Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Neurology 1/2014

Open Access 01.12.2014 | Research article

Card-placing test in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and its neural correlates

verfasst von: Seong-Joon Lee, Young-Sil An, Tae Sung Lim, So Young Moon

Erschienen in: BMC Neurology | Ausgabe 1/2014

Abstract

Backgroud

We investigated anatomical correlates of the card-placing test (CPT) in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI).

Methods

Fifteen aMCI patients underwent part A and part B of the CPT and FDG-PET. The CPT scores and MMSE scores of 29 cognitively normal people were used for comparison. Statistical parametric mapping (SPM) correlation analysis was used to extract the regions whose changes in regional cerebral metabolism correlated significantly with part A and B of the CPT with adjustment of age, education and sex of patients.

Results

The aMCI patients had significantly lower MMSE scores (26.0 ± 2.0 vs. 28.2 ± 1.4, p < 0.001), CPT A (25.5 ± 3.5 vs. 27.7 ± 2.7, p = 0.026) and CPT B scores (16.3 ± 4.4 vs. 19.7 ± 3.7, p = 0.011) compared to the normal population. The test scores of part B of the CPT correlated well with hypometabolism of the posterior cingulate gyrus and precuneus.

Conclusions

This study suggests that the CPT B may reflect the functional status of the posterior cingulate gyrus in patients with aMCI.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1471-2377-14-123) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

There are no financial or non-financial competing interests on this study.

Authors’ contributions

SJL, YSA, TSL, and SYM carried out the study, analyzed the data and produced the draft. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Background

'Topographically disoriented' patients lose their ability to find their way within large-scale, locomotor environments. Heading disorientation (HD) is a type of pure topographical disorientation [1]. Patients with HD can represent the relative locations of objects and they are able to easily recognize landmarks, but suffer from topographical impairment not only in a novel but also in a familiar environment. It is implied that the primary deficit in HD is an inability to derive directional information from landmarks to reach a destination [13]. In other words, one that can be selectively damaged in HD is exocentric spatial representations, in which spatial relations between objects within the environment, including the observer are emphasized [4]. Although the number of reported cases has been limited so far, the lesion site responsible for HD is presumed to be the posterior cingulate gyrus, which is supposed to be the place where information necessary for navigation converges [1]. However, there had not been a specific test for HD until the card-placing test (CPT) was introduced [3].
The CPT examines the ability to represent spatial locations of objects placed on the floor around a subject. The CPT consists of two parts – part A and B. Part A of the test assesses the ability of a subject to retain information on spatial locations of cards placed on the floor around her/him. Part B examines the subject's ability to integrate information on the spatial locations of similarly arranged cards and that on changes of the body directions. Hashimoto et al. [3] reported that three patients with HD after damages in the right retrosplenial cortex showed good performance for part A but very poor one for part B. In the CPT, patients with HD were defective in the processing of directional signals of the self, or unable to integrate information on the registered external spatial locations of objects with that on their body direction. In addition, it was suggested that the CPT could be used to evaluate the functional state of right retrosplenial cortex. The retrosplenial region is a brain area and part of the cingulate cortex. It is defined by Brodmann area 26, Brodmann area 29 and Brodmann area 30.
The posterior cingulate gyrus, is of much interest because metabolic reduction in that area is seen in very early Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and its presence has also been proven to predict progression to AD in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) [57]. Considering that performance in the part B of the CPT was impaired in patients with right retrosplenial lesions, the CPT presumes to be the tests to evaluate the functional status of the posterior cingulate gyrus in patients with MCI. However, the CPT has not been used in patients with MCI. In addition, neural substrates of the test have not been replicated in other studies yet. In our study, using statistical parametric mapping (SPM) analysis we aimed to investigate anatomical correlates of the CPT in fluoro-deoxy-glucose positron emission tomography (FDG-PET) studies of patients with amnestic MCI (aMCI).

Methods

Participants

Patients were 15 individuals (eight men and seven women) with aMCI. Diagnostic evaluation of patients included a complete medical history, physical and neurologic examinations, neuropsychological tests, magnetic resonance image (MRI) scans, and blood tests which included complete blood counts, blood chemistry profiles, vitamin B12/folate levels, syphilis serology, and thyroid function tests. The aMCI patients were diagnosed according to the criteria proposed by Petersen et al. [8]. We excluded patients with a history of significant hearing or visual impairment that rendered interview participation difficult, as well as those with a history of neurological disorders (e.g., active epilepsy), psychiatric illnesses (e.g., schizophrenia, mental retardation, anxiety disorders, major depression, and mania), those taking psychotropic medications, and those with a history of significant alcohol and/or other substance abuse. In addition, twenty nine healthy volunteers (12 men and 17 women) participated. All subjects had corrected visual acuity of 20/40 or better, and no other known neurological or ophthalmological conditions. Controls were free of cognitive problems, which was confirmed by no cognitive complaints as well as by not scoring less than the 16th percentile of the norms for the age-, sex-, and education-matched normal subjects on the Korean version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [9]. We obtained informed consent from all the patients and controls, and the study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Ajou University Hospital. If participants had impaired decisional capacity, caregivers provided consent and patients provided assent.

Neuropsychological tests

All patients underwent neuropsychological tests using a standardized neuropsychological battery, called the Seoul Neuropsychological Screening Battery (SNSB) [10]. This battery contained tests for attention, language, praxis, the four symptoms of Gerstmann syndrome, visuoconstructive function, verbal and visual memory, and frontal/executive function. Memory function was evaluated by the delayed recall on the Seoul Verbal Learning Test (SVLT) or Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (RCFT). Language was assessed by the Korean version of the Boston Naming Test (K-BNT). Visuospatial function was evaluated by the copying score of the RCFT. Finally, frontal/executive tests were classified into three groups: motor executive function (contrasting program, go/no-go, fist-edge-palm, alternating hand movement, alternative square and triangle, and Luria loop), Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT), and Stroop Test. Impaired frontal/executive function was operationally defined as impairment in at least two of the three groups. Among neuropsychological tests, scorable tests included the digit span test (forward and backward), the K-BNT, the RCFT (copying, immediate and 20-minute delayed recall, and recognition), the SVLT (three free recall trials of 12 words, and a 20-minute delayed recall trial of the same 12 items, and a recognition test), the phonemic and semantic COWAT, and the Stroop Test (word and color reading of 112 items for two minutes). The scores on cognitive tests were classified as abnormal when they were below the 16th percentile of the norms for the age-, sex-, and education-matched normal subjects. On the basis of the results of the SNSB, aMCI patients were diagnosed with amnestic MCI-single domain or amnestic MCI-multiple domains.

CPT

Participants underwent the CPT as previously described [3] in a featureless room. In part A of the CPT, a subject stands in the center square of nine squares arranged in three rows of three. The subject was instructed to remember the spatial locations of three different cards randomly placed in one of the eight squares. After 10 s, the cards were taken away and the subject is to restore them to their original positions. In part B, immediately after the cards had been removed, the subject was rotated to the right or to the left by 90 or 180°, and then asked to replace the cards. For both part A and part B of the CPT, the subject underwent 10 consecutive trials, and scored 1 point if the location of a card that the subject replaced was correct. The full score of each of part A and part B of the CPT was 30 points.

FDG PET studies

Fifteen patients with aMCI received FDG-PET. PET/CT data were acquired on a Discovery ST scanner (General Electric Medical Systems, USA). After fasting for at least 4 h, patients received 300 MBq of FDG intravenously. We checked serum glucose-levels in all subjects prior to FDG injection, and the subjects whose glucose-level exceeds 150 mg/dl were excluded. All subjects were instructed to rest comfortably for 30 min with their eyes closed and ears unplugged and then image acquisition was started. To reduce head movement during scanning, the patients were positioned and maintained using an individually molded head holder. They first had a CT scan (tube-rotation time of 1 s per revolution, 120 kV, 70 mA, 5.0 mm per rotation and an acquisition time of 11.8 s for a scan length of 150.42 mm). Subsequently, one frame (8 min per frame) of emission PET data was acquired in a three-dimensional mode. PET images were reconstructed by iterative reconstruction (ordered subsets expectation maximization, with one iteration and 32 subsets), using the CT images for attenuation correction. Also, the random correction by singles and model-based scatter correction were applied.

FDG PET data analysis

FDG-PET images were spatially normalized to a standard template provided by SPM2 (Statistical Parametric Mapping 2, Institute of Neurology, University of London, UK) on MATLAB (version 7.1; Mathworks, Natick, MA). A local optimization of the 12 parameters of an affine transformation was applied to spatial normalization. These images were then smoothed with a Gaussian kernel (full-width at half-maximum = 16 mm) to minimize noise and improve between-subject spatial alignment. Appropriate voxel-by-voxel statistical tests were used to evaluate differences in glucose metabolism. A correlation analysis was performed to extract regions whose changes in regional cerebral metabolism correlated significantly with part A and part B of the CPT as well as scorable neuropsychological tests and MMSE, controlling for age, education and sex. Anatomical labeling of significant clusters was performed using automated anatomical labeling SPM toolbox, which was based on anatomy provided by the Montreal Neurological Institute.

Statistical analyses

We used chi square and the student’s t-test to compare demographic data and test scores between patients and normal population. A p-values <0.05 were deemed significant. The statistical analysis was performed using commercially available software (SPSS, version 18.0). In the SPM analyses, regions reaching uncorrected threshold of p < 0.005, were considered to be significant due to the small number of the patient population.

Results

aMCI patients versus controls

The demographics of the patients and the normal population are outlined in the Table 1. There was no statistically significant difference in their age, sex and education between aMCI and normal controls (p > 0.05). The aMCI patients had significantly lower MMSE scores (26.0 ± 2.0 vs. 28.2 ± 1.4, p < 0.001), CPT A (25.5 ± 3.5 vs. 27.7 ± 2.7, p = 0.026) and CPT B scores (16.3 ± 4.4 vs. 19.7 ± 3.7, p = 0.011) compared to the normal population. The aMCI patients comprised of 10 (66.7%) single domain aMCI, and five (33.3%) multiple domain aMCI patients. Multiple domain aMCI patients were older (years old, 67.0 ± 8.1 vs 65.1 ± 4.3, p = 0.559), more highly-educated (years, 10.0 ± 3.8 vs 8.8 ± 5.2, p = 0.672), but more impaired in both CPT A (23.6 ± 3.7 vs. 26.4 ± 3.0, p = 0.148) and CPT B (14.4 ± 5.2 vs. 17.3 ± 3.8, p = 0.241) than single domain aMCI patients, although their difference was not statistically significant.
Table 1
Demographic findings and test results of normal population and aMCI patients
 
aMCI patients (n = 15)
Normal population (n = 29)
p-value
Age, years
65.3 ± 5.5
68.7 ± 5.5
0.066
Sex, male (%)
8 (53.3%)
12 (41.4%)
0.450
Education, years
9.2 ± 4.7
11.7 ± 4.7
0.111
MMSE
26.0 ± 2.0
28.2 ± 1.4
<0.001
Part A, CPT (30)
25.5 ± 3.5
27.7 ± 2.7
0.026
Part B, CPT (30)
16.3 ± 4.4
19.7 ± 3.7
0.011
Attention
   
Digit span forward test (9)
5.8 ± 1.4
  
Working memory
   
Digit span backward test (8)
3.4 ± 0.8
  
Language function
   
K-BNT (60)
43.2 ± 8.9
  
Visuospatial function
   
RCFT Copy (36)
31.3 ± 6.2
  
Memory function
   
SVLT immediate recall (36)
13.4 ± 3.4
  
SVLT delayed recall (12)
2.6 ± 1.4
  
SVLT recognition (24)
17.2 ± 1.7
  
RCFT immediate recall (36)
9.5 ± 5.6
  
RCFT delayed recall (36)
8.8 ± 5.1
  
RCFT recognition (24)
18.4 ± 2.7
  
Frontal function
   
Contrasting program (20)
19.2 ± 2.5
  
Go-no go (20)
19.6 ± 1.2
  
Phonemic COWAT
22.2 ± 6.9
  
aMCI, amnestic mild cognitive impairment; Mini-Mental State Examination; CPT, card-placing test; K-BNT, Korean version of Boston Naming Test; RCFT, Rey Complex Figure Test; SVLT, Seoul Verbal Learning Test; COWAT, Controlled Oral Word Association Test.
Data for all variables except for sex: mean ± SD.
The full mark for each test is presented in the brackets.

SPM correlation analysis

Table 2 presents the results of the correlation analysis to evaluate the regions whose changes in regional cerebral metabolism correlated significantly with part B of the CPT. There was no regions whose hypometablism showed correlation with scores of part A. Decrease in scores of part B correlated with hypometabolism in bilateral precuneus, middle and posterior cingulate gyri (Figure 1, uncorrected p < 0.005). Table 3 reveals the results of the correlation analysis to evaluate the regions whose changes in regional cerebral metabolism correlated significantly with scorable neuropsychological tests and MMSE. Any tests except for part B of the CPT did not show correlation with hypometabolism of posterior cingulate gyrus.
Table 2
SPM correlation analysis between part B scores of the card-placing test and regional hypometabolism
Voxel level
Voxel level
Coordinate
Side
Area
P (uncorrected)
P (FDR-corr)
X
Y
Z
  
0.003
0.972
14
-50
28
Right
Precuneus
      
Middle cingulate
      
Posterior cingulate
0.003
0.972
0
-44
16
Right
Posterior cingulate
      
Precuneus
     
Left
Posterior cingulate
      
Precuneus
FDR-corr, false discovery rate-corrected.
Table 3
SPM correlation analysis between neuropsychological tests and regional hypometabolism
Test
Voxel level
Voxel level
Coordinate
Side
Area
 
P (uncorrected)
P (FDR-corr)
X
Y
Z
  
Digit span forward
0.001
1.000
46
-46
-40
Right
Cerebellum
0.001
1.000
-34
6
-50
Left
Middle temporal
Digit span backward
0.001
0.833
24
-34
-28
Right
Cerebellum
0.002
0.833
18
-40
-18
Right
Cerebellum
K-BNT
<0.001
0.071
2
38
-4
Right
Anterior cingulate
<0.001
0.074
8
38
16
Right
Anterior cingulate
<0.001
0.098
-4
38
24
Left
Anterior cingulate
<0.001
0.071
-38
-6
-50
Left
Inferior temporal
 
0.002
0.254
-30
-12
-36
Left
Fusiform
 
0.003
0.298
-30
14
-44
Left
Middle temporal
RCFT
0.001
0.158
36
-10
-42
Right
Fusiform
0.001
0.222
24
-26
-26
Right
Parahippocampal
SVLT immediate recall
0.003
0.999
70
-30
42
Right
Supramarginal
0.005
0.999
66
-30
50
Right
Supramarginal
0.001
1.000
30
60
-16
Right
Orbitofrontal
SVLT delayed recall
0.001
1.000
-56
8
38
Left
Precentral
0.001
1.000
-48
28
30
Left
Inferior frontal
SLVT recognition
<0.001
0.816
24
36
58
Right
Superior frontal
RCFT immediate recall
0.001
0.929
-8
42
2
Left
Anterior cingulate
0.002
0.929
-8
26
36
Left
Superior frontal
RCFT delayed recall
0.003
0.929
-8
34
26
Left
Anterior cingulate
0.001
0.880
-8
40
0
Left
Anterior cingulate
0.003
0.880
8
34
-2
Right
Anterior cingulate
RCFT recognition
<0.001
1.000
-54
22
32
Left
Inferior frontal
0.002
1.000
6
56
32
Right
Superior frontal
Contrasting program
0.002
1.000
8
42
46
Right
Superior frontal
<0.001
0.061
-4
30
38
Left
Superior frontal
<0.001
0.061
-4
40
50
Left
Superior frontal
<0.001
0.061
-2
24
62
Left
Supplementary motor
<0.001
0.062
-4
-56
4
Left
Calcarine
<0.001
0.070
62
18
24
Right
Inferior frontal
<0.001
0.070
-56
24
22
Left
Inferior frontal
0.001
0.120
-44
-62
-24
Left
Cerebellum
Go-no go
<0.001
0.438
-46
-14
62
Left
Precentral
<0.001
0.438
22
42
48
Right
Superior frontal
0.001
0.438
32
32
54
Right
Middle frontal
<0.001
0.438
74
-24
14
Right
Superior temporal
0.003
0.438
58
-22
22
Right
Supramarginal
0.001
0.438
30
-10
72
Right
Superior frontal
0.002
0.438
18
14
72
Right
Supplementary motor
0.001
0.438
74
-40
-10
Right
Middle temporal
0.001
0.438
60
6
8
Right
Inferior frontal
0.002
0.438
58
4
22
Right
Precentral
Phonemic
0.001
0.878
48
-48
-46
Right
Cerebellum
COWAT
0.001
0.878
28
12
-42
Right
Middle temporal
0.001
0.878
30
-8
-48
Right
Fusiform
0.001
0.878
2
8
42
Right
Middle cingulate
MMSE
0.001
0.994
28
66
10
Right
Superior frontal
FDR-corr, false discovery rate-corrected; K-BNT, Korean version of Boston Naming Test; RCFT, Rey Complex Figure Test; SVLT, Seoul Verbal Learning Test; COWAT, Controlled Oral Word Association Test.

Discussion

The results of our study showed that patients with aMCI compared to normal subjects had lower performances in both CPT A and CPT B. The SPM correlation analyses revealed that decrease in scores of part B correlated with hypometabolism in bilateral precuneus, middle and posterior cingulate gyri.
To accomplish part A of the test, the subjects should use an egocentric (or body-centered) reference frame to represent the spatial locations of objects surrounding them. Therefore, the part A seemed to evaluate their egocentric orientation. The only difference between part A and part B is the subjects’ rotation in part B just before they replace the cards. To accomplish part B of the test, the subjects should use the information on changes in their body direction. In other words, they should process directional signals of the self, or integrate information on the registered external spatial locations of objects with that on their body direction. Therefore, the part B evaluated their exocentric orientation. As a result, in our study, patients with aMCI were impaired in both egocentric and exocentric orientation. Other previous study also evaluated egocentric orientation and exocentric orientation in MCI [11]. In the study, a four-subtest task was used that required the subjects to locate an invisible goal inside a circular arena. The patients with multiple domain aMCI were impaired on all subtests, while the single domain aMCI group was impaired significantly on tests that focused on exocentric orientation and at the beginning of the real space egocentric subtest [11]. Their deficits in egocentric and exocentric orientation may cause topographical disorientation affecting their activities of daily living. A previous report showed that 17 out of the 41 MCI patients (41.4%) experienced topographical disorientation [12]. Another previous study compared patients with MCI with matched controls on a route learning task, and concluded that while MCI patients recognized landmarks as effectively as controls, they could not find their locations on maps or recall the order in which they were encountered [13].
Our SPM correlational analysis showed that the scores of CPT B correlated with hypometabolism of the posterior cingulate region (p < 0.005). We made two possible explanations for our result. First, it is assumed that the part B of the CPT is the specific test for HD and the posterior cingulate gyrus is the neural correlate of the CPT B. Our results agreed with the results of the case studies which originally mentioned the CPT [3]. In rodents, head direction cells (h cells) that are excited when rats are maintaining a certain heading or orientation within an environment are found in the retrosplenial cortex in addition to several neural structures, such as the anterior dorsal nucleus of the thalamus, lateral dorsal thalamus, lateral mammillary nuclei, striatum, and posterior subiculum [14, 15]. These neural substrates may constitute a functional circuit dealing with directional signals of the self [15]. Second, it seems that the CPT B is reflective of levels of general cognitive deficit in patients and hypometabolism in the posterior cingulate gyrus is correlated with levels of their cognition. A previous study compared regional cortical thickness between single domain aMCI and multiple domain aMCI [16]. The study suggested that multiple domain aMCI is a transitional state between single domain aMCI and AD, and that the cortical thinning is evidence that the precuneus is responsible for the multiple cognitive impairments in multiple domain aMCI. Although both possible explanations for our result suggest that the CPT B may reflect the functional status of the posterior cingulate gyrus, we performed further correlational analyses with MMSE and neuropsychological tests covering each cognitive domain and metabolism on FDG-PET to distinguish two hypotheses. We found only correlation between posterior cingulate gyrus and the CPT B as shown in the Table 2 and Table 3. Therefore, our result may be supportive for the first hypothesis which the CPT B is the specific test of heading disorientation dependent on the posterior cingulate gyrus.
We found associations between the CPT B and bilateral posterior cingulate gyrus and precuneus, but the original report from Hashimoto et al. [3] found impairment of the CPT B in patients with only right posterior cingulate gyrus/precuneus lesions indicating that the right retrosplenial region is the critical site for heading disorientation. In addition, several studies reported that damage to the left retrosplenial region is not associated with heading disorientation but with episodic memory deficits [17]. In our study, any memory-related tests did not show correlation with metabolism in the posterior cingulate gyrus. We think that the study needs to be done to evaluate whether the CPT would be also impaired in patients with damage to the left retrosplenial region.
We recognize that there are some limitations to our study. First, we recruited the controls who were free of cognitive problems, which was confirmed by no cognitive complaints as well as by not scoring less than the 16th percentile of the norms for the age-, sex-, and education-matched normal subjects on the Korean version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). However, we should admit that we could not exclude the possibilities that the controls might have cognitive impairment which could be detected only by detailed neuropsychological tests. Second, our study recruited 10 (66.7%) single domain aMCI and five (33.3%) multiple domain aMCI patients. Considering the result from the previous MRI morphometric study that the precuneus is responsible for the multiple cognitive impairments in multiple domain aMCI patients, we should have evaluated only patients with multiple domain aMCI patients. Including also patients with single domain amnestic MCI might lead to non-significant results due to type 2 error. Third, because of the limited number of the patients, we used uncorrected p-values for the correlational analysis between the PET images and CPT scores. For this reason, these findings cannot be generalized to the broader community based on this study alone and studies with larger samples are necessary.

Conclusions

This study suggests that the CPT B may reflect the functional status of the posterior cingulate gyrus in patients with aMCI.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by grants from the Korea Health 21 R&D Project, Ministry of Health, Welfare, and Family Affairs, Republic of Korea (HI10C2020).
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made.
The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.
The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (https://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Competing interests

There are no financial or non-financial competing interests on this study.

Authors’ contributions

SJL, YSA, TSL, and SYM carried out the study, analyzed the data and produced the draft. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Anhänge

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Aguirre GK, D'Esposito M: Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. Brain. 1999, 122 (Pt 9): 1613-1628.CrossRefPubMed Aguirre GK, D'Esposito M: Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. Brain. 1999, 122 (Pt 9): 1613-1628.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Takahashi N, Kawamura M, Shiota J, Kasahata N, Hirayama K: Pure topographic disorientation due to right retrosplenial lesion. Neurology. 1997, 49: 464-469. 10.1212/WNL.49.2.464.CrossRefPubMed Takahashi N, Kawamura M, Shiota J, Kasahata N, Hirayama K: Pure topographic disorientation due to right retrosplenial lesion. Neurology. 1997, 49: 464-469. 10.1212/WNL.49.2.464.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Hashimoto R, Tanaka Y, Nakano I: Heading disorientation: a new test and a possible underlying mechanism. Eur Neurol. 2010, 63: 87-93. 10.1159/000276398.CrossRefPubMed Hashimoto R, Tanaka Y, Nakano I: Heading disorientation: a new test and a possible underlying mechanism. Eur Neurol. 2010, 63: 87-93. 10.1159/000276398.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor H, Tversky B: Spatial mental models derived from survey and route descriptions. J Mem Lang. 1992, 31: 261-282. 10.1016/0749-596X(92)90014-O.CrossRef Taylor H, Tversky B: Spatial mental models derived from survey and route descriptions. J Mem Lang. 1992, 31: 261-282. 10.1016/0749-596X(92)90014-O.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Huang C, Wahlund LO, Svensson L, Winblad B, Julin P: Cingulate cortex hypoperfusion predicts Alzheimer's disease in mild cognitive impairment. BMC Neurol. 2002, 2: 9-10.1186/1471-2377-2-9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Huang C, Wahlund LO, Svensson L, Winblad B, Julin P: Cingulate cortex hypoperfusion predicts Alzheimer's disease in mild cognitive impairment. BMC Neurol. 2002, 2: 9-10.1186/1471-2377-2-9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Minoshima S, Giordani B, Berent S, Frey KA, Foster NL, Kuhl DE: Metabolic reduction in the posterior cingulate cortex in very early Alzheimer's disease. Ann Neurol. 1997, 42: 85-94. 10.1002/ana.410420114.CrossRefPubMed Minoshima S, Giordani B, Berent S, Frey KA, Foster NL, Kuhl DE: Metabolic reduction in the posterior cingulate cortex in very early Alzheimer's disease. Ann Neurol. 1997, 42: 85-94. 10.1002/ana.410420114.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Whitwell JL, Przybelski SA, Weigand SD, Knopman DS, Boeve BF, Petersen RC, Jack CR: 3D maps from multiple MRI illustrate changing atrophy patterns as subjects progress from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer's disease. Brain. 2007, 130 (Pt 7): 1777-1786.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Whitwell JL, Przybelski SA, Weigand SD, Knopman DS, Boeve BF, Petersen RC, Jack CR: 3D maps from multiple MRI illustrate changing atrophy patterns as subjects progress from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer's disease. Brain. 2007, 130 (Pt 7): 1777-1786.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Petersen RC: Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004, 256: 183-194. 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x.CrossRefPubMed Petersen RC: Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004, 256: 183-194. 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Oh E, Kang Y, Shin JH, Yeon BK: A validity study of K-MMSE as a screening test for dementia: Comparison against a comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation. Dementia Neurocognitive Disord. 2010, 9: 8-12. Oh E, Kang Y, Shin JH, Yeon BK: A validity study of K-MMSE as a screening test for dementia: Comparison against a comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation. Dementia Neurocognitive Disord. 2010, 9: 8-12.
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Ahn HJ, Chin J, Park A, Lee BH, Suh MK, Seo SW, Na DL: Seoul neuropsychological screening battery-dementia version (SNSB-D): A useful tool for assessing and monitoring cognitive impairments in dementia patients. J Korean Med Sci. 2010, 25: 1071-1076. 10.3346/jkms.2010.25.7.1071.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ahn HJ, Chin J, Park A, Lee BH, Suh MK, Seo SW, Na DL: Seoul neuropsychological screening battery-dementia version (SNSB-D): A useful tool for assessing and monitoring cognitive impairments in dementia patients. J Korean Med Sci. 2010, 25: 1071-1076. 10.3346/jkms.2010.25.7.1071.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Hort J, Laczó J, Vyhnálek M, Bojar M, Bures J, Vlcek K: Spatial navigation deficit in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007, 104: 4042-4047. 10.1073/pnas.0611314104.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hort J, Laczó J, Vyhnálek M, Bojar M, Bures J, Vlcek K: Spatial navigation deficit in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007, 104: 4042-4047. 10.1073/pnas.0611314104.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Lim TS, Iaria G, Moon SY: Topographical disorientation in mild cognitive impairment: a voxel-based morphometry study. J Clin Neurol. 2010, 6: 204-211. 10.3988/jcn.2010.6.4.204.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lim TS, Iaria G, Moon SY: Topographical disorientation in mild cognitive impairment: a voxel-based morphometry study. J Clin Neurol. 2010, 6: 204-211. 10.3988/jcn.2010.6.4.204.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
13.
Zurück zum Zitat DeIpolyi AR, Rankin KP, Mucke L, Miller BL, Gorno-Tempini ML: Spatial cognition and the human navigation network in AD and MCI. Neurology. 2007, 69: 986-997. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000271376.19515.c6.CrossRefPubMed DeIpolyi AR, Rankin KP, Mucke L, Miller BL, Gorno-Tempini ML: Spatial cognition and the human navigation network in AD and MCI. Neurology. 2007, 69: 986-997. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000271376.19515.c6.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen LL, Lin LH, Green EJ, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL: Head-direction cells in the rat posterior cortex. I. Anatomical distribution and behavioral modulation. Exp Brain Res. 1994, 101: 8-23. 10.1007/BF00243212.CrossRefPubMed Chen LL, Lin LH, Green EJ, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL: Head-direction cells in the rat posterior cortex. I. Anatomical distribution and behavioral modulation. Exp Brain Res. 1994, 101: 8-23. 10.1007/BF00243212.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Taube JS: Head direction cells and the neurophysiological basis for a sense of direction. Prog Neurobiol. 1998, 55: 225-256. 10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00004-5.CrossRefPubMed Taube JS: Head direction cells and the neurophysiological basis for a sense of direction. Prog Neurobiol. 1998, 55: 225-256. 10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00004-5.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Seo SW, Im K, Lee JM, Kim YH, Kim ST, Kim SY, Yang DW, Kim SI, Cho YS, Na DL: Cortical thickness in single- versus multiple-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuroimage. 2007, 36: 289-297. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.042.CrossRefPubMed Seo SW, Im K, Lee JM, Kim YH, Kim ST, Kim SY, Yang DW, Kim SI, Cho YS, Na DL: Cortical thickness in single- versus multiple-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuroimage. 2007, 36: 289-297. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.042.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Gainotti G, Almonti S, Di Betta AM, Silverci MC: Retrograde amnesia in a patient with retrosplenial tumour. Neurocase. 1998, 4: 519-526. 10.1080/13554799808410644.CrossRef Gainotti G, Almonti S, Di Betta AM, Silverci MC: Retrograde amnesia in a patient with retrosplenial tumour. Neurocase. 1998, 4: 519-526. 10.1080/13554799808410644.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Card-placing test in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and its neural correlates
verfasst von
Seong-Joon Lee
Young-Sil An
Tae Sung Lim
So Young Moon
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2014
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Neurology / Ausgabe 1/2014
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2377
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2377-14-123

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2014

BMC Neurology 1/2014 Zur Ausgabe

Neu in den Fachgebieten Neurologie und Psychiatrie

Akuter Schwindel: Wann lohnt sich eine MRT?

28.04.2024 Schwindel Nachrichten

Akuter Schwindel stellt oft eine diagnostische Herausforderung dar. Wie nützlich dabei eine MRT ist, hat eine Studie aus Finnland untersucht. Immerhin einer von sechs Patienten wurde mit akutem ischämischem Schlaganfall diagnostiziert.

Niedriger diastolischer Blutdruck erhöht Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Komplikationen

25.04.2024 Hypotonie Nachrichten

Wenn unter einer medikamentösen Hochdrucktherapie der diastolische Blutdruck in den Keller geht, steigt das Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Ereignisse: Darauf deutet eine Sekundäranalyse der SPRINT-Studie hin.

Frühe Alzheimertherapie lohnt sich

25.04.2024 AAN-Jahrestagung 2024 Nachrichten

Ist die Tau-Last noch gering, scheint der Vorteil von Lecanemab besonders groß zu sein. Und beginnen Erkrankte verzögert mit der Behandlung, erreichen sie nicht mehr die kognitive Leistung wie bei einem früheren Start. Darauf deuten neue Analysen der Phase-3-Studie Clarity AD.

Viel Bewegung in der Parkinsonforschung

25.04.2024 Parkinson-Krankheit Nachrichten

Neue arznei- und zellbasierte Ansätze, Frühdiagnose mit Bewegungssensoren, Rückenmarkstimulation gegen Gehblockaden – in der Parkinsonforschung tut sich einiges. Auf dem Deutschen Parkinsonkongress ging es auch viel um technische Innovationen.