.
Detailed discipline-specific frequency analyses of MITS elements revealed the following differences (italics) from the average positive MITS.
For Education, participants (Figure 4A-F) performed MI predominantly before physical practice (PP) during directed MITS. Three MITS elements showed both categories: position of participants during MITS (task-specific and not task-specific), instruction mode (life and pre-recorded), and perspective (internal and external). MI content focused on cognitive task-related activities. MI mode was not reported. Regarding the temporal parameters in Education, the study and MI intervention duration and the total MI time were less than half of those in the average positive MI intervention, but the MITS duration was twice as long as in the average positive MITS. The number of MI trials per MITS was not reported.
MI interventions in Medicine (Figure 5A-F), the MI interventions predominantly used directed MITS with pre-recorded MI instructions. All temporal parameters had longer durations and total counts compared with the average positive MITS, especially for study and MITS duration, total MITS count and total MI time.
MI interventions in Music (Figure 6A-F) tended to be embedded into PP. MI instructions in Music were mainly written. Instruction mode and type allowed for more than one categorisation. Location of the MITS, and the MI perspective and mode used were not described. Almost all temporal parameters had lower durations and numbers than in the average positive MITS, particularly the number of MI trials per MITS, but the study duration was higher for MI interventions in Music.
MI interventions in Psychology (Figure 7A-F) most closely resembled the average positive MITS with similar distributions of MITS elements in instruction type and in MI perspective. MI interventions had the same number of MITS per week to the average positive MITS, but shorter MI intervention and MITS duration. Total MI time was half of that of the average positive MI intervention.
MI interventions in Sports (Figure 8A-F) reported embedded and directed MITS, [and after or between PP. Instructions during MITS were tailored and in written form. Study and MI intervention duration were almost twice as long as those in the average positive MI intervention. MI trials in a Sports MITS were only half of the number of the average positive MITS.

Comparison of added and embedded MI integration approaches: does a specific set of MITS element for each method exist?

This analysis was performed in successful MI interventions, which included PP in the study design. Of the 34 retrieved MI interventions, 20 described an added and 14 an embedded MI training method (Figure 9A-F). There was a preference for added MITS to be directed using pre-recorded instructions. Neither the locations of MITS nor the position of the participants during MI practice were task-specific. Added MITS used a kinaesthetic or mixed MI mode. MI training embedded into PP tended to be supervised, and was implemented between physical trials of the same task. Most MI interventions did not report details on location and position.
The averages of the temporal parameters of both categories (added and embedded) differed from those of the average positive MI intervention; however, there was wide variation between MI interventions. The duration of the study MI intervention and MITS duration were longer for both categories than in the average positive MITS, but the number of MI trials per MITS was lower. MITS duration was longer in the added than in the embedded training methods.

Comparison of MI interventions with different MI training focus: is MI particularly suited to one training focus?

Based on the primary focus of activities that were imagined, positive MI interventions were categorised into motor-focused (94), cognitive-focused (29) and strength-focused (6) activities. Compared with motor and cognitive-focused MI interventions, which were mainly published between the 1970s and 2010, the majority of strength-focused MI interventions were published in 1991 and in the period 2004 to 2009. Motor- and strength-focused MI interventions were often designed according to the average positive MITS (Figure 10A-F). Strength-focused MI interventions were investigated in healthy participants aged 20 to 39 years only. Motor-focused MI interventions had the highest number of MI trials per MITS and the longest MITS duration and total MI time.
Cognitive-focused MI interventions differed from the average positive MI intervention: there was a preference for MITS to be embedded and directed. Cognitive-focused MI interventions had shorter durations and lower numbers in all temporal parameters compared with motor-focused MI interventions.

Comparison of MI interventions with different MI session types: do group sessions require a different design from individual ones?

This analysis could be performed for 37 positive MI interventions that reported details. In total, 21 MI interventions described MITS in group sessions, and 71 in individual sessions (Figure 11A-F).
Group MITS tended to be directed and embedded into PP, and included MI practice before and after PP. Neither the locations of the MITS nor the position of the participants during MI practice were task-specific. Both the MI perspective used during MI practice and the MI mode changed. Total MI time and number of MI trials per session were only half those of the average positive MI intervention.
For the individual sessions, we investigated two options: directed and non-directed MITS. Compared with the average positive MI intervention, individual sessions had larger values for many of the temporal parameters, particularly total MI time.

Comparison of MI interventions with regard to participant age: did the implementation differ for particular age groups?

Participant age in successful MI interventions was classified into seven categories: up to 9 years (2 interventions), 10 to 19 years (18), 20 to 29 years (63), 30 to 39 years (13), 40 to 49 years (2), 50 to 59 years (9), and 60 and older (20). Two MI interventions did not mention the age of the participant and were thus not considered in this analysis.
There were only two MI interventions with participants aged up to 9 years, published in 1973 [35] and 2004 [36]. Both were studies in Psychology, which considered healthy children of both genders with an average age of 6 and 9 years, respectively, and were supervised with acoustic instructions. Rapp and Schoder described the MI intervention as a non-directed group session with live and standardised instructions. Children closed their eyes during MI as they imagined a motor-focused task [35]. No further details were provided. Taktek et al. designed the MI intervention as a directed session with pre-recorded instructions [36]. Participants used a task-specific position during MI, closed their eyes and used a kinaesthetic MI mode when imagining a cognitive-focused task. MI trials were preformed before PP trials using standardised and detailed instructions. Temporal parameters differed between both investigations.
The MI interventions (n = 18) with teenagers (10 to 19 years) were in the fields of Psychology and Sports. Investigations were designed as directed individual sessions. Where reported, MI was practiced either before, or before and after PP. Participants received their MI instructions in written form. Only the number of MI trials per MITS was less than that in the average positive MI intervention (Figure 12A-F).
The MI interventions (n = 63) with participants aged between 20 and 29 years most closely resembled the average positive MI intervention. Deviations were observed in three temporal parameters: study duration and total MI time were two-thirds of those in the average positive MI intervention, and participants performed more MI trials per session than in the average positive MI intervention.
MI interventions (n = 13) with participants aged between 30 and 39 years were mainly designed as added MITS with MI practice before PP and coarse (broad) MI instructions. MI mode was reported in two MI interventions as kinaesthetic and changing mode, respectively. Four temporal parameters had twice the duration or frequency than in the average positive MI intervention: study duration, MITS duration, total MITS count and total MI time. The number of MI trials per session was lower than those of the average positive MI intervention.
Only two MI interventions could be classified in the age group 40 to 49 years [37, 38]. The MI intervention described by Vergeer and Roberts was performed with healthy participants of both genders [37]. MITS elements resembled those of the average positive MI intervention, with group sessions and a multimodal approach for MI instructions (written, visual and acoustic). The second article, published by Guillot et al., described an MI intervention with participants (male and female) who had burns [38]. The authors used added, individual, supervised and directed MITS. Participant received detailed, tailored and written instructions.
The MI interventions (n = 9) with participants aged between 50 to 59 years were directed MITS (Figure 13A-F). Neither the locations of the MITS nor the position of the participants during MI practice were task-specific. Internal and external MI perspective options were offered. A changing MI mode was preferred. Temporal parameters had almost twice the duration of MI study and MI intervention than in the average positive MI intervention.
The MI interventions (n = 20) with participants aged 60 years and older were directed MITS, similar to the previous age group. There was no deviation in MITS elements from those of the average positive MI intervention. Temporal parameters had a longer study duration and total MI time.

Comparison of MI interventions with regard to participant's gender: should gender-specific settings be considered for MITS implementation?

Gender-specific analyses of positive MI interventions showed the following distribution: sixty-nine MI interventions with participants of both genders, eight MI interventions with female participants only and thirty-four MI interventions with male participants only. Gender-specific investigations were carried out in Medicine, Psychology and Sports.
The MI interventions with participants of both genders (n = 69) were designed according to the average positive MI intervention. They had the longest study duration and total MI time and the largest number of MI trials per session compared with gender-specific MI interventions (Figure 14A-F).
The MI interventions (n = 8) with female participants only were primarily designed as embedded MITS with MI trials between PP trials. The locations of the MITS were both task-specific and non-task-specific. MI instructions were live or pre-recorded. MI interventions with female participants were investigated with mainly cognitive-focused tasks. The MITS duration and number of MI trials were lower than those of MI interventions with both genders and with male participants only; however, the duration of the MI intervention was longer.
The MI interventions (n = 34) with male participants used task-specific or non-task-specific positions, and the MI mode selected was the changing mode. Temporal parameters closely resembled those of the average positive MI intervention.

Analyses based on change in content, duration and dosage of MI training: what MITS element variations have been investigated?

Successful MI interventions were categorised with regard to change in MI intervention during the total MI intervention period into change (n = 31) and no change (n = 38). Change in MI intervention could include three domains: MI dosage, MI content and MI time. MI interventions were excluded from the analysis if a change was not clearly described (n = 55) or if the categorisation was not applicable (n = 5) due to the study design.
Only minor differences were found between categories (Figure 15A-F). MI interventions with a change during the MI intervention period included directed MITS. Duration of study and MI intervention, total MITS count, and total MI time were almost twice those of the average positive MI intervention. MI interventions without a change during the MI intervention period were designed as embedded MITS with shorter study duration, lower number of MI trials and lower total MI time than in the average positive MI intervention.

Discussion

Summary of findings

A question frequently raised by clinicians is 'How should motor imagery be done?'. Our literature review aimed to answer this question and to describe which elements characterise successful MITS. It was not our intention to evaluate the effectiveness of MI or to compare effect sizes, as this has already been addressed in other literature reviews [1, 6, 11, 13]. The results of the trend analyses revealed changes in the frequencies of the MITS elements, which represent important variations between MI interventions. In addition, the review identified differences between the studies with positive results and those with no changes or negative results. Thus, the trend analyses might help clinicians to implement MI interventions successfully. By contrast, the χ2 test revealed general frequency distribution differences only, which were often caused by frequency variations and did not represent actual trend changes. Owing to limitations in the reporting rates, the χ2 test for MITS elements and the group mean comparison tests for temporal parameters could not be applied in many cases. We expect that our trend analyses in combination with the statistical test would be able to serve as indicators for potential future research directions.
Our analyses considered the differences in specific disciplines (Education, Medicine, Music, Psychology, Sports), MI integration types (added/embedded), session types (individual/group), focus of the task (motor, cognitive, strength), age, gender groups (female, male, both ) and change in content, duration and dosage. From 141 MI interventions, data were extracted and analysed for 17 MITS elements based on the PETTLEP approach and 7 temporal parameters.

MI intervention outcome

The comparison of the MI interventions with positive results versus those with no change or negative results provided the basis for all subsequent analyses. An average positive MI intervention was derived comprising MITS elements and temporal parameters. Characteristics of the average positive MI intervention were seen in studies in Psychology, in interventions with motor-focused or strength-focused tasks in all disciplines, in interventions with participants aged 20 to 29 years old, and in interventions with both genders. Four MITS elements differed between the MI interventions with positive results and those with no change or negative results: order (embedded/simultaneous), directedness (directed), number of MITS per week (n = 3) and number of MI trials per MITS (n = 34). We hypothesise that several of these elements jointly inhibit positive results. Depending on the length of a MITS and the experience level of the participant, the most frequent number of MITS per week chosen in successful MI interventions was three.
Data analyses determined that the average MITS duration was 17 minutes, with 34 MI trials per MITS. Both of these temporal parameters were also retrieved in the review of Feltz and Landers, published in 1983 [1], which yielded similar values. Our results suggest that not more than two MI trials per minute per MITS might be performed.
MI interventions with no change or negative results were present in all four study designs (RCT, CCT, CS and SCRD), with a higher average quality score for SCRDs than for RCTs. Therefore, it cannot be concluded that a certain design leads to a negative outcome.

Discipline-specific intervention adaptation

The use of imagery originated in the field of Psychology, with investigations dating back to publications in 1880 and 1897 [39, 40]. Presumably, MITS adaptations were necessary to direct each step of a surgical procedure in Education, to tailor imagery tasks to the needs of participants in Medicine, to use written instructions (musical notes) in Music, and to embed MI between PP trials as recovery breaks during an intensive training day in Sports.
In the current review, the positive MI interventions were mainly performed after PP. This result stands in contrast to the reported order of performing MI trials before PP in the meta-analysis of Feltz and Landers and the investigation of Etnier and Landers [1, 41]. No overall conclusion on the reported order could be derived because of its dependency on the aim of the MI training, such as the learning of a new motor task, its adaptation, preparation for performance of a known motor task, achievement of peak performance, and memorisation of performance aspects.
Temporal parameters varied between disciplines. The longest study and MI intervention durations and the highest total number of MITS were seen in Medicine and Sports. Some of these variations could be explained by their very nature. For example, in Medicine, time to learn and perform the MI was required, reflecting system impairments, older age of the participants and chronic pain, whereas in Sports, MITS can be part of the daily training routine. The longest MITS duration could be found in Music, reflecting the length of the music pieces that were imagined. Medicine and Psychology had the highest numbers of MI trials per MITS. This supports the hypothesis that MI is effective in these fields when the imagined movement is short and simple (for example, one limb movement) to perform, with as many repetitions as possible during a short concentration period, as described above in the section on MI intervention outcome.

MI session type

The decision to implement MITS as group or individual sessions does not depend on the MI integration approach. Both group and individual sessions included added and embedded MITS. Both classifications were used in positive MI interventions during the entire publication period analysed. The MI intervention duration was longer for group MITS and shorter for individual MITS compared with the positive MITS. We hypothesised that the selection of session type was based rather on practical considerations than on scientific reasoning. Further research is needed to evaluate the influence of session type on the effectiveness of MI interventions.

Age groups

Most MI interventions were performed with healthy students and young adults aged 20 to 29 years old. Hence there is a need for MI techniques and investigation of their effectiveness in young children and middle-aged adults, for which only a few references were found. Jarus and Ratzon reported that children aged 9 years and older adults aged 65 and 70 years benefited more from the combination of MI and PP than did young adults aged 21 to 40 years [42]. The full potential of MI in younger and older participants has not yet been sufficiently investigated, as evidenced by the low number of MI investigations found in these age groups.

Gender effect

In the current review gender differences were found in the chosen MITS elements. The results obtained will add to the ongoing debate on gender-dependent MI intervention design. Is it believed that males are better imagers than females, because of the different brain area activation and inhibition [43]. The 'bottom-up neural strategy' found in the work of Butler et al. could be related to the visuospatial performance benefit of men, with larger improvements for men gained from a motor-focused MI intervention compared with women [43]. This hypothesis could have influenced the MI intervention design in studies with female participants, which used mainly cognitive-focused tasks. However, a questionnaire survey given to healthy participants aged 18 to 65 years [44] did not confirm a gender imbalance on imagery usage. Furthermore, Lutz et al. did not detect a gender effect among high- and low-skilled golfers in a putting task after MI [45], nor were gender differences found in an investigation with two widely used imagery questionnaires [46]. In the current review, the study imbalance for female to male participants is 1 to 4.25. Therefore, we hope our analyses will prompt researchers to further explore potential gender differences in, for example, MI ability.

Methodological considerations

The only available MeSH term for searches was 'mental imagery', which must be considered as an umbrella term for various mental techniques. MI is one technique focusing on movements, which is important in rehabilitation medicine. Historically, other terms have been used for the same purpose in literature. Our literature search included various terms associated with imagery, yielding a large initial reference count.
Studies were included regardless of their study design, country and year of publication. This method enabled us to obtain a global view of the MI literature in different disciplines and of the MI approaches that were evaluated in different study designs. We used and adapted two widely accepted scales to evaluate all studies for their methodological quality.
The analysed studies primarily investigated the short-term effect of MI with a simple pre-/post-test design. The longest time period evaluated was a 6-month follow-up in an RCT by Moseley et al., in which significant improvements were seen in the MI treatment group compared with a control group [47].
Overall, data reporting in the selected articles was low, and the implications of this are highlighted by one of the least reported elements: imagery perspective. Depending on the chosen perspective (first or third person), different brain areas will be activated [48]. Publications on successful and non-successful athletes reported contradictory results for the imagery perspectives used [46, 49, 50]. Furthermore, Kim et al. investigated the exercise-related imagery perspective in middle-aged adults and, reported an internal:external perspective ratio of 1.8 [51]. Mulder et al. found a slightly better MI vividness in adults over 64 years when using the external MI perspective. The authors also mentioned that MI from an internal perspective is more important than MI from an external perspective in learning a motor skill [52, 53]. Furthermore, they could detect a shift in perspective related to age, with younger people more likely to use the internal perspective and older people more likely to use the external perspective. Taking imagery perspective as an example, future research should detail MITS elements more carefully.

Limitations and outlook

There were two important sources of possible information bias: firstly, 51 references were not obtainable, and secondly, our selected references included only 12 MI interventions with no change or negative results versus 129 MI interventions with positive results. We therefore hypothesised that MI interventions without positive results are rarely published. This hypothesis is further supported by our identification during the selection process of abstracts detailing preliminary results of MI interventions but no follow-up full article describing the whole MI intervention and its final results. Nevertheless, the aim of this review was to analyse MI interventions with positive results, and to identify discipline-specific MI interventions and fundamental intervention designs.
We found that the reporting standard of MI intervention had improved in recent years; however, investigations published before 2007 often lacked details on MITS elements, which resulted in missing data in the frequency analyses. Many investigations included more than one experimental or control group. In such cases, we focused our analyses focused on the experimental group with the largest change in measurement between pre- and post-intervention measurement.
The MI interventions were heterogeneous, which explained the large standard deviations in temporal parameters.
Task evaluation is complex and subjective, and to date, no standard classification exists. In our review we classified the investigated tasks based on their main focus: motor, cognitive or strength.
Before applying an MI intervention, it is essential to evaluate the MI ability of the participants to determine whether they are able to perform MI. Additionally, MI ability might change over an intervention period. In the current literature review, we found that assessments of MI ability had been used in thirty-six studies with positive results [3638, 5483] and in five studies with no change or with negative results [8488]. Heterogeneity between the MI ability assessments used, which were partially custom-designed for individual MI interventions, prevented direct comparison and relation to the study results in this review. We hope this will encourage researchers to use assessments of MI ability and to state participant scores in their research reports.
This review does not include MI interventions that were published after June 2010, because of the reference selection and analysis process. However, we briefly mention new articles currently under review in Medicine. Braun et al. embedded MI training into regular therapy in patients with stroke in nursing homes and in patients with Parkinson disease at different disease levels. In both investigations, embedded MI did not show a significant advantage compared with the control group receiving regular care [89, 90]. These interventions may add information for analysing positive results versus no change or negative results.
The current review focused on MITS elements to improve motor function or learn a motor skill. Further reviews should consider the influence of MI on psychological factors, such as goal-setting [91], self-efficacy, motivation and mood [92], and working memory.

Conclusion

This review covering five disciplines identified key MITS elements and temporal parameters of a successful MI intervention design. Successful design characteristics were dominant in the Psychology literature for all of the following: interventions using motor and strength-focused tasks, interventions with participants aged 20 to 29 years old, and interventions including both genders. Four MI elements were identified that differed between experiments with positive results and those with no change or negative results; however, success was not related to intervention study design. MI interventions in Education, Medicine, Music and Sports were adapted for different MITS elements and temporal parameters. No distinct characteristics were identified regarding the choice of group or individual sessions. Reports on MI interventions did not use consistent terminology, and often lacked details on MITS elements and temporal parameters. We hope this review will prompt researchers to a coherent usage of the MI term, which could facilitate subsequent meta-analyses.

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Appendix

Example search strategy

Search strategy Scopus database from 22 February 2007: (((TITLE-ABS-KEY("mental imagery")) OR (TITLE-ABS-KEY-AUTH("mental practice")) OR (TITLE-ABS-KEY-AUTH("mental rehearsal")) OR (TITLE-ABS-KEY-AUTH("mental movements")) OR (TITLE-ABS-KEY-AUTH("eidetic imagery")) OR (TITLE-ABS-KEY-AUTH("visual imagery")) OR (TITLE-ABS-KEY-AUTH("guided imagery")) OR (TITLE-ABS-KEY-AUTH("motor imagery")) OR (TITLE-ABS-KEY-AUTH("mental training"))) AND NOT (TITLE-ABS-KEY-AUTH("mental health"))) AND NOT (TITLE-ABS-KEY-AUTH("body image")) 2,556 references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Mr Jo Akers, Librarian, who helped with the search strategy and the reference export from the databases. We are also very thankful to all librarians who helped with the identification of authors, references and publications, and provided necessary documents. Additionally, we thank all the authors who answered our requests and provided the searched literature.
Funding
The project was partially funded by the Gottfried und Julia Bangerter-Rhyner Foundation.
Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Authors' contributions

CS, BA, JB, UK and TE made substantial contributions to conception and design of the review. CS, RH, OAM, AS made substantial contributions to acquisition of data, and analysis and interpretation of data. CS, OAM, BA, JB, UK, TE were involved in drafting the manuscript and critically revising it. All authors have given final approval of the manuscript.
insite
INHALT
download
DOWNLOAD
print
DRUCKEN
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Feltz DL, Landers DM: The effects of mental practice on motor skill learning and performance: a meta-analysis. J Sport Exerc Psych. 1983, 5: 25-57. Feltz DL, Landers DM: The effects of mental practice on motor skill learning and performance: a meta-analysis. J Sport Exerc Psych. 1983, 5: 25-57.
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Feltz D, Landers DM, Becker BJ: A revised meta-analysis of the mental practice literature on motor skill learning. Enhancing Human Performance: Issues, theories, and techniques. Edited by: Druckmann D, Swets JA. 1988, Washington D.C. National Academy Press, 61-101. Feltz D, Landers DM, Becker BJ: A revised meta-analysis of the mental practice literature on motor skill learning. Enhancing Human Performance: Issues, theories, and techniques. Edited by: Druckmann D, Swets JA. 1988, Washington D.C. National Academy Press, 61-101.
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Fell NT: Mental rehearsal as a complementary treatment in geriatric rehabilitation. Phys Occup Ther Geriatr. 2001, 18 (4): 51-63. Fell NT: Mental rehearsal as a complementary treatment in geriatric rehabilitation. Phys Occup Ther Geriatr. 2001, 18 (4): 51-63.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Batson G: Motor imagery for stroke rehabilitation: current research as a guide to clinical practice. Alternative & Complementary Therapies. 2004, 10 (2): 84-89. 10.1089/107628004773933325. Batson G: Motor imagery for stroke rehabilitation: current research as a guide to clinical practice. Alternative & Complementary Therapies. 2004, 10 (2): 84-89. 10.1089/107628004773933325.
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Braun SM, Beurskens AJ, Borm PJ, Schack T, Wade DT: The effects of mental practice in stroke rehabilitation: a systematic review. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2006, 87 (6): 842-852. 10.1016/j.apmr.2006.02.034.PubMed Braun SM, Beurskens AJ, Borm PJ, Schack T, Wade DT: The effects of mental practice in stroke rehabilitation: a systematic review. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2006, 87 (6): 842-852. 10.1016/j.apmr.2006.02.034.PubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Feltz DL, Landers DM, Becker BJ: A Revised Meta-analysis of the Mental Practice Literature on Motor Skill Learning. 1988, Washington, DC: National Academy Press Feltz DL, Landers DM, Becker BJ: A Revised Meta-analysis of the Mental Practice Literature on Motor Skill Learning. 1988, Washington, DC: National Academy Press
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Hall JC: Imagery practice and the development of surgical skills. Am J Surg. 2002, 184 (5): 465-10.1016/S0002-9610(02)01007-3.PubMed Hall JC: Imagery practice and the development of surgical skills. Am J Surg. 2002, 184 (5): 465-10.1016/S0002-9610(02)01007-3.PubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Mulder T: Motor imagery and action observation: cognitive tools for rehabilitation. J Neural Transm. 2007, 114 (10): 1265-1278. 10.1007/s00702-007-0763-z.PubMedPubMedCentral Mulder T: Motor imagery and action observation: cognitive tools for rehabilitation. J Neural Transm. 2007, 114 (10): 1265-1278. 10.1007/s00702-007-0763-z.PubMedPubMedCentral
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Sharma N, Pomeroy VM, Baron JC: Motor imagery - A backdoor to the motor system after stroke?. Stroke. 2006, 37 (7): 1941-1952. 10.1161/01.STR.0000226902.43357.fc.PubMed Sharma N, Pomeroy VM, Baron JC: Motor imagery - A backdoor to the motor system after stroke?. Stroke. 2006, 37 (7): 1941-1952. 10.1161/01.STR.0000226902.43357.fc.PubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Warner L, McNeill ME: Mental imagery and its potential for physical therapy. Phys Ther. 1988, 68 (4): 516-521.PubMed Warner L, McNeill ME: Mental imagery and its potential for physical therapy. Phys Ther. 1988, 68 (4): 516-521.PubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Zimmermann-Schlatter A, Schuster C, Puhan M, Siekierka E, Steurer J: Efficacy of motor imagery in post-stroke rehabilitation: a systematic review. J NeuroEng Rehabil. 2008, 5 (8). Zimmermann-Schlatter A, Schuster C, Puhan M, Siekierka E, Steurer J: Efficacy of motor imagery in post-stroke rehabilitation: a systematic review. J NeuroEng Rehabil. 2008, 5 (8).
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Annett J: Motor imagery: perception or action?. Neuropsychologia. 1995, 33 (11): 1395-1417. 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00072-B.PubMed Annett J: Motor imagery: perception or action?. Neuropsychologia. 1995, 33 (11): 1395-1417. 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00072-B.PubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Driskell JC, Copper C, Moran A: Does mental practice enhance performance?. J Appl Psychol. 1994, 79: 481-192. Driskell JC, Copper C, Moran A: Does mental practice enhance performance?. J Appl Psychol. 1994, 79: 481-192.
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Hamel O: "Imagerie mentale": A review of mental imagery in the French language on the world wide web. J Ment Imagery. 2001, 25 (1/2): 83-98. Hamel O: "Imagerie mentale": A review of mental imagery in the French language on the world wide web. J Ment Imagery. 2001, 25 (1/2): 83-98.
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Honeycutt JM, Ford SG: Mental imagery and intrapersonal communication: a review of research on imagined interactions (IIs) and current developments. Communication Yearbook. 2001, 25: 315-346. 10.1207/s15567419cy2501_9. Honeycutt JM, Ford SG: Mental imagery and intrapersonal communication: a review of research on imagined interactions (IIs) and current developments. Communication Yearbook. 2001, 25: 315-346. 10.1207/s15567419cy2501_9.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat McKenna KT, Tooth LR: Mental practice and its implications for occupational therapists: A literature review. Brit J Occup Ther. 1991, 54 (5): 169-172. McKenna KT, Tooth LR: Mental practice and its implications for occupational therapists: A literature review. Brit J Occup Ther. 1991, 54 (5): 169-172.
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Sheikh AA, Panagiotou NC: Use of mental imagery in psychotherapy: a critical review. Percept Mot Skills. 1975, 41 (2): 555-585.PubMed Sheikh AA, Panagiotou NC: Use of mental imagery in psychotherapy: a critical review. Percept Mot Skills. 1975, 41 (2): 555-585.PubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Surburg PR: Application of imagery techniques to special populations. Adap Phys Activty Quart. 1989, 6 (4): 328-337. Surburg PR: Application of imagery techniques to special populations. Adap Phys Activty Quart. 1989, 6 (4): 328-337.
19.
Zurück zum Zitat LeBoutillier N, David FM: Mental imagery and creativity: a meta-analytic review study. Brit J Psychol. 2003, 94 (1): 29-10.1348/000712603762842084.PubMed LeBoutillier N, David FM: Mental imagery and creativity: a meta-analytic review study. Brit J Psychol. 2003, 94 (1): 29-10.1348/000712603762842084.PubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin K, Moritz S, Hall C: Imagery use in sports: a literature review and applied model. Mental practice. Int J Sport Psychol. 1999, 245-268. Martin K, Moritz S, Hall C: Imagery use in sports: a literature review and applied model. Mental practice. Int J Sport Psychol. 1999, 245-268.
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Murphy SJ: Models of imagery in sport psychology: a review. J Ment Imagery. 1990, 153-172. 14 Murphy SJ: Models of imagery in sport psychology: a review. J Ment Imagery. 1990, 153-172. 14
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Richardson A: Mental practice: a review and discussion part I. Res Q. 1967, 38: 95-107. Richardson A: Mental practice: a review and discussion part I. Res Q. 1967, 38: 95-107.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Richardson A: Mental practice: a review and discussion II. Res Q. 1967, 38 (2): 263-273.PubMed Richardson A: Mental practice: a review and discussion II. Res Q. 1967, 38 (2): 263-273.PubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Taktek K: The effects of mental imagery on the acquisition of motor skills and performance: A literature review with theoretical implications. J Ment Imagery. 2004, 28: 79-114. Taktek K: The effects of mental imagery on the acquisition of motor skills and performance: A literature review with theoretical implications. J Ment Imagery. 2004, 28: 79-114.
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Grouios G: Mental practice: a review. J Sport Behav. 1992, 15 (1): 42-59. Grouios G: Mental practice: a review. J Sport Behav. 1992, 15 (1): 42-59.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Guillot A, Collet C: Construction of the motor imagery integrative model in sport: a review and theoretical investigation of motor imagery use. Int Rev Sport Exercise Psych. 2008, 1 (1): 31-44. 10.1080/17509840701823139. Guillot A, Collet C: Construction of the motor imagery integrative model in sport: a review and theoretical investigation of motor imagery use. Int Rev Sport Exercise Psych. 2008, 1 (1): 31-44. 10.1080/17509840701823139.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Leeuwen R, Inglis JTi: Mental practice and imagery: a potential role in stroke rehabilitation. Phys Ther Rev. 1998, 3 (1): 47-54. Van Leeuwen R, Inglis JTi: Mental practice and imagery: a potential role in stroke rehabilitation. Phys Ther Rev. 1998, 3 (1): 47-54.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Munroe KJ, Giacobbi PR, Hall C, Weinberg R: The four Ws of imagery use: where, when, why, and what. Sport Psychol. 2000, 14 (2): 119-137. Munroe KJ, Giacobbi PR, Hall C, Weinberg R: The four Ws of imagery use: where, when, why, and what. Sport Psychol. 2000, 14 (2): 119-137.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat MacIntyre T, Moran A: A qualitative investigation of meta-imagery processes and imagery direction among elite athletes. J Imagery Res Sport Phys Activity. 2007, 2 (1): article 4. MacIntyre T, Moran A: A qualitative investigation of meta-imagery processes and imagery direction among elite athletes. J Imagery Res Sport Phys Activity. 2007, 2 (1): article 4.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Holmes PS, Collins DJ: The PETTLEP approach to motor imagery: A functional equivalence model for sport psychologists. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2001, 13 (1): 60-83. Holmes PS, Collins DJ: The PETTLEP approach to motor imagery: A functional equivalence model for sport psychologists. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2001, 13 (1): 60-83.
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Watt AP, Morris T, Andersen MB: Issues in the development of a measure of imagery ability in sport. J Ment Imagery. 2004, 28: 149-180. Watt AP, Morris T, Andersen MB: Issues in the development of a measure of imagery ability in sport. J Ment Imagery. 2004, 28: 149-180.
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Maher CG, Sherrington C, Herbert RD, Moseley AM, Elkins M: Reliability of the PEDro scale for rating quality of randomized controlled trials. Phys Ther. 2003, 83 (8): 713-721.PubMed Maher CG, Sherrington C, Herbert RD, Moseley AM, Elkins M: Reliability of the PEDro scale for rating quality of randomized controlled trials. Phys Ther. 2003, 83 (8): 713-721.PubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Tate RL, McDonald S, Perdices M, Togher L, Schultz R, Savage S: Rating the methodological quality of single-subject designs and n-of-1 trials: introducing the Single-Case Experimental Design (SCED) Scale. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2008, 18 (4): 385-401. 10.1080/09602010802009201.PubMed Tate RL, McDonald S, Perdices M, Togher L, Schultz R, Savage S: Rating the methodological quality of single-subject designs and n-of-1 trials: introducing the Single-Case Experimental Design (SCED) Scale. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2008, 18 (4): 385-401. 10.1080/09602010802009201.PubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Munro BH: Statistical methods for health care research. 2005, Philadelphia, Penn.; London: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 5 Munro BH: Statistical methods for health care research. 2005, Philadelphia, Penn.; London: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 5
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Rapp G, Schoder G: Kinesthetic imagination and learning of motor skills. Psychol Erz Unterr. 1973, 20 (5): 279-288. Rapp G, Schoder G: Kinesthetic imagination and learning of motor skills. Psychol Erz Unterr. 1973, 20 (5): 279-288.
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Taktek K, Salmoni A, Rigal R: The Effects of Mental Imagery on the Learning and Transfer of a Discrete Motor Task by Young Children. J Ment Imagery. 2004, 28 (3-4): 87-120. Taktek K, Salmoni A, Rigal R: The Effects of Mental Imagery on the Learning and Transfer of a Discrete Motor Task by Young Children. J Ment Imagery. 2004, 28 (3-4): 87-120.
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Vergeer I, J R: Movement and stretching imagery during flexibility training. Journal of sports sciences. 2006, 24 (2): 197-208. 10.1080/02640410500131811.PubMed Vergeer I, J R: Movement and stretching imagery during flexibility training. Journal of sports sciences. 2006, 24 (2): 197-208. 10.1080/02640410500131811.PubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Guillot A, Lebon F, Vernay M, Girbon JP, Doyon J, Collet C: Effect of motor imagery in the rehabilitation of burn patients. J Burn Care Res. 2009, 30 (4): 686-693. 10.1097/BCR.0b013e3181ac0003.PubMed Guillot A, Lebon F, Vernay M, Girbon JP, Doyon J, Collet C: Effect of motor imagery in the rehabilitation of burn patients. J Burn Care Res. 2009, 30 (4): 686-693. 10.1097/BCR.0b013e3181ac0003.PubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Cohn J: Experimentelle Untersuchungen über das Zusammenwirken des akustisch motorischen und des visuellen Gedächtnisses. Z Psychol Sinn. 1897, 15: 161-183. Cohn J: Experimentelle Untersuchungen über das Zusammenwirken des akustisch motorischen und des visuellen Gedächtnisses. Z Psychol Sinn. 1897, 15: 161-183.
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Galton F: Statistics of mental imagery. Mind. 1880, 5: 301-318. Galton F: Statistics of mental imagery. Mind. 1880, 5: 301-318.
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Etnier JL, Landers DM: The influence of procedural variables on the efficacy of mental practice. Sport Psychol. 1996, 10 (1): 48-57. Etnier JL, Landers DM: The influence of procedural variables on the efficacy of mental practice. Sport Psychol. 1996, 10 (1): 48-57.
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Jarus T, Ratzon NZ: Can you imagine? The effect of mental practice on the acquisition and retention of a motor skill as a function of age. Occup ther J Res. 2000, 20 (3): 163-178. Jarus T, Ratzon NZ: Can you imagine? The effect of mental practice on the acquisition and retention of a motor skill as a function of age. Occup ther J Res. 2000, 20 (3): 163-178.
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Butler T, Imperato-McGinley J, Pan H, Voyer D, Cordero J, Zhu YS, Stern E, Silbersweig D: Sex differences in mental rotation: Top-down versus bottom-up processing. Neuroimage. 2006, 32 (1): 445-456. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.030.PubMed Butler T, Imperato-McGinley J, Pan H, Voyer D, Cordero J, Zhu YS, Stern E, Silbersweig D: Sex differences in mental rotation: Top-down versus bottom-up processing. Neuroimage. 2006, 32 (1): 445-456. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.030.PubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Giacobbi P: Age and activity-level differences in the use of exercise imagery. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2007, 19 (4): 487-493. 10.1080/10413200701601508. Giacobbi P: Age and activity-level differences in the use of exercise imagery. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2007, 19 (4): 487-493. 10.1080/10413200701601508.
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Lutz R, Landers DM, Linder DE: Procedural variables and skill level influences on pre-performance mental practice efficacy. J Ment Imagery. 2001, 25 (3-4): 115-134. Lutz R, Landers DM, Linder DE: Procedural variables and skill level influences on pre-performance mental practice efficacy. J Ment Imagery. 2001, 25 (3-4): 115-134.
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Callow N, Hardy L: The relationship between the use of kinaesthetic imagery and different visual imagery perspectives. J Sports Sci. 2004, 22 (2): 167-177. 10.1080/02640410310001641449.PubMed Callow N, Hardy L: The relationship between the use of kinaesthetic imagery and different visual imagery perspectives. J Sports Sci. 2004, 22 (2): 167-177. 10.1080/02640410310001641449.PubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Moseley GL: Graded motor imagery for pathologic pain - A randomized controlled trial. Neurology. 2006, 67 (12): 2129-2134. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000249112.56935.32.PubMed Moseley GL: Graded motor imagery for pathologic pain - A randomized controlled trial. Neurology. 2006, 67 (12): 2129-2134. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000249112.56935.32.PubMed
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Lorey B, Bischoff M, Pilgramm S, Stark R, Munzert J, Zentgraf K: The embodied nature of motor imagery: the influence of posture and perspective. Exp Brain Res. 2009, 194 (2): 233-243. 10.1007/s00221-008-1693-1.PubMed Lorey B, Bischoff M, Pilgramm S, Stark R, Munzert J, Zentgraf K: The embodied nature of motor imagery: the influence of posture and perspective. Exp Brain Res. 2009, 194 (2): 233-243. 10.1007/s00221-008-1693-1.PubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Mahoney MJ, Avener M: Psychology of the elite athlete: an exploratory study. Cognitive Ther Res. 1977, 2: 135-141. Mahoney MJ, Avener M: Psychology of the elite athlete: an exploratory study. Cognitive Ther Res. 1977, 2: 135-141.
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Ungerleider S, Golding JM: Mental practice among Olympic athletes. Percep motor skills. 1991, 72: 1007-1017. 10.2466/PMS.72.3.1007-1017. Ungerleider S, Golding JM: Mental practice among Olympic athletes. Percep motor skills. 1991, 72: 1007-1017. 10.2466/PMS.72.3.1007-1017.
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim BH, Giacobbi PR: The Use of exercise-related mental imagery by middle-aged adults. J Imagery Res Sport Phys Activity. 2009, 4 (1): Article 1 Kim BH, Giacobbi PR: The Use of exercise-related mental imagery by middle-aged adults. J Imagery Res Sport Phys Activity. 2009, 4 (1): Article 1
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Mulder T, Hochstenbach JBH, van Heuvelen MJG, den Otter AR: Motor imagery: The relation between age and imagery capacity. Hum Movement Sci. 2007, 26 (2): 203-211. 10.1016/j.humov.2007.01.001. Mulder T, Hochstenbach JBH, van Heuvelen MJG, den Otter AR: Motor imagery: The relation between age and imagery capacity. Hum Movement Sci. 2007, 26 (2): 203-211. 10.1016/j.humov.2007.01.001.
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Jackson PL, Lafleur MF, Malouin F, Richards C, Doyon J: Potential role of mental practice using motor imagery in neurologic rehabilitation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2001, 82 (8): 1133-1141. 10.1053/apmr.2001.24286.PubMed Jackson PL, Lafleur MF, Malouin F, Richards C, Doyon J: Potential role of mental practice using motor imagery in neurologic rehabilitation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2001, 82 (8): 1133-1141. 10.1053/apmr.2001.24286.PubMed
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Grouios G: On the reduction of reaction time with mental practice. J Sport Behav. 1992, 15 (2): 141-157. Grouios G: On the reduction of reaction time with mental practice. J Sport Behav. 1992, 15 (2): 141-157.
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Guillot A, Tolleron C, Collet C: Does motor imagery enhance stretching and flexibility?. J Sport Sci. 2010, 28 (3): 291-298. 10.1080/02640410903473828. Guillot A, Tolleron C, Collet C: Does motor imagery enhance stretching and flexibility?. J Sport Sci. 2010, 28 (3): 291-298. 10.1080/02640410903473828.
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Isaac AR: Mental practice: Does it work in the field?. Sport Psychol. 1992, 6 (2): 192-198. Isaac AR: Mental practice: Does it work in the field?. Sport Psychol. 1992, 6 (2): 192-198.
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Lejeune M, Decker C, Sanchez X: Mental rehearsal in table tennis performance. Percept Motor Skills. 1994, 79 (Spec Issue Aug): 627-641.PubMed Lejeune M, Decker C, Sanchez X: Mental rehearsal in table tennis performance. Percept Motor Skills. 1994, 79 (Spec Issue Aug): 627-641.PubMed
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Bovend'Eerdt TJ, Dawes H, Sackley C, Izadi H, Wade DT: An integrated motor imagery program to improve functional task performance in neurorehabilitation: a single-blind randomized controlled trial. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2010, 91 (6): 939-946. 10.1016/j.apmr.2010.03.008.PubMed Bovend'Eerdt TJ, Dawes H, Sackley C, Izadi H, Wade DT: An integrated motor imagery program to improve functional task performance in neurorehabilitation: a single-blind randomized controlled trial. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2010, 91 (6): 939-946. 10.1016/j.apmr.2010.03.008.PubMed
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Dickstein R, Dunsky A, Marcovitz E: Motor imagery for gait rehabilitation in post-stroke hemiparesis. Phys Ther. 2004, 84 (12): 1167-1177.PubMed Dickstein R, Dunsky A, Marcovitz E: Motor imagery for gait rehabilitation in post-stroke hemiparesis. Phys Ther. 2004, 84 (12): 1167-1177.PubMed
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Wright C, Hogard E, Ellis R, Smith D, Kelly C: Effect of PETTLEP imagery training on performance of nursing skills: Pilot study. J Adv Nurs. 2008, 63 (3): 259-265. 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2008.04706.x.PubMed Wright C, Hogard E, Ellis R, Smith D, Kelly C: Effect of PETTLEP imagery training on performance of nursing skills: Pilot study. J Adv Nurs. 2008, 63 (3): 259-265. 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2008.04706.x.PubMed
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Jackson PL, Richards CL, Doyon J, Malouin Fi: The efficacy of combined physical and mental practice in the learning of a foot-sequence task after stroke: A case report. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2004, 18 (2): 106-111. 10.1177/0888439004265249.PubMed Jackson PL, Richards CL, Doyon J, Malouin Fi: The efficacy of combined physical and mental practice in the learning of a foot-sequence task after stroke: A case report. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2004, 18 (2): 106-111. 10.1177/0888439004265249.PubMed
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Malouin F, Richards CL, Doyon J, Desrosiers J, Belleville S: Training mobility tasks after stroke with combined mental and physical practice: a feasibility study. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2004, 18 (2): 66-75. 10.1177/0888439004266304.PubMed Malouin F, Richards CL, Doyon J, Desrosiers J, Belleville S: Training mobility tasks after stroke with combined mental and physical practice: a feasibility study. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2004, 18 (2): 66-75. 10.1177/0888439004266304.PubMed
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Malouin F, Richards CL, Durand A, Descent M, Poire D, Fremont P, Pelet S, Gresset J, Doyon J: Effects of practice, visual loss, limb amputation, and disuse on motor imagery vividness. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2009, 23 (5): 449-463. 10.1177/1545968308328733.PubMed Malouin F, Richards CL, Durand A, Descent M, Poire D, Fremont P, Pelet S, Gresset J, Doyon J: Effects of practice, visual loss, limb amputation, and disuse on motor imagery vividness. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2009, 23 (5): 449-463. 10.1177/1545968308328733.PubMed
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Page SJ, Levine P, Sisto S, Johnston MV: A randomized efficacy and feasibility study of imagery in acute stroke. Clin Rehabil. 2001, 15 (3): 233-240. 10.1191/026921501672063235.PubMed Page SJ, Levine P, Sisto S, Johnston MV: A randomized efficacy and feasibility study of imagery in acute stroke. Clin Rehabil. 2001, 15 (3): 233-240. 10.1191/026921501672063235.PubMed
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Page SJ, Levine P, Sisto SA, Johnston MV: Mental practice combined with physical practice for upper-limb motor deficit in subacute stroke. Phys Ther. 2001, 81 (8): 1455-1462.PubMed Page SJ, Levine P, Sisto SA, Johnston MV: Mental practice combined with physical practice for upper-limb motor deficit in subacute stroke. Phys Ther. 2001, 81 (8): 1455-1462.PubMed
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Stenekes MW, Geertzen JH, Nicolai JPA, De Jong BM, Mulder T: Motor imagery for peripheral injury response. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2009, 90 (8): 1443-1444. Stenekes MW, Geertzen JH, Nicolai JPA, De Jong BM, Mulder T: Motor imagery for peripheral injury response. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2009, 90 (8): 1443-1444.
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin KA, Hall CR: Using mental imagery to enhance intrinsic motivation. J Sport Exerc Psych. 1995, 17 (1): 54-69. Martin KA, Hall CR: Using mental imagery to enhance intrinsic motivation. J Sport Exerc Psych. 1995, 17 (1): 54-69.
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Munroe JO, Chandler KJ: The effects of image speed on the performance of a soccer task. Sport Psychol. 2008, 22 (1): 1-17. Munroe JO, Chandler KJ: The effects of image speed on the performance of a soccer task. Sport Psychol. 2008, 22 (1): 1-17.
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Reiser M: Strength gains by motor imagery of maximal muscle contractions. Z Sportpsychol. 2005, 11-21. Reiser M: Strength gains by motor imagery of maximal muscle contractions. Z Sportpsychol. 2005, 11-21.
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Ryan ED, Simons J: Cognitive demand, imagery, and frequency of mental rehearsal as factors influencing acquistion of motor skills. J Sport Psychol. 1981, 3: 35-45. Ryan ED, Simons J: Cognitive demand, imagery, and frequency of mental rehearsal as factors influencing acquistion of motor skills. J Sport Psychol. 1981, 3: 35-45.
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith D, Collins D: Mental practice, motor performance, and the late CNV. J Sport Exerc Psych. 2004, 26 (3): 412-426. Smith D, Collins D: Mental practice, motor performance, and the late CNV. J Sport Exerc Psych. 2004, 26 (3): 412-426.
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith D, Holmes P, Whitemore L, Collins D, Devonport T: The effect of theoretically-based imagery scripts on field hockey performance. J Sport Behav. 2001, 24 (4): 408-419. Smith D, Holmes P, Whitemore L, Collins D, Devonport T: The effect of theoretically-based imagery scripts on field hockey performance. J Sport Behav. 2001, 24 (4): 408-419.
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Toussaint L, Blandin Y: On the role of imagery modalities on motor learning. J Sport Sci. 2010, 28 (5): 497-504. 10.1080/02640410903555855. Toussaint L, Blandin Y: On the role of imagery modalities on motor learning. J Sport Sci. 2010, 28 (5): 497-504. 10.1080/02640410903555855.
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Wakefield CJ, Smith D: Impact of differing frequencies of PETTLEP imagery on netball shooting performance. J Imagery Res Sport Phys Act. 2009, 4 (1): ArtID 7. Wakefield CJ, Smith D: Impact of differing frequencies of PETTLEP imagery on netball shooting performance. J Imagery Res Sport Phys Act. 2009, 4 (1): ArtID 7.
75.
Zurück zum Zitat White KD, Ashton R, Lewis S: Learning a complex skill: Effects of mental practice, physical practice, and imagery ability. Int J Sport Psychol. 1979, 10 (2): 71-78. White KD, Ashton R, Lewis S: Learning a complex skill: Effects of mental practice, physical practice, and imagery ability. Int J Sport Psychol. 1979, 10 (2): 71-78.
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Williams JG, Odley JL, Callaghan M: Motor imagery boosts proprioceptive neuromuscular facilitation in the attainment and retention of range of motion at the hip joint. J Sport Sci Med. 2004, 3 (3): 160-166. Williams JG, Odley JL, Callaghan M: Motor imagery boosts proprioceptive neuromuscular facilitation in the attainment and retention of range of motion at the hip joint. J Sport Sci Med. 2004, 3 (3): 160-166.
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Wright CJ, Smith D: The effect of PETTLEP imagery on strength performance. J Sport Exerc Psych. 2009, 18-31. Wright CJ, Smith D: The effect of PETTLEP imagery on strength performance. J Sport Exerc Psych. 2009, 18-31.
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Yaguez L, Nagel D, Hoffman H, Canavan AGM, Wist E, Homberg V: A mental route to motor learning: Improving trajectorial kinematics through imagery training. Behav Brain Res. 1998, 90 (1): 95-106. 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00087-9.PubMed Yaguez L, Nagel D, Hoffman H, Canavan AGM, Wist E, Homberg V: A mental route to motor learning: Improving trajectorial kinematics through imagery training. Behav Brain Res. 1998, 90 (1): 95-106. 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00087-9.PubMed
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Robin N, Dominique L, Toussaint L, Blandin Y, Guillot A, Le Her M: Effects of motor imagery training in service return accuracy in tennis: the role of imagery ability. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2007, 5 (2): 175-186. Robin N, Dominique L, Toussaint L, Blandin Y, Guillot A, Le Her M: Effects of motor imagery training in service return accuracy in tennis: the role of imagery ability. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2007, 5 (2): 175-186.
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith D, Wright C, Allsopp A, Westhead H: It's All in the Mind: PETTLEP-Based Imagery and Sports Performance. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2007, 19 (1): 80-92. 10.1080/10413200600944132. Smith D, Wright C, Allsopp A, Westhead H: It's All in the Mind: PETTLEP-Based Imagery and Sports Performance. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2007, 19 (1): 80-92. 10.1080/10413200600944132.
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith D, Wright CJ, Cantwell C: Beating the Bunker: The effect of PETTLEP imagery on golf bunker shot performance. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2008, 79 (3): 385-391.PubMed Smith D, Wright CJ, Cantwell C: Beating the Bunker: The effect of PETTLEP imagery on golf bunker shot performance. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2008, 79 (3): 385-391.PubMed
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith GG, Morey J, Tjoe E: Feature Masking in Computer Game Promotes Visual Imagery. Journal of Educational Computing Research. 2007, 36 (3): 351-372. 10.2190/U124-1761-7PU5-3666. Smith GG, Morey J, Tjoe E: Feature Masking in Computer Game Promotes Visual Imagery. Journal of Educational Computing Research. 2007, 36 (3): 351-372. 10.2190/U124-1761-7PU5-3666.
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Rodrigues EC, Lemos T, Gouvea B, Volchan E, Imbiriba LA, Vargas CD: Kinesthetic motor imagery modulates body sway. Neuroscience. 2010, 169 (2): 743-750. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.04.081.PubMed Rodrigues EC, Lemos T, Gouvea B, Volchan E, Imbiriba LA, Vargas CD: Kinesthetic motor imagery modulates body sway. Neuroscience. 2010, 169 (2): 743-750. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.04.081.PubMed
84.
Zurück zum Zitat Bovend'Eerdt TJH, Dawes H, Sackley C, Izadi H, Wade DT: Mental techniques during manual stretching in spasticity - a pilot randomized controlled trial. Clin Rehabil. 2009, 23 (2): 137-145. 10.1177/0269215508097298.PubMed Bovend'Eerdt TJH, Dawes H, Sackley C, Izadi H, Wade DT: Mental techniques during manual stretching in spasticity - a pilot randomized controlled trial. Clin Rehabil. 2009, 23 (2): 137-145. 10.1177/0269215508097298.PubMed
85.
Zurück zum Zitat Doheny MO: Effects of mental practice on performance of a psychomotor skill. J Ment Imagery. 1993, 17: 111-118. Doheny MO: Effects of mental practice on performance of a psychomotor skill. J Ment Imagery. 1993, 17: 111-118.
86.
Zurück zum Zitat Moseley GL, Zalucki N, Birklein F, Marinus J, van Hilten JJ, Luomajoki H: Thinking about movement hurts: The effect of motor imagery on pain and swelling in people with chronic arm pain. Arthrit rheumat. 2008, 59 (5): 623-631. 10.1002/art.23580. Moseley GL, Zalucki N, Birklein F, Marinus J, van Hilten JJ, Luomajoki H: Thinking about movement hurts: The effect of motor imagery on pain and swelling in people with chronic arm pain. Arthrit rheumat. 2008, 59 (5): 623-631. 10.1002/art.23580.
87.
Zurück zum Zitat Start KB: Intelligence and the improvement in a gross motor skill after mental practice. Brit J Educ Psychol. 1964, 34 (1): 85-90. 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1964.tb00608.x. Start KB: Intelligence and the improvement in a gross motor skill after mental practice. Brit J Educ Psychol. 1964, 34 (1): 85-90. 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1964.tb00608.x.
88.
Zurück zum Zitat Start KB, Richardson A: Imagery and mental practice. Brit J Educ Psychol. 1964, 34 (3): 280-284. 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1964.tb00638.x. Start KB, Richardson A: Imagery and mental practice. Brit J Educ Psychol. 1964, 34 (3): 280-284. 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1964.tb00638.x.
89.
Zurück zum Zitat Braun S, Beurskens AJ, Kleynen M, Oudelaar B, Schols JM, Wade DT: Effects of mental practice in stroke rehabilitation in Dutch nursing homes: results of a randomised controlled trial. 2010. Braun S, Beurskens AJ, Kleynen M, Oudelaar B, Schols JM, Wade DT: Effects of mental practice in stroke rehabilitation in Dutch nursing homes: results of a randomised controlled trial. 2010.
90.
Zurück zum Zitat Braun S, Beurskens AJ, Kleynen M, Schols JM, Wade DT: Effects of mental practice in patients with Parkinson's disease: results from a multi-centre randomised controlled trial. 2010. Braun S, Beurskens AJ, Kleynen M, Schols JM, Wade DT: Effects of mental practice in patients with Parkinson's disease: results from a multi-centre randomised controlled trial. 2010.
91.
Zurück zum Zitat Morin L, Latham GP: The effect of mental practice and goal setting as a transfer of training intervention on supervisors' self-efficacy and communication skills: An exploratory study. Appl Psychol Int Rev. 2000, 49 (3): 566-578. 10.1111/1464-0597.00032. Morin L, Latham GP: The effect of mental practice and goal setting as a transfer of training intervention on supervisors' self-efficacy and communication skills: An exploratory study. Appl Psychol Int Rev. 2000, 49 (3): 566-578. 10.1111/1464-0597.00032.
92.
Zurück zum Zitat Rodgers KC: Directed imagery versus free imagery for inducing changes in self-perspective and mood. Dis Abstr Int. 1982, 42 (7-B): 3001. Rodgers KC: Directed imagery versus free imagery for inducing changes in self-perspective and mood. Dis Abstr Int. 1982, 42 (7-B): 3001.
93.
Zurück zum Zitat Bucher L: The effects of imagery abilities and mental rehearsal on learning a nursing skill. J Nurs Edu. 1993, 32 (7): 318-324. Bucher L: The effects of imagery abilities and mental rehearsal on learning a nursing skill. J Nurs Edu. 1993, 32 (7): 318-324.
94.
Zurück zum Zitat Doheny MO: Effects of mental practice on psychomotor skills with baccalaureate nursing students. Dis Abstr Int. 1990, 51 (2-B): 659. Doheny MO: Effects of mental practice on psychomotor skills with baccalaureate nursing students. Dis Abstr Int. 1990, 51 (2-B): 659.
95.
Zurück zum Zitat Immenroth M, Eberspacher H, Nagelschmidt M, Troidl H, Burger T, Brenner J, Berg T, Muller M, Kemmler R: Mental training in surgery - Security by a better training: Design and first results of a study. MIC. 2005, 14 (1): 69-74. Immenroth M, Eberspacher H, Nagelschmidt M, Troidl H, Burger T, Brenner J, Berg T, Muller M, Kemmler R: Mental training in surgery - Security by a better training: Design and first results of a study. MIC. 2005, 14 (1): 69-74.
96.
Zurück zum Zitat Komesu Y, UrwitzLane R, Ozel B, Lukban J, Kahn M, Muir T, Fenner D, Rogers R: Does mental imagery prior to cystoscopy make a difference? A randomized controlled trial. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2009, 201 (2): 218.e211-218.e219. Komesu Y, UrwitzLane R, Ozel B, Lukban J, Kahn M, Muir T, Fenner D, Rogers R: Does mental imagery prior to cystoscopy make a difference? A randomized controlled trial. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2009, 201 (2): 218.e211-218.e219.
97.
Zurück zum Zitat Sanders CW, Sadoski M, Bramson R, Wiprud R, Van Walsum K: Comparing the effects of physical practice and mental imagery rehearsal on learning basic surgical skills by medical students. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2004, 191 (5): 1811-1814. 10.1016/j.ajog.2004.07.075.PubMed Sanders CW, Sadoski M, Bramson R, Wiprud R, Van Walsum K: Comparing the effects of physical practice and mental imagery rehearsal on learning basic surgical skills by medical students. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2004, 191 (5): 1811-1814. 10.1016/j.ajog.2004.07.075.PubMed
98.
Zurück zum Zitat Sanders CW, Sadoski M, van Walsum K, Bramson R, Wiprud R, Fossum TW: Learning basic surgical skills with mental imagery: using the simulation centre in the mind. Med Educ. 2008, 42 (6): 607-612. 10.1111/j.1365-2923.2007.02964.x.PubMed Sanders CW, Sadoski M, van Walsum K, Bramson R, Wiprud R, Fossum TW: Learning basic surgical skills with mental imagery: using the simulation centre in the mind. Med Educ. 2008, 42 (6): 607-612. 10.1111/j.1365-2923.2007.02964.x.PubMed
99.
Zurück zum Zitat Stig LC, Christensen HW, Byfield D, Sasnow M: Comparison of the effectiveness of physical practice and mental practice in the learning of chiropractic adjustive skills. Eur J Chiropractic. 1989, 37 (3): 70-76. Stig LC, Christensen HW, Byfield D, Sasnow M: Comparison of the effectiveness of physical practice and mental practice in the learning of chiropractic adjustive skills. Eur J Chiropractic. 1989, 37 (3): 70-76.
100.
Zurück zum Zitat Welk A, Immenroth M, Sakic P, Bernhardt O, Eberspächer H, Meyer G: Mental training in dentistry. Quintessence Int. 2007, 38 (6): 489-497.PubMed Welk A, Immenroth M, Sakic P, Bernhardt O, Eberspächer H, Meyer G: Mental training in dentistry. Quintessence Int. 2007, 38 (6): 489-497.PubMed
101.
Zurück zum Zitat Cramer SC, Orr ELR, Cohen MJ, Lacourse MG: Effects of motor imagery training after chronic, complete spinal cord injury. Exp Brain Res. 2007, 177 (2): 233-242. 10.1007/s00221-006-0662-9.PubMed Cramer SC, Orr ELR, Cohen MJ, Lacourse MG: Effects of motor imagery training after chronic, complete spinal cord injury. Exp Brain Res. 2007, 177 (2): 233-242. 10.1007/s00221-006-0662-9.PubMed
102.
Zurück zum Zitat Crosbie JH, McDonough SM, Gilmore DH, Wiggam MI: The adjunctive role of mental practice in the rehabilitation of the upper limb after hemiplegic stroke: a pilot study. Clin Rehabil. 2004, 18 (1): 60-68. 10.1191/0269215504cr702oa.PubMed Crosbie JH, McDonough SM, Gilmore DH, Wiggam MI: The adjunctive role of mental practice in the rehabilitation of the upper limb after hemiplegic stroke: a pilot study. Clin Rehabil. 2004, 18 (1): 60-68. 10.1191/0269215504cr702oa.PubMed
103.
Zurück zum Zitat Dijkerman HC, Ietswaart M, Johnston M, MacWalter RS: Does motor imagery training improve hand function in chronic stroke patients? A pilot study. Clin Rehabil. 2004, 18 (5): 538-549. 10.1191/0269215504cr769oa.PubMed Dijkerman HC, Ietswaart M, Johnston M, MacWalter RS: Does motor imagery training improve hand function in chronic stroke patients? A pilot study. Clin Rehabil. 2004, 18 (5): 538-549. 10.1191/0269215504cr769oa.PubMed
104.
Zurück zum Zitat Dunsky A, Dickstein R, Ariav C, Deutsch J, Marcovitz E: Motor imagery practice in gait rehabilitation of chronic post-stroke hemiparesis: four case studies. Int J Rehabil Res. 2006, 29 (4): 351-356. 10.1097/MRR.0b013e328010f559.PubMed Dunsky A, Dickstein R, Ariav C, Deutsch J, Marcovitz E: Motor imagery practice in gait rehabilitation of chronic post-stroke hemiparesis: four case studies. Int J Rehabil Res. 2006, 29 (4): 351-356. 10.1097/MRR.0b013e328010f559.PubMed
105.
Zurück zum Zitat Dunsky A, Dickstein R, Marcovitz E, Levy S, Deutsch J: Home-based motor imagery training for gait rehabilitation of people with chronic poststroke hemiparesis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2008, 89 (8): 1580-1588. 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.12.039.PubMed Dunsky A, Dickstein R, Marcovitz E, Levy S, Deutsch J: Home-based motor imagery training for gait rehabilitation of people with chronic poststroke hemiparesis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2008, 89 (8): 1580-1588. 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.12.039.PubMed
106.
Zurück zum Zitat Gustin SM, Wrigley PJ, Gandevia SC, Middleton JW, Henderson LA, Siddall PJ: Movement imagery increases pain in people with neuropathic pain following complete thoracic spinal cord injury. Pain. 2008, 137 (2): 237-244. 10.1016/j.pain.2007.08.032.PubMed Gustin SM, Wrigley PJ, Gandevia SC, Middleton JW, Henderson LA, Siddall PJ: Movement imagery increases pain in people with neuropathic pain following complete thoracic spinal cord injury. Pain. 2008, 137 (2): 237-244. 10.1016/j.pain.2007.08.032.PubMed
107.
Zurück zum Zitat Hewett TE, Ford KR, Levine P, Page SJ: Reaching kinematics to measure motor changes after mental practice in stroke. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2007, 14 (4): 23-29. 10.1310/tsr1404-23.PubMed Hewett TE, Ford KR, Levine P, Page SJ: Reaching kinematics to measure motor changes after mental practice in stroke. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2007, 14 (4): 23-29. 10.1310/tsr1404-23.PubMed
108.
Zurück zum Zitat Jackson PL, Doyon J, Richards CL, Malouin F: The efficacy of combined physical and mental practice in the learning of a foot-sequence task after stroke: a case report. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2004, 18 (2): 106-111. 10.1177/0888439004265249.PubMed Jackson PL, Doyon J, Richards CL, Malouin F: The efficacy of combined physical and mental practice in the learning of a foot-sequence task after stroke: a case report. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2004, 18 (2): 106-111. 10.1177/0888439004265249.PubMed
109.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu KP, Chan CC, Lee TM, HuiChan CW: Mental imagery for promoting relearning for people after stroke: A randomized controlled trial. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2004, 85 (9): 1403-1408. 10.1016/j.apmr.2003.12.035.PubMed Liu KP, Chan CC, Lee TM, HuiChan CW: Mental imagery for promoting relearning for people after stroke: A randomized controlled trial. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2004, 85 (9): 1403-1408. 10.1016/j.apmr.2003.12.035.PubMed
110.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu KP, Chan CC, Lee TM, Hui-Chan CW: Mental imagery for relearning of people after brain injury. Brain Inj. 2004, 18 (11): 1163-1172. 10.1080/02699050410001671883.PubMed Liu KP, Chan CC, Lee TM, Hui-Chan CW: Mental imagery for relearning of people after brain injury. Brain Inj. 2004, 18 (11): 1163-1172. 10.1080/02699050410001671883.PubMed
111.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu KPY, Chan CCH, Wong RSM, Kwan IWL, Yau CSF, Li LSW, Lee TMC: A randomized controlled trial of mental imagery augment generalization of learning in acute poststroke patients. Stroke. 2009, 40 (6): 2222-2225. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.540997.PubMed Liu KPY, Chan CCH, Wong RSM, Kwan IWL, Yau CSF, Li LSW, Lee TMC: A randomized controlled trial of mental imagery augment generalization of learning in acute poststroke patients. Stroke. 2009, 40 (6): 2222-2225. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.540997.PubMed
112.
Zurück zum Zitat McCarthy M, Beaumont JG, Thompson R, Pringle H: The role of imagery in the rehabilitation of neglect in severely disabled brain-injured adults. Arch Clin Neuropsych. 2002, 17 (5): 407-422. McCarthy M, Beaumont JG, Thompson R, Pringle H: The role of imagery in the rehabilitation of neglect in severely disabled brain-injured adults. Arch Clin Neuropsych. 2002, 17 (5): 407-422.
113.
Zurück zum Zitat Moseley GL: Graded motor imagery is effective for long-standing complex regional pain syndrome: a randomised controlled trial. Pain. 2004, 108 (1-2): 192-198. 10.1016/j.pain.2004.01.006.PubMed Moseley GL: Graded motor imagery is effective for long-standing complex regional pain syndrome: a randomised controlled trial. Pain. 2004, 108 (1-2): 192-198. 10.1016/j.pain.2004.01.006.PubMed
114.
Zurück zum Zitat Moseley GL: Is successful rehabilitation of complex regional pain syndrome due to sustained attention to the affected limb? A randomised clinical trial. PPA News. 2005, 114 (1-2): 54-61. Moseley GL: Is successful rehabilitation of complex regional pain syndrome due to sustained attention to the affected limb? A randomised clinical trial. PPA News. 2005, 114 (1-2): 54-61.
115.
Zurück zum Zitat Mueller K, Butefisch CM, Seitz RJ, Homberg V: Mental practice improves hand function after hemiparetic stroke. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2007, 25 (5-6): 501-511. Mueller K, Butefisch CM, Seitz RJ, Homberg V: Mental practice improves hand function after hemiparetic stroke. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2007, 25 (5-6): 501-511.
116.
Zurück zum Zitat Page SJ: Imagery improves upper extremity motor function in chronic stroke patients: a pilot study. Occup Ther J Res. 2000, 20 (3): 200-215. Page SJ: Imagery improves upper extremity motor function in chronic stroke patients: a pilot study. Occup Ther J Res. 2000, 20 (3): 200-215.
117.
Zurück zum Zitat Page SJ, Levine P, Leonard AC: Effects of mental practice on affected limb use and function in chronic stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2005, 86 (3): 399-402. 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.10.002.PubMed Page SJ, Levine P, Leonard AC: Effects of mental practice on affected limb use and function in chronic stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2005, 86 (3): 399-402. 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.10.002.PubMed
118.
Zurück zum Zitat Page SJ, Levine P, Hill V: Mental practice as a gateway to modified constraint-induced movement therapy: A promising combination to improve function. Am J Occup Ther. 2007, 61 (3): 321-327.PubMed Page SJ, Levine P, Hill V: Mental practice as a gateway to modified constraint-induced movement therapy: A promising combination to improve function. Am J Occup Ther. 2007, 61 (3): 321-327.PubMed
119.
Zurück zum Zitat Page SJ, Levine P, Leonard A: Mental practice in chronic stroke: Results of a randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Stroke. 2007, 38 (4): 1293-1297. 10.1161/01.STR.0000260205.67348.2b.PubMed Page SJ, Levine P, Leonard A: Mental practice in chronic stroke: Results of a randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Stroke. 2007, 38 (4): 1293-1297. 10.1161/01.STR.0000260205.67348.2b.PubMed
120.
Zurück zum Zitat Page SJ, Szaflarski JP, Eliassen JC, Pan H, Cramer SC: Cortical plasticity following motor skill learning during mental practice in stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2009, 23 (4): 382-388.PubMedPubMedCentral Page SJ, Szaflarski JP, Eliassen JC, Pan H, Cramer SC: Cortical plasticity following motor skill learning during mental practice in stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2009, 23 (4): 382-388.PubMedPubMedCentral
121.
Zurück zum Zitat Page SJ, Levine P, Khoury JC: Modified constraint-induced therapy combined with mental practice thinking through better motor outcomes. Stroke. 2009, 40 (2): 551-554. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.528760.PubMed Page SJ, Levine P, Khoury JC: Modified constraint-induced therapy combined with mental practice thinking through better motor outcomes. Stroke. 2009, 40 (2): 551-554. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.528760.PubMed
122.
Zurück zum Zitat Riccio I, Iolascon G, Barillari MR, Gimigliano R, Gimigliano F: Mental practice is effective in upper limb recovery after stroke: A randomized single-blind cross-over study. Eur J Rehabil Med. 2010, 46 (1): 19-25. Riccio I, Iolascon G, Barillari MR, Gimigliano R, Gimigliano F: Mental practice is effective in upper limb recovery after stroke: A randomized single-blind cross-over study. Eur J Rehabil Med. 2010, 46 (1): 19-25.
123.
Zurück zum Zitat Simmons L, Sharma N, Baron JC, Pomeroy VM: Motor imagery to enhance recovery after subcortical stroke: who might benefit, daily dose, and potential effects. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2008, 22 (5): 458-467. 10.1177/1545968308315597.PubMed Simmons L, Sharma N, Baron JC, Pomeroy VM: Motor imagery to enhance recovery after subcortical stroke: who might benefit, daily dose, and potential effects. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2008, 22 (5): 458-467. 10.1177/1545968308315597.PubMed
124.
Zurück zum Zitat Stevens JA, Stoykov ME: Using motor imagery in the rehabilitation of hemiparesis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2003, 84 (7): 1090-1092. 10.1016/S0003-9993(03)00042-X.PubMed Stevens JA, Stoykov ME: Using motor imagery in the rehabilitation of hemiparesis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2003, 84 (7): 1090-1092. 10.1016/S0003-9993(03)00042-X.PubMed
125.
Zurück zum Zitat Tamir R, Dickstein R, Huberman M: Integration of motor imagery and physical practice in group treatment applied to subjects with Parkinson's disease. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2007, 21 (1): 68-75. 10.1177/1545968306292608.PubMed Tamir R, Dickstein R, Huberman M: Integration of motor imagery and physical practice in group treatment applied to subjects with Parkinson's disease. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2007, 21 (1): 68-75. 10.1177/1545968306292608.PubMed
126.
Zurück zum Zitat Yoo E, Chung B: The effect of visual feedback plus mental practice on symmetrical weight-bearing training in people with hemiparesis. Clin Rehabil. 2006, 20 (5): 388-397. 10.1191/0269215506cr962oa. (334 ref) NLI: 8802181PubMed Yoo E, Chung B: The effect of visual feedback plus mental practice on symmetrical weight-bearing training in people with hemiparesis. Clin Rehabil. 2006, 20 (5): 388-397. 10.1191/0269215506cr962oa. (334 ref) NLI: 8802181PubMed
127.
Zurück zum Zitat Coffman DD: Effects of mental practice, physical practice, and knowledge of results on piano performance. J Res Music Educ. 1990, 38 (3): 187-196. 10.2307/3345182. Coffman DD: Effects of mental practice, physical practice, and knowledge of results on piano performance. J Res Music Educ. 1990, 38 (3): 187-196. 10.2307/3345182.
128.
Zurück zum Zitat Ross SL: The effectiveness of mental practice in improving the performance of college trombonists. J Res Music Educ. 1985, 33 (4): 221-230. 10.2307/3345249. Ross SL: The effectiveness of mental practice in improving the performance of college trombonists. J Res Music Educ. 1985, 33 (4): 221-230. 10.2307/3345249.
129.
Zurück zum Zitat Rubin Rabson G: Studies in the psychology of memorizing piano music. VI: A comparison of two forms of mental rehearsal and keyboard overlearning. J Educ Psychol. 1941, 32 (8): 593-602. Rubin Rabson G: Studies in the psychology of memorizing piano music. VI: A comparison of two forms of mental rehearsal and keyboard overlearning. J Educ Psychol. 1941, 32 (8): 593-602.
130.
Zurück zum Zitat Sonnenschein I: Mentales Training in der Instrumentenausbildung (MTI). Psychol Erz Unterr. 1990, 37 (3): 232-236. Sonnenschein I: Mentales Training in der Instrumentenausbildung (MTI). Psychol Erz Unterr. 1990, 37 (3): 232-236.
131.
Zurück zum Zitat Theiler AM, Lippman LG: Effects of mental practice and modeling on guitar and vocal performance. J Gen Psychol. 1995, 122 (4): 329-343. 10.1080/00221309.1995.9921245. Theiler AM, Lippman LG: Effects of mental practice and modeling on guitar and vocal performance. J Gen Psychol. 1995, 122 (4): 329-343. 10.1080/00221309.1995.9921245.
132.
Zurück zum Zitat Allami N, Paulignan Y, Brovelli A, Boussaoud D: Visuo-motor learning with combination of different rates of motor imagery and physical practice. Exp Brain Res. 2008, 184 (1): 105-113.PubMed Allami N, Paulignan Y, Brovelli A, Boussaoud D: Visuo-motor learning with combination of different rates of motor imagery and physical practice. Exp Brain Res. 2008, 184 (1): 105-113.PubMed
133.
Zurück zum Zitat Alves J, Belga P, Brito AP: Mental Training and Motor Learning in Volleyball. Prague: European Federation of Sport Psychology. 1999, 59-61. Alves J, Belga P, Brito AP: Mental Training and Motor Learning in Volleyball. Prague: European Federation of Sport Psychology. 1999, 59-61.
134.
Zurück zum Zitat Andre JC, Means JR: Rate of imagery in mental practice: An experimental investigation. J Sport Psychol. 1986, 8 (2): 124-128. Andre JC, Means JR: Rate of imagery in mental practice: An experimental investigation. J Sport Psychol. 1986, 8 (2): 124-128.
135.
Zurück zum Zitat Chevalier N, Boucher JP, Geoffrion R, Denis M: Visual and kinesthetic imagery in the mental rehearsal of a complex movement: an exploratory study. Sports science: 8 Commonwealth and international conference on sport, physical education, dance, recreation and health: 1986; Glasgow. 1986, 343-348. Chevalier N, Boucher JP, Geoffrion R, Denis M: Visual and kinesthetic imagery in the mental rehearsal of a complex movement: an exploratory study. Sports science: 8 Commonwealth and international conference on sport, physical education, dance, recreation and health: 1986; Glasgow. 1986, 343-348.
136.
Zurück zum Zitat Clark LV: Effect of mental practice on the development of a certain motor skill. Res Q Am Ass Health, Phys Ed Recr. 1960, 31: 560-569. Clark LV: Effect of mental practice on the development of a certain motor skill. Res Q Am Ass Health, Phys Ed Recr. 1960, 31: 560-569.
137.
Zurück zum Zitat Clegg BA, Wood JA, Bugg JM: Real and imagined movements in older and younger adults. J Ment Imagery. 2004, 28 (1-2): 1-16. Clegg BA, Wood JA, Bugg JM: Real and imagined movements in older and younger adults. J Ment Imagery. 2004, 28 (1-2): 1-16.
138.
Zurück zum Zitat Corbin CB: Effects of mental practice on the development of a specific motor skill. PhD thesis. 1966, Eugene, Ore.; United States: University of New Mexico Corbin CB: Effects of mental practice on the development of a specific motor skill. PhD thesis. 1966, Eugene, Ore.; United States: University of New Mexico
139.
Zurück zum Zitat Cornwall MW, Bruscato MP, Barry S: The effect of mental practice on isometric muscular strength. J Orthop Sport Phys. 1991, 13: 231-234. Cornwall MW, Bruscato MP, Barry S: The effect of mental practice on isometric muscular strength. J Orthop Sport Phys. 1991, 13: 231-234.
140.
Zurück zum Zitat Decety J: Motor information may be important for updating the cognitive processes involved in mental imagery of movement. Cur Psychol Cogn. 1991, 11 (4): 415-426. Decety J: Motor information may be important for updating the cognitive processes involved in mental imagery of movement. Cur Psychol Cogn. 1991, 11 (4): 415-426.
141.
Zurück zum Zitat Gassner K, Einsiedel T, Linke M, Grlich P, Mayer J: Does mental training improve learning to walk with a femoral prosthesis?. Orthopade. 2007, 36 (7): 673-678. 10.1007/s00132-007-1092-5.PubMed Gassner K, Einsiedel T, Linke M, Grlich P, Mayer J: Does mental training improve learning to walk with a femoral prosthesis?. Orthopade. 2007, 36 (7): 673-678. 10.1007/s00132-007-1092-5.PubMed
142.
Zurück zum Zitat Gordon S, Weinberg R, Jackson A: Effect of internal and external imagery on cricket performance. J Sport Behav. 1994, 17: 60-76. Gordon S, Weinberg R, Jackson A: Effect of internal and external imagery on cricket performance. J Sport Behav. 1994, 17: 60-76.
143.
Zurück zum Zitat Gray SW: Effect of visuomotor rehearsal with videotaped modeling on racquetball performance of beginning players. Percept Motor Skills. 1990, 70 (2): 379-385. 10.2466/PMS.70.2.379-385.PubMed Gray SW: Effect of visuomotor rehearsal with videotaped modeling on racquetball performance of beginning players. Percept Motor Skills. 1990, 70 (2): 379-385. 10.2466/PMS.70.2.379-385.PubMed
144.
Zurück zum Zitat Hellwing W, Knuff W: Mental preparation: a possibility for intensifying physical education. Sportunterricht. 1976, 25 (6): 180-187. Hellwing W, Knuff W: Mental preparation: a possibility for intensifying physical education. Sportunterricht. 1976, 25 (6): 180-187.
145.
Zurück zum Zitat Hemayattalab R, Movahedi A: Effects of different variations of mental and physical practice on sport skill learning in adolescents with mental retardation. Res Dev Disabil. 2009, 31 (1): 81-86.PubMed Hemayattalab R, Movahedi A: Effects of different variations of mental and physical practice on sport skill learning in adolescents with mental retardation. Res Dev Disabil. 2009, 31 (1): 81-86.PubMed
146.
Zurück zum Zitat Herrero JA, Garcia D, Soria E: Influence of imagery rehearsal on the maximum isometric force in bench press. Policlinico Sez Med. 2004, 111 (1): 11-20. Herrero JA, Garcia D, Soria E: Influence of imagery rehearsal on the maximum isometric force in bench press. Policlinico Sez Med. 2004, 111 (1): 11-20.
147.
Zurück zum Zitat Jaehme W, Steinhoefer D: Empirical study on the effectiveness of mental training in swimming with 10th and 11th grade high school students. Lehrhilf Sportunterr. 1978, 27 (5): 71-74. Jaehme W, Steinhoefer D: Empirical study on the effectiveness of mental training in swimming with 10th and 11th grade high school students. Lehrhilf Sportunterr. 1978, 27 (5): 71-74.
148.
Zurück zum Zitat Jones JG: Motor learning without demonstration of physical practice, under two conditions of mental practice. Res Quart. 1965, 36 (3): 270-276. Jones JG: Motor learning without demonstration of physical practice, under two conditions of mental practice. Res Quart. 1965, 36 (3): 270-276.
149.
Zurück zum Zitat Kelsey IB: Effects of mental practice and physical practice upon muscular endurance. Res Quat. 1961, 32: 47-54. Kelsey IB: Effects of mental practice and physical practice upon muscular endurance. Res Quat. 1961, 32: 47-54.
150.
Zurück zum Zitat Kohl RM, Roenker DL: Bilateral transfer as a function of mental imagery. J Motor Behav. 1980, 12 (3): 197-206. Kohl RM, Roenker DL: Bilateral transfer as a function of mental imagery. J Motor Behav. 1980, 12 (3): 197-206.
151.
Zurück zum Zitat Kornspan AS, Overby L, Lerner BS: Analysis and performance ofpre-performance imagery and other strategies on a golf putting task. J Ment Imagery. 2004, 28: 59-74. Kornspan AS, Overby L, Lerner BS: Analysis and performance ofpre-performance imagery and other strategies on a golf putting task. J Ment Imagery. 2004, 28: 59-74.
152.
Zurück zum Zitat Kremer P, Spittle M, McNeil D, Shinners C: Amount of mental practice and performance of a simple motor task. Percept Motor Skills. 2009, 109 (2): 347-356. 10.2466/pms.109.2.347-356.PubMed Kremer P, Spittle M, McNeil D, Shinners C: Amount of mental practice and performance of a simple motor task. Percept Motor Skills. 2009, 109 (2): 347-356. 10.2466/pms.109.2.347-356.PubMed
153.
Zurück zum Zitat Krigolson O, Van Gyn G, Tremblay L, Heath M: Is There "Feedback" During Visual Imagery? Evidence From a Specificity of Practice Paradigm. Can J Exp Psychol. 2006, 60 (1): 24-32.PubMed Krigolson O, Van Gyn G, Tremblay L, Heath M: Is There "Feedback" During Visual Imagery? Evidence From a Specificity of Practice Paradigm. Can J Exp Psychol. 2006, 60 (1): 24-32.PubMed
154.
Zurück zum Zitat Linden CA, Uhley JE, Smith D, Bush MA: The effects of mental practice on walking balance in an elderly population. Occup Ther J Res. 1989, 9 (3): 155-169. Linden CA, Uhley JE, Smith D, Bush MA: The effects of mental practice on walking balance in an elderly population. Occup Ther J Res. 1989, 9 (3): 155-169.
155.
Zurück zum Zitat Maring JR: Effects of mental practice on rate of skill acquisition. Phys Ther. 1990, 70 (3): 165-172.PubMed Maring JR: Effects of mental practice on rate of skill acquisition. Phys Ther. 1990, 70 (3): 165-172.PubMed
156.
Zurück zum Zitat McAleney PJ, Barabasz AF, Barabasz M: Effects of flotation restricted environmental stimulation on intercollegiate tennis performance. Percept Motor Skills. 1990, 71 (3): 1023-1028. McAleney PJ, Barabasz AF, Barabasz M: Effects of flotation restricted environmental stimulation on intercollegiate tennis performance. Percept Motor Skills. 1990, 71 (3): 1023-1028.
157.
Zurück zum Zitat Minas SC: Mental practice of a complex perceptual-motor skill. J Hum Movement Stud. 1978, 4 (2): 102-107. Minas SC: Mental practice of a complex perceptual-motor skill. J Hum Movement Stud. 1978, 4 (2): 102-107.
158.
Zurück zum Zitat Papaxanthis C, Pozzo T, Skoura X, Schieppati M: Does order and timing in performance of imagined and actual movements affect the motor imagery process? The duration of walking and writing task. Behav Brain Res. 2002, 134 (1-2): 209-215. 10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00030-X.PubMed Papaxanthis C, Pozzo T, Skoura X, Schieppati M: Does order and timing in performance of imagined and actual movements affect the motor imagery process? The duration of walking and writing task. Behav Brain Res. 2002, 134 (1-2): 209-215. 10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00030-X.PubMed
159.
Zurück zum Zitat Phipps SJ, Morehouse CA: Effects of mental practice on the acquisition of motor skills of varied difficulty. Res Q Exericse Sport. 1969, 40 (4): 773-778. Phipps SJ, Morehouse CA: Effects of mental practice on the acquisition of motor skills of varied difficulty. Res Q Exericse Sport. 1969, 40 (4): 773-778.
160.
Zurück zum Zitat Ranganathan VK, Siemionow V, Liu JZ, Sahgal V, Yue GH: From mental power to muscle power--gaining strength by using the mind. Neuropsychologia. 2004, 42 (7): 944-956. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.018.PubMed Ranganathan VK, Siemionow V, Liu JZ, Sahgal V, Yue GH: From mental power to muscle power--gaining strength by using the mind. Neuropsychologia. 2004, 42 (7): 944-956. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.018.PubMed
161.
Zurück zum Zitat Rawlings EI, Rawlings IL, Chen SS, Yilk MD: The facilitating effects of mental rehearsal in the acquisition of rotary pursuit tracking. Psychon Sci. 1972, 26 (2): 71-73. Rawlings EI, Rawlings IL, Chen SS, Yilk MD: The facilitating effects of mental rehearsal in the acquisition of rotary pursuit tracking. Psychon Sci. 1972, 26 (2): 71-73.
162.
Zurück zum Zitat Ryan ED, Simons J: Cognitive demand, imagery, and frequency of mental rehearsal as factors influencing acquisition of motor skills. J Sport Psychol. 1981, 3 (1): 35-45. Ryan ED, Simons J: Cognitive demand, imagery, and frequency of mental rehearsal as factors influencing acquisition of motor skills. J Sport Psychol. 1981, 3 (1): 35-45.
163.
Zurück zum Zitat Ryan ED, Simons J: Efficacy of mental imagery in enhancing mental rehearsal of motor skills. J Sport Psychol. 1982, 4 (1): 41-51. Ryan ED, Simons J: Efficacy of mental imagery in enhancing mental rehearsal of motor skills. J Sport Psychol. 1982, 4 (1): 41-51.
164.
Zurück zum Zitat Shackell EM, Standing LG: Mind Over Matter: Mental Training Increases Physical Strength. N Am J Psychol. 2007, 9 (1): 189-200. Shackell EM, Standing LG: Mind Over Matter: Mental Training Increases Physical Strength. N Am J Psychol. 2007, 9 (1): 189-200.
165.
Zurück zum Zitat Sidaway S, Trzaska A: Can mental practice increase ankle dorsiflexor torque?. Phys Ther. 2005, 85 (10): 1053-1060.PubMed Sidaway S, Trzaska A: Can mental practice increase ankle dorsiflexor torque?. Phys Ther. 2005, 85 (10): 1053-1060.PubMed
166.
Zurück zum Zitat Singer RN, Witker J: Mental rehearsal and point of introduction within the context of overt practice. Percept Motor Skills. 1970, 31 (1): 169-170. Singer RN, Witker J: Mental rehearsal and point of introduction within the context of overt practice. Percept Motor Skills. 1970, 31 (1): 169-170.
167.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith LE, Harrison JS: Comparison of the effects of visual, motor, mental, and guided practice upon speed and accuracy of performing a simple eye-hand coordination task. Res Q Am Assoc Health, Phys Ed Recr. 1962, 33 (2): 299-307. Smith LE, Harrison JS: Comparison of the effects of visual, motor, mental, and guided practice upon speed and accuracy of performing a simple eye-hand coordination task. Res Q Am Assoc Health, Phys Ed Recr. 1962, 33 (2): 299-307.
168.
Zurück zum Zitat Smyth MM: The role of mental practice in skill acquisition. J Motor Behav. 1975, 7 (3): 199-206. Smyth MM: The role of mental practice in skill acquisition. J Motor Behav. 1975, 7 (3): 199-206.
169.
Zurück zum Zitat Start KB: Relationship between intelligence and the effect of mental practice on the performance of a motor skill. Res Q Am Assoc Health, Phys Ed Recr. 1960, 31: 644-649. Start KB: Relationship between intelligence and the effect of mental practice on the performance of a motor skill. Res Q Am Assoc Health, Phys Ed Recr. 1960, 31: 644-649.
170.
Zurück zum Zitat Start KB: Kinaesthesis and mental practice. Res Q Am Assoc Health, Phys Ed Recr. 1964, 35 (3): 316-320. Start KB: Kinaesthesis and mental practice. Res Q Am Assoc Health, Phys Ed Recr. 1964, 35 (3): 316-320.
171.
Zurück zum Zitat Stebbins RJ: A comparison of the effects of physical and mental practice in learning a motor skill. Res Quart. 1968, 39 (3): 714-720. Stebbins RJ: A comparison of the effects of physical and mental practice in learning a motor skill. Res Quart. 1968, 39 (3): 714-720.
172.
Zurück zum Zitat Surburg PR: Aging and effect of physical-mental practice upon acquisition and retention of a motor skill. J Gerontol. 1976, 31 (1): 64-67.PubMed Surburg PR: Aging and effect of physical-mental practice upon acquisition and retention of a motor skill. J Gerontol. 1976, 31 (1): 64-67.PubMed
173.
Zurück zum Zitat Tunney N, Billings K, Blakely BG, Burch D, Jackson K: Mental practice and motor learning of a functional motor task in older adults: a pilot study. Phys Occup Ther Geriatr. 2006, 24 (3): 63-80. Tunney N, Billings K, Blakely BG, Burch D, Jackson K: Mental practice and motor learning of a functional motor task in older adults: a pilot study. Phys Occup Ther Geriatr. 2006, 24 (3): 63-80.
174.
Zurück zum Zitat Twining WE: Mental practice and physical practice in learning a motor skill. Res Q Am Assoc Health, Phys Ed Recr. 1949, 20: 432-435. Twining WE: Mental practice and physical practice in learning a motor skill. Res Q Am Assoc Health, Phys Ed Recr. 1949, 20: 432-435.
175.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Gyn G, Wenger HA, Gaul C: Imagery as a method of enhancing trenasfer from training to performance. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 1990, 366-375. 12 Van Gyn G, Wenger HA, Gaul C: Imagery as a method of enhancing trenasfer from training to performance. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 1990, 366-375. 12
176.
Zurück zum Zitat Vandell RA, Davis RA, Clugston HA: The function of mental practice in the acquisition of motor skills. J Gen Psychol. 1943, 29: 243-250. Vandell RA, Davis RA, Clugston HA: The function of mental practice in the acquisition of motor skills. J Gen Psychol. 1943, 29: 243-250.
177.
Zurück zum Zitat Whiteley G: Effect of mental rehearsal in association with physical practice in the acquisitions of simple gymnastic techniques. Research papers in physical education. 1966, 3 (Dec): 25-28. Whiteley G: Effect of mental rehearsal in association with physical practice in the acquisitions of simple gymnastic techniques. Research papers in physical education. 1966, 3 (Dec): 25-28.
178.
Zurück zum Zitat Wohldmann EL, Healy AF, Bourne LE: Pushing the Limits of Imagination: Mental Practice for Learning Sequences. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2007, 33 (1): 254-261.PubMed Wohldmann EL, Healy AF, Bourne LE: Pushing the Limits of Imagination: Mental Practice for Learning Sequences. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2007, 33 (1): 254-261.PubMed
179.
Zurück zum Zitat Woolfolk RL, Murphy SM, Gottesfeld D, Aitken D: Effects of mental rehearsal of task motor activity and mental depiction of task outcome on motor skill performance. J Sport Psychol. 1985, 7 (2): 191-197. Woolfolk RL, Murphy SM, Gottesfeld D, Aitken D: Effects of mental rehearsal of task motor activity and mental depiction of task outcome on motor skill performance. J Sport Psychol. 1985, 7 (2): 191-197.
180.
Zurück zum Zitat Woolfolk RL, Parrish MW, Murphy SM: The effects of positive and negative imagery on motor skill performance. Cognitive Ther Res. 1985, 9 (3): 335-341. 10.1007/BF01183852. Woolfolk RL, Parrish MW, Murphy SM: The effects of positive and negative imagery on motor skill performance. Cognitive Ther Res. 1985, 9 (3): 335-341. 10.1007/BF01183852.
181.
Zurück zum Zitat Zecker SG: Mental practice and knowledge of results in the learning of a perceptual motor skill. J Sport Psychol. 1982, 4 (1): 52-63. Zecker SG: Mental practice and knowledge of results in the learning of a perceptual motor skill. J Sport Psychol. 1982, 4 (1): 52-63.
182.
Zurück zum Zitat Casby A, Moran A: Exploring mental imagery in swimmers: A single-case study design. Irish J Psychol. 1998, 19 (4): 525-531. Casby A, Moran A: Exploring mental imagery in swimmers: A single-case study design. Irish J Psychol. 1998, 19 (4): 525-531.
183.
Zurück zum Zitat Guillot A, Nadrowska E, Collet C: Using Motor Imagery to Learn Tactical Movements in Basketball. J Sport Behav. 2009, 32 (2): 189. Guillot A, Nadrowska E, Collet C: Using Motor Imagery to Learn Tactical Movements in Basketball. J Sport Behav. 2009, 32 (2): 189.
184.
Zurück zum Zitat Olsson CJ, Jonsson B, Nyberg L: Internal imagery training in active high jumpers. Scand J Psychol. 2008, Apr 2008 Olsson CJ, Jonsson B, Nyberg L: Internal imagery training in active high jumpers. Scand J Psychol. 2008, Apr 2008
185.
Zurück zum Zitat Shambrook CJ, Bull SJ: The use of a single-case research design to investigate the efficacy of imagery training. J Appl Sport Psychol. 1996, 8 (1): 27-43. 10.1080/10413209608406306. Shambrook CJ, Bull SJ: The use of a single-case research design to investigate the efficacy of imagery training. J Appl Sport Psychol. 1996, 8 (1): 27-43. 10.1080/10413209608406306.
186.
Zurück zum Zitat Ziemainz H, Stoll O, Kuster C, Adler K: Evaluation of the mental training of the change from one event to the other in youth and junior triathlon. Leistungssport. 2003, 33 (2): 20-22. Ziemainz H, Stoll O, Kuster C, Adler K: Evaluation of the mental training of the change from one event to the other in youth and junior triathlon. Leistungssport. 2003, 33 (2): 20-22.
Metadaten
Titel
Best practice for motor imagery: a systematic literature review on motor imagery training elements in five different disciplines
verfasst von
Corina Schuster
Roger Hilfiker
Oliver Amft
Anne Scheidhauer
Brian Andrews
Jenny Butler
Udo Kischka
Thierry Ettlin
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2011
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Medicine / Ausgabe 1/2011
Elektronische ISSN: 1741-7015
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-9-75

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2011

BMC Medicine 1/2011 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Allgemeinmedizin

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Facharzt-Training Allgemeinmedizin

Die ideale Vorbereitung zur anstehenden Prüfung mit den ersten 24 von 100 klinischen Fallbeispielen verschiedener Themenfelder

Mehr erfahren

Niedriger diastolischer Blutdruck erhöht Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Komplikationen

25.04.2024 Hypotonie Nachrichten

Wenn unter einer medikamentösen Hochdrucktherapie der diastolische Blutdruck in den Keller geht, steigt das Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Ereignisse: Darauf deutet eine Sekundäranalyse der SPRINT-Studie hin.

Therapiestart mit Blutdrucksenkern erhöht Frakturrisiko

25.04.2024 Hypertonie Nachrichten

Beginnen ältere Männer im Pflegeheim eine Antihypertensiva-Therapie, dann ist die Frakturrate in den folgenden 30 Tagen mehr als verdoppelt. Besonders häufig stürzen Demenzkranke und Männer, die erstmals Blutdrucksenker nehmen. Dafür spricht eine Analyse unter US-Veteranen.

Metformin rückt in den Hintergrund

24.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Es hat sich über Jahrzehnte klinisch bewährt. Doch wo harte Endpunkte zählen, ist Metformin als alleinige Erstlinientherapie nicht mehr zeitgemäß.

Myokarditis nach Infekt – Richtig schwierig wird es bei Profisportlern

24.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Unerkannte Herzmuskelentzündungen infolge einer Virusinfektion führen immer wieder dazu, dass junge, gesunde Menschen plötzlich beim Sport einen Herzstillstand bekommen. Gerade milde Herzbeteiligungen sind oft schwer zu diagnostizieren – speziell bei Leistungssportlern. 

Update Allgemeinmedizin

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.