Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Neurology 1/2011

Open Access 01.12.2011 | Case control study

Hypervigilance or avoidance of trigger related cues in migraineurs? - A case-control study using the emotional stroop task

verfasst von: Anne-Katrin Puschmann, Claudia Sommer

Erschienen in: BMC Neurology | Ausgabe 1/2011

Abstract

Background

"Negative affect" is one of the major migraine triggers. The aim of the study was to assess attentional biases for negative affective stimuli that might be related to migraine triggers in migraine patients with either few or frequent migraine and healthy controls.

Methods

Thirty-three subjects with frequent migraine (FM) or with less frequent episodic migraine, and 20 healthy controls conducted two emotional Stroop tasks in the interictal period. In task 1, general affective words and in task 2, pictures of affective faces (angry, neutral, happy) were used. For each task we calculated two emotional Stroop indices. Groups were compared using one-way ANOVAs.

Results

The expected attentional bias in migraine patients was not found. However, in task 2 the controls showed a significant attentional bias to negative faces, whereas the FM group showed indices near zero. Thus, the FM group responded faster to negative than to positive stimuli. The difference between the groups was statistically significant.

Conclusions

The findings in the FM group may reflect a learned avoidance mechanism away from affective migraine triggers.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1471-2377-11-141) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

AP conceived of the study, conducted the experiments and performed the statistical analyses. CS participated in the design of the study and its coordination and helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Background

The reported prevalence of chronic migraine in the population is 1.4-2.2.% [1]. Patients with episodic migraine have an annual risk of 2,5% to develop chronic migraine [2]. An intermediate headache frequency of 6 to 9 days per month and even more a critical frequency of 10 to 14 headache days per month increase the risk for chronicity [3]. Further risk factors are obesity, stressful life events, snoring, and overuse of certain classes of medication. Up to 90% of migraine patients are able to name trigger factors like emotional stress, sleep deprivation, visual triggers (e.g. flickering lights) or hormonal changes that precipitate their attacks [4, 5]. Triggers are equally named by patients with episodic and chronic migraine [6]. The mode of action of these migraine triggers is as yet unknown. Several authors proposed trigeminal signaling mechanisms that sensitize certain brain areas, resulting in general sensitization of meningeal nociceptors and subsequently migraine pain [7, 8]. It has also been suggested that the interaction of several triggers leads to migraine attacks [9, 10].
An attentional bias towards pain-related cues, i.e. selective attention to pain-related information, is one of the psychological variables that are thought to be involved in turning an episodic into a chronic pain disorder [11, 12]. The emotional Stroop paradigm is a means to assess this bias. In this task subjects have to name the color of emotionally relevant stimuli as fast as possible while ignoring the affective content of the stimuli. The underlying theory assumes that threatening (pain-related) stimuli draw the subjects' attention, and thus increase the latencies for colour naming compared to neutral stimuli or positive stimuli [13, 14]. The opposite effect can occur when the affective content of the stimuli is avoided and color naming of negative stimuli therefore is speeded up [15]. A recent metanalysis identified five studies using an emotional Stroop task with pain-related words that found an attentional bias towards sensory and affective pain words in chronic pain patients [16]. Similar results were found in a study using personalized pain words [17]. However, no such attentional bias was found in chronic headache patients [18]. Thus, until now, there is no evidence of attentional biases in the Stroop task in patients with migraine. Other studies investigated chronic pain patients using the so called visual probe task and failed to find the expected attentional bias [1921]. In contrast, Liossi and collegues [22] found selective attention towards pain related words in a group of headache patients.
We hypothesized that for migraine patients trigger related stimuli rather than pain related stimuli are threatening and thus cause an attentional bias. As most of the patients associate their attacks to trigger factors as reported above, it is likely that these trigger factors over time develop a threatening character for the patients. Therefore, we conducted two emotional Stroop tasks, one with general affective words and the second with social affective cues. In the latter, pictures of affective faces are used due to their higher ecological validity [23]. An attentional bias towards negative affective stimuli was expected in both tasks for a group of subjects with very frequent migraine (FM, thus at higher risk for or already transformed to chronic migraine) compared to subjects with less frequent, episodic migraine (EM) and healthy controls. We hypothesized that FM patients should be faster in colour naming of neutral or positive stimuli than of negative stimuli, which would be reflected in higher stroop indices.

Methods

Participants

33 subjects with migraine diagnosed according to the diagnostic criteria of the International Headache Society (IHS, 2004) were recruited via the Pain and Headache Clinic of the Department of Neurology at the University hospital, Würzburg. The control group consisted of 20 age- and sex-matched healthy persons without migraines or frequent headaches, recruited via newspaper announcements. Exclusion criteria were: other pain, neurological or inflammatory acute or chronic diseases, antidepressive, anticonvulsive, neuroleptic or anti-inflammatory medication, and symptomatic medication like triptans or NSAIDs in the 48 hours preceding the day of testing. Patients with only menstrual migraine were excluded. Each subject gave written informed consent. Approval of the ethics comitee of the university of Wuerzburg was granted. Subjects received a small compensation for participation. German was the prime language used by all participants. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. None were under the influence of alcohol or drugs at the time of testing, and none had consumed caffeine within the three hours prior to testing. Information about alcohol, drug, and caffeine consumption was obtained through self report (i.e. no blood or urine indices were conducted for confirmation). Patients were tested interictally at least 48 hours after the last migraine attack and all were headache free at the time of testing. One limitation of conducting this type of study in a population of patients with frequent headache is that some of them might already be in a pre-ictal stage. They were asked to name their personal migraine trigger factors.
Patients were divided into two groups according to their monthly migraine and headache frequency: FM, when the headache frequency was at least 10 headache days per month and the monthly frequency of migraine attacks was at least four (n = 16), and EM, when the headache frequency was below 10 headache days per month and the monthly attack frequency was below four (n = 17, Table 1). This division was chosen instead of following the IHS definition for episodic and chronic migraine, because we wanted to include those patients at high risk for migraine chronification [3].
Table 1
Demographic data and headache parameters for all three groups.
 
Group
 
Co
EM
FM
n
20
17
16
female
90.5%
85%
100%
age (M, SD)
39.8 (10.5)
41.35 (11.87)
43.4 (13.3)
migraine duration in years (M, SD)
no headaches
22 (12)
27 (15)
migraine attacks per month (M, SD)
no headaches
1.7 (1.2)
6.4 (2.7)
headache days per month (M, SD)
no headaches
4.5 (2.2)
17.2 (6.37)
prophylaxis - betablockers a
5%
35%
16%
triggers b
no headaches
  
   stress
 
85%
81%
   hormones
 
30%
44%
   weather
 
50%
75%
   visual triggers
 
5%
50%
   sleep problems
 
55%
56%
Co: healthy controls; EM: episodic migraine patients; FM: frequent migraine patients.
a Most frequently named trigger factors of a list of 20.
Table 1 displays descriptive data and the headache parameters of the participants. The three groups did not differ regarding their age (M=41 years, SD=12). The two migraine groups did not differ regarding time since migraine onset (disease duration; M=15, SD=15). The migraine groups differed significantly with respect to the number of headache days per month (EM: M=4.5, SD=2.2; FM: M=17.2; SD=6.4; t(18)=-7.58, p < .001) and to the monthly number of migraine attacks (EM: M=1.6, SD=1.2; FM: M=6.4, SD=2.8; t(20)=-6.30, p < .001). They did not differ in the frequency of migraine with aura (EM: 37%, FM: 59%; χ 2(1) = 1.72, p = .316). The majority of the patients named "psychosocial stress" as an important trigger factor for their attacks (83%). Hormonal changes, weather and sleep problems were also named by nearly half of the patients.

Self-report instruments

Participants completed several self-report questionnaires. To assess general disability due to headaches subjects filled in the German version of the Migraine Disability Assessment Score (MIDAS; [24]). Trait anxiety was assessed using the German trait version of the State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-T; [25]), a 20-item likert-scaled anxiety questionnaire. Depressive symptoms were measured using the German revised Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II; [26, 27]). Additionally, the Penn State Worry Questionnaire was used to learn about the occurrence of worries in the participants (PSWQ; [28]).

Experiment

Both tasks were computerized versions of the modified Stroop test [13] with emotional stimuli (emotional Stroop test). Since our group of our participants consisted mainly of middle aged women with little experience in computer usage, we chose this relativly simple task to make sure they would be able to perform it without problems or misunderstandings after a set of practice trials with neutral stimuli.

Task 1: Emotional Stroop test - words

To assess a possible attentional bias towards negative emotional stimuli, in the first experiment 36 affective nouns from the validated Berlin Affective Word List [29] were used, 12 for each valence category (negative, neutral, positive) (Table 2). Words were matched according to length and frequency. The experiment was programmed using the software Presentation® by Neurobehavioral Systems. Stimuli were presented in random order with a size of 36 pt. Each word was presented in the colors red, green, and blue on a black background in the centre of a 17"-computer screen. In total the experiment consisted of 108 trials. Participants were instructed to name the color of the words by pressing the button with the correct color on a colored keyboard as fast as possible while ignoring word content. After pressing the button the word disappeared. If the button was not pressed, words disappeared after 2000 ms. After an inter trial interval of 500 ms the next word appeared. The experiment was preceded by a 40-trial practice part with neutral words. The color naming latencies for each word and also the number of incorrect responses were recorded for each participant.
Table 2
Words from the Berlin Affective Word List (translated).
Valence category
Negative
Neutral
Positive
murder
chin
fortune
fear
base
fun
army
stone
kiss
poison
figure
friend
homicide
mood
pleasure
slave
hall
blossom
betrayal
area
humor
panic
guarantee
love
prison
reaction
trust
eradication
semester
laughter
threat
interview
amusement
agony
influence
health

Task 2: Emotional Stroop test - faces

To assess a possible attentional bias towards social affective cues, in the second experiment 81 pictures of affective faces were used as stimuli. Pictures were taken from the Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces (KDEF; [30]). Each valence category (negative: angry faces, neutral: neutral faces, positive: happy faces) consisted of 27 pictures. Each picture was displayed in black and white and had a frame of red, green, or blue. In total, the experiment consisted of 81 trials which were presented in random order on a black background at a 17"-computer screen. There were three sets of trials to prevent systematic errors due to colour. In each set pictures were only presented once, but each time with different colors. The sets were allocated equally to the participants. Similarly to the previous experiment participants were instructed to name the color of the frame as fast as possible through pressing the corresponding colored button on the keyboard while ignoring picture content. Between the trials a fixation cross was displayed for 500 ms. Maximum possible response time was 2000 ms. Reaction times as well as errors were recorded.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted using the software SPSS 17.02. Two emotional Stroop indices (ESI) were calculated. The first index compares the color naming latencies of negative stimuli with the latencies of neutral stimuli (ESI-N), as done in the anxiety [31] and pain literature [12]. ESI-N is calculated using the following formula: ESI-N = RTnegative - RTneutral . The second index compares the response latencies of negative stimuli with the latencies of positive stimuli (ESI-P), as described in the original emotional Stroop study [13]. ESI-P is calculated as follows: ESI-P = RTnegative - RTpositive. RTnegative are the mean color naming latencies for negative words or faces, RTneutral are the mean color naming latencies for neutral words or faces, and RTpositive are the mean color naming latencies for positive words or faces. All indices are displayed in milliseconds. One-sample Student's t-tests were calculated to obtain informations about the difference of the indices from zero. Positive indices indicate slower reactions to negative stimuli than to neutral or positive stimuli; negative indices indicate faster reactions to negative stimuli. To compare the three groups, one-way analyses of variances were calculated for each index and experiment separately. A priori contrasts were set to specify the group differences according to the hypotheses. Alpha-level was set at p < .05. In addition, Pearson correlations between the indices and headache parameters as well as the self-report scores were calculated.

Results

Data reduction and errors

Fifty-three participants completed the above described tests. In total 2.3% of the data were excluded due to reaction errors. There were no group differences in the occurrence of errors (p>.05). Data with latencies smaller than 200 ms and above 1200 ms were excluded as outliers (3.5%).

Self reports

Table 3 shows the mean scores of the self-report questionnaires for each group. Regarding the disability due to migraine and headaches the two patient groups differed significantly (F(32,1) = 12.67, p=.001) with the FM group exhibiting a threefold higher MIDAS score than the EM group. 67% of the FM patients reached grade IV with "severe disability" in contrast to 16% of the EM patients. The three groups furthermore differed regarding their BDI-II scores (F(2,50) = 3.64; p=.033) with the FM group exhibiting the highest score (M=12, SD=9). 20% of the FM group had moderate to severe depressive symptoms in comparison to 11% in the EM group and 5% in the control group, respectively. With respect to trait anxiety the STAI-T scores of the three groups did not differ (F(2,50) = 3.01, p=.058). Analyses of the PSWQ scores revealed a significant main effect for group (F(2,50) = 5.49, p=.007), which means that the FM group reported significantly more worries in the PSWQ than the control group (p=.003), but not more worries than the EM group (p=.420).
Table 3
Results of the self report measures.
 
Group
ANOVA
 
Co
EM
FM
F- test
MIDAS (M, SD)
no headaches
14 (10)
46 (38)
p=.001 **
severe disability
no headaches
16%
67%
 
BDI-II (M, SD)
6 (5)
10 (7)
12 (9)
p=.033 *
STAI-T (M, SD)
36 (9)
43 (11)
42 (12)
p=.058
PSWQ (M, SD)
39 (11)
46 (10)
49 (12)
p=.007 **
Groups were compared using one-way ANOVAs. * p < .05 ** p < .01
Co: control group; EM: episodic migraine group; FM: frequent migraine group

Task 1: Emotional Stroop test - words

Figure 1 shows means and standard errors of the emotional Stroop indices ESI-N and ESI-P for words. Mean ESI-N of all three groups had positive values (controls: M=9.5 ms, SEM = 8; EM: M=12.5 ms, SEM = 8; FM: M=14.6 ms, SEM = 7), which implies that all three groups were slower in colour naming of negative than of neutral words. T-tests revealed a significant difference from zero only for the FM group (t(15) = 2.21, p=.043). ANOVA did not reveal a significant main effect of group, i.e. no group difference (F(2,49) = 0.11, p = .895). Mean ESI-P was slightly below zero for the control group and slightly positive for the EM and FM group (controls: M=-6.4 ms, SEM = 11; EM: M=4.3 ms, SEM = 6; FM: M=-0.4 ms, SEM = 9). T-tests did not reveal a significant difference from zero for any of the groups. There was no group difference, either (F(2,49)=.38, p=.687).

Task 2: Emotional Stroop test - faces

The indices ESI-N and ESI-P for faces are displayed in Figure 2. The ESI-N for faces showed a linear decline from the control group to the FM group (controls: M=29.0 ms, SEM = 7; EM: M=7.7 ms, SEM = 11; FM: M=3.6 ms, SEM = 6). The ESI-N of the controls differed significantly from zero (t(19) = 3.94, p=.001), whereas the ESI-N of the EM and FM group were not significantly different from zero. This result indicates that the controls responded more slowly to negative faces than to neutral faces. Analysis of variances resulted in a trend toward a group effect (F(2, 50) = 2.73, p=.075). A priori set contrasts yielded a trend toward a difference between the ESI-N of the control group and the EM group (p=.076) and a significant difference between the control and the FM group (p=.038) with both migraine groups exhibiting lower indices than the controls. The results of the ESI-P for faces showed the same pattern: a linear decline from the control group to the FM group (controls: M=23.6 ms, SEM = 8; EM: M=11.2 ms, SEM = 12; FM: M=-9.92 ms, SEM = 7). The control group had the highest ESI-P, significantly above zero (t(19) = 2.91, p=.003) whereas the FM group had the lowest index, slightly below zero (t(15)=-1.36, p=.195), which means, the FM group responded faster to negative than to positive stimuli. The analysis of variances revealed a marginally significant group effect (F(2, 50) = 3.15, p=.05). A priori contrasts showed that the index of the control group was significantly higher than the ESI-P of the FM group (p=.016) but not higher than the ESI-P of the EM group (p=.35). This indicates that the group effect is due to the reaction differences of the control group and the FM group wheres the EM group reacted similar to the controls.

Correlations

Table 4 displays the correlations of the indices with the self report scores and participants' characteristics. The word indices did not correlate with any of the headache parameters or self report scores. The face index ESI-N correlated significantly with migraine duration (r=-.341, p=.012). There was a trend to a correlation with headache frequency (r=-.228, p=.101). The ESI-P correlated significantly with headache frequency (r=-.305, p=.026) and marginally significantly with migraine duration (r=-.257, p=.064)
Table 4
Correlations of all indices with the headache parameters and self report scores.
 
correlations
 
word indices
face indices
 
ESI-N
ESI-P
ESI-N
ESI-P
age
.201
.160
-.135
-.177
migraine duration
.187
.174
-.341 *
-.257 (*)
headache frequency
.050
.046
-.228 (*)
-.305 *
MIDAS
.037
.106
-.205
-.313 *
BDI-II
-.021
.078
-.218
-.097
STAI-T
-.105
.013
-.187
-.143
PSWQ
-.083
.098
-.242
-.185
* p < .05 (*) p < .10
ESI-N: Emotional Stroop index comparing the color naming latencies of negative stimuli with the latencies of neutral stimuli.
ESI-P: Emotional Stroop index comparing the color naming latencies of negative stimuli with the latencies of positive stimuli.

Discussion and conclusions

According to the results from the self reports, the migraine patients in this study, especially the patients with very frequent and chronic migraine, resulted to be highly disabled by their headaches. Moreover, they reported more anxiety, depressive symptoms, and worries than the healthy control group.
In task 1, no group effects for the ESI-N were found despite a significant difference in color naming latencies of neutral and negative words in the FM group. All groups reacted the same way, with slower reactions to negative than to neutral words. No group effect was found for the index comparing the reaction times to negative words with the reaction times to positive words (ESI-P), either. Because of the missing group effects, the results cannot be interpreted in favor of the hypothesis of an attentional bias for negative words in the FM group.
One reason for the weak results in task one might lie in the small sample size. With low numbers like in the present study it is only possible to detect strong effects. Smaller but possibly meaningful differences do not become statistically significant. Moreover, already several former studies doubted the use of words as stimuli, because words might not be strong enough to elicit significant attentional biases, especially in pain patients [3234]. Also, the words we used in the present study were general affective words. The use of words that reflect the common migraine triggers more specifically might lead to more pronounced effects.
In task 2, the controls showed a significant attentional bias to negative faces, whereas the FM group and the EM group did not. There was almost no difference between the reactions to neutral faces and the reactions to negative faces in the FM group, which was reflected by an index near zero. Comparing the color naming latencies for negative faces to those for positive faces, the effect became even stronger. The FM group here showed faster reactions to negative faces than to positive faces, with an index below zero, in contrast to the control group, which showed a high positive index.
The small indices of the FM patients, which in case of ESI-P is even negative, and the differences between FM patients and controls, could be an indicator of avoidance. Instead of resulting in the expected attentional bias towards negative social stimuli as reflected by angry faces, the subjects with FM showed unexpected avoidance behavior, i.e. they were faster in color naming for negative pictures than for neutral or positive pictures. Furthermore, an association of this avoidance with disease duration and migraine frequency, as reflected by significant correlations, could be found.
Our behavioral data support the self report data from a recent study. There, patients with chronic migraines reported significantly more avoidant coping strategies than patients with episodic migraines [35]. These findings are in line with the assumptions of Martin [36] and Martin and MacLeod [8], who stated that general avoidance of headache triggers might be a part of migraine worsening. They claimed that the role of avoidance in migraine exacerbation might be twofold: first, trigger avoidance does not decrease but rather increases the sensitivity towards the triggers, and therefore leads to an increased probability of migraine attacks in response to triggers [37]. Second, constantly avoiding situations that might possibly lead to migraine attacks may further result in loss of functional coping abilities in these situations and therefore may cause exactly the stress to the subjects they actually try to avoid [38]. Some findings of Martin and colleagues [38, 39] examining the relationship between sensitivity and exposure to certain triggers like stress, noise or visual disturbances, support the idea of heightened trigger sensitivity with avoidance.
Our results indicate that the advice to avoid personal trigger factors in subjects with frequent migraines might lead to automated and maybe dysfunctional avoidance behavior in the long term. From the anxiety literature it is known that behavioral and cognitive avoidance play a role in the maintenance of disorders [40]. Clinicians should be made aware of the role dysfunctional avoidance behaviors could play in the process of migraine chronification.
As our study did not primarily aim to assess avoidance behavior, studies with psychological paradigms like the visual probe task, where both directions of attention can be investigated, would be needed to further strengthen this point. The relationship between trigger avoidance, dysfunctional coping strategies and disability in migraine patients merits further investigation.

Acknowledgements

We thank P. Pauli, P. Weyers, G. Alpers and M. Andreatta, Deparment of Psychology I of the University of Wuerzburg for support during the planning phase of the study.
A.P. was supported by a stipend from the Research Training Group „Emotions‟, RTG 1253/1 (German Research Fundation, DFG) and by research funds of the University of Wuerzburg. This publication was funded by the DFG and the University of Wuerzburg in the funding programme Open Access Publishing.
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

AP conceived of the study, conducted the experiments and performed the statistical analyses. CS participated in the design of the study and its coordination and helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Anhänge

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Natoli JL, Manack A, Dean B, Butler Q, Turkel CC, Stovner L, Lipton RB: Global prevalence of chronic migraine: a systematic review. Cephalalgia. 2010, 30 (5): 599-609.PubMed Natoli JL, Manack A, Dean B, Butler Q, Turkel CC, Stovner L, Lipton RB: Global prevalence of chronic migraine: a systematic review. Cephalalgia. 2010, 30 (5): 599-609.PubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Bigal ME, Serrano D, Buse D, Scher A, Stewart WF, Lipton RB: Acute migraine medications and evolution from episodic to chronic migraine: a longitudinal population-based study. Headache. 2008, 48 (8): 1157-1168. 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2008.01217.x.CrossRefPubMed Bigal ME, Serrano D, Buse D, Scher A, Stewart WF, Lipton RB: Acute migraine medications and evolution from episodic to chronic migraine: a longitudinal population-based study. Headache. 2008, 48 (8): 1157-1168. 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2008.01217.x.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Lipton RB: Tracing transformation: chronic migraine classification, progression, and epidemiology. Neurology. 2009, 72 (5 Suppl): S3-7.CrossRefPubMed Lipton RB: Tracing transformation: chronic migraine classification, progression, and epidemiology. Neurology. 2009, 72 (5 Suppl): S3-7.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin PR, Milech D, Nathan PR: Towards a functional model of chronic headaches: investigation of antecedents and consequences. Headache. 1993, 33 (9): 461-470. 10.1111/j.1526-4610.1993.hed3309461.x.CrossRefPubMed Martin PR, Milech D, Nathan PR: Towards a functional model of chronic headaches: investigation of antecedents and consequences. Headache. 1993, 33 (9): 461-470. 10.1111/j.1526-4610.1993.hed3309461.x.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Kelman L: The triggers or precipitants of the acute migraine attack. Cephalalgia. 2007, 27 (5): 394-402. 10.1111/j.1468-2982.2007.01303.x.CrossRefPubMed Kelman L: The triggers or precipitants of the acute migraine attack. Cephalalgia. 2007, 27 (5): 394-402. 10.1111/j.1468-2982.2007.01303.x.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Andress-Rothrock D, King W, Rothrock J: An analysis of migraine triggers in a clinic-based population. Headache. 2010, 50 (8): 1366-1370. 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2010.01753.x.CrossRefPubMed Andress-Rothrock D, King W, Rothrock J: An analysis of migraine triggers in a clinic-based population. Headache. 2010, 50 (8): 1366-1370. 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2010.01753.x.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Burstein R, Jakubowski M: Unitary hypothesis for multiple triggers of the pain and strain of migraine. J Comp Neurol. 2005, 493 (1): 9-14. 10.1002/cne.20688.CrossRefPubMed Burstein R, Jakubowski M: Unitary hypothesis for multiple triggers of the pain and strain of migraine. J Comp Neurol. 2005, 493 (1): 9-14. 10.1002/cne.20688.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin PR, MacLeod C: Behavioral management of headache triggers: Avoidance of triggers is an inadequate strategy. Clin Psychol Rev. 2009, 29 (6): 483-495. 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.05.002.CrossRefPubMed Martin PR, MacLeod C: Behavioral management of headache triggers: Avoidance of triggers is an inadequate strategy. Clin Psychol Rev. 2009, 29 (6): 483-495. 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.05.002.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Leviton A: To what extent does food sensitivity contribute to headache recurrence?. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1984, 26 (4): 542-545.CrossRefPubMed Leviton A: To what extent does food sensitivity contribute to headache recurrence?. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1984, 26 (4): 542-545.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Lambert GA, Zagami AS: The mode of action of migraine triggers: a hypothesis. Headache. 2009, 49 (2): 253-275. 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2008.01230.x.CrossRefPubMed Lambert GA, Zagami AS: The mode of action of migraine triggers: a hypothesis. Headache. 2009, 49 (2): 253-275. 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2008.01230.x.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Morley S, Eccleston C, Williams A: Systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials of cognitive behaviour therapy and behaviour therapy for chronic pain in adults, excluding headache. Pain. 1999, 80 (1-2): 1-13. 10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00255-3.CrossRefPubMed Morley S, Eccleston C, Williams A: Systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials of cognitive behaviour therapy and behaviour therapy for chronic pain in adults, excluding headache. Pain. 1999, 80 (1-2): 1-13. 10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00255-3.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Pincus T, Morley S: Cognitive-processing bias in chronic pain: a review and integration. Psychol Bull. 2001, 127 (5): 599-617.CrossRefPubMed Pincus T, Morley S: Cognitive-processing bias in chronic pain: a review and integration. Psychol Bull. 2001, 127 (5): 599-617.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Mathews A, MacLeod C: Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. Behav Res Ther. 1985, 23 (5): 563-569. 10.1016/0005-7967(85)90104-4.CrossRefPubMed Mathews A, MacLeod C: Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. Behav Res Ther. 1985, 23 (5): 563-569. 10.1016/0005-7967(85)90104-4.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Williams JM, Mathews A, MacLeod C: The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychol Bull. 1996, 120 (1): 3-24.CrossRefPubMed Williams JM, Mathews A, MacLeod C: The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychol Bull. 1996, 120 (1): 3-24.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat de Ruiter C, Brosschot JF: The emotional Stroop interference effect in anxiety: attentional bias or cognitive avoidance?. Behav Res Ther. 1994, 32 (3): 315-319. 10.1016/0005-7967(94)90128-7.CrossRefPubMed de Ruiter C, Brosschot JF: The emotional Stroop interference effect in anxiety: attentional bias or cognitive avoidance?. Behav Res Ther. 1994, 32 (3): 315-319. 10.1016/0005-7967(94)90128-7.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Roelofs J, Peters ML, Zeegers MP, Vlaeyen JW: The modified Stroop paradigm as a measure of selective attention towards pain-related stimuli among chronic pain patients: a meta-analysis. Eur J Pain. 2002, 6 (4): 273-281. 10.1053/eujp.2002.0337.CrossRefPubMed Roelofs J, Peters ML, Zeegers MP, Vlaeyen JW: The modified Stroop paradigm as a measure of selective attention towards pain-related stimuli among chronic pain patients: a meta-analysis. Eur J Pain. 2002, 6 (4): 273-281. 10.1053/eujp.2002.0337.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Andersson G, Haldrup D: Personalized pain words and Stroop interference in chronic pain patients. Eur J Pain. 2003, 7 (5): 431-438. 10.1016/S1090-3801(03)00002-8.CrossRefPubMed Andersson G, Haldrup D: Personalized pain words and Stroop interference in chronic pain patients. Eur J Pain. 2003, 7 (5): 431-438. 10.1016/S1090-3801(03)00002-8.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Asmundson GJ, Carleton RN, Ekong J: Dot-probe evaluation of selective attentional processing of pain cues in patients with chronic headaches. Pain. 2005, 114 (1-2): 250-256. 10.1016/j.pain.2004.12.025.CrossRefPubMed Asmundson GJ, Carleton RN, Ekong J: Dot-probe evaluation of selective attentional processing of pain cues in patients with chronic headaches. Pain. 2005, 114 (1-2): 250-256. 10.1016/j.pain.2004.12.025.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Asmundson GJ, Kuperos JL, Norton GR: Do patients with chronic pain selectively attend to pain-related information?: preliminary evidence for the mediating role of fear. Pain. 1997, 72 (1-2): 27-32. 10.1016/S0304-3959(97)00010-9.CrossRefPubMed Asmundson GJ, Kuperos JL, Norton GR: Do patients with chronic pain selectively attend to pain-related information?: preliminary evidence for the mediating role of fear. Pain. 1997, 72 (1-2): 27-32. 10.1016/S0304-3959(97)00010-9.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Roelofs J, Peters ML, Fassaert T, Vlaeyen JW: The role of fear of movement and injury in selective attentional processing in patients with chronic low back pain: a dot-probe evaluation. J Pain. 2005, 6 (5): 294-300. 10.1016/j.jpain.2004.12.011.CrossRefPubMed Roelofs J, Peters ML, Fassaert T, Vlaeyen JW: The role of fear of movement and injury in selective attentional processing in patients with chronic low back pain: a dot-probe evaluation. J Pain. 2005, 6 (5): 294-300. 10.1016/j.jpain.2004.12.011.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Asmundson GJ, Hadjistavropoulos HD: Is high fear of pain associated with attentional biases for pain-related or general threat? A categorical reanalysis. J Pain. 2007, 8 (1): 11-18. 10.1016/j.jpain.2006.05.008.CrossRefPubMed Asmundson GJ, Hadjistavropoulos HD: Is high fear of pain associated with attentional biases for pain-related or general threat? A categorical reanalysis. J Pain. 2007, 8 (1): 11-18. 10.1016/j.jpain.2006.05.008.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Liossi C, White P, Schoth DE: Time-course of attentional bias for threat-related cues in patients with chronic daily headache-tension type: evidence for the role of anger. Eur J Pain. 2010, 15 (1): 92-98.CrossRefPubMed Liossi C, White P, Schoth DE: Time-course of attentional bias for threat-related cues in patients with chronic daily headache-tension type: evidence for the role of anger. Eur J Pain. 2010, 15 (1): 92-98.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Beall PM HA: The face wins: Stronger automatic processing of affect in facial expressions than words in a modified Stroop task. Cogn Em. 2008, 22: 1613-1642. 10.1080/02699930801940370.CrossRef Beall PM HA: The face wins: Stronger automatic processing of affect in facial expressions than words in a modified Stroop task. Cogn Em. 2008, 22: 1613-1642. 10.1080/02699930801940370.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Stewart WF, Lipton RB, Whyte J, Dowson A, Kolodner K, Liberman JN, Sawyer J: An international study to assess reliability of the Migraine Disability Assessment (MIDAS) score. Neurology. 1999, 53 (5): 988-994.CrossRefPubMed Stewart WF, Lipton RB, Whyte J, Dowson A, Kolodner K, Liberman JN, Sawyer J: An international study to assess reliability of the Migraine Disability Assessment (MIDAS) score. Neurology. 1999, 53 (5): 988-994.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Laux L GP, Schaffner P, Spielberger CD: Das State-Trait-Angstinventar. Theoretische Grundlagen und Handanweisung. 1981, Weinheim: Beltz Test GmbH Laux L GP, Schaffner P, Spielberger CD: Das State-Trait-Angstinventar. Theoretische Grundlagen und Handanweisung. 1981, Weinheim: Beltz Test GmbH
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Beck AT SR, Brown GK, Beck Depression Inventory: San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation. 1998, Bullinger & Kirchberger Beck AT SR, Brown GK, Beck Depression Inventory: San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation. 1998, Bullinger & Kirchberger
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Hautzinger M KF, Kühner C: Das Beck-Depressionsinventar II: Deutsche Bearbeitung und Handbuch zum BDI-II. 2006, Frankfurt a. M: Harcourt Test Services Hautzinger M KF, Kühner C: Das Beck-Depressionsinventar II: Deutsche Bearbeitung und Handbuch zum BDI-II. 2006, Frankfurt a. M: Harcourt Test Services
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Meyer TJ, Miller ML, Metzger RL, Borkovec TD: Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behav Res Ther. 1990, 28 (6): 487-495. 10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6.CrossRefPubMed Meyer TJ, Miller ML, Metzger RL, Borkovec TD: Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behav Res Ther. 1990, 28 (6): 487-495. 10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Vo ML, Jacobs AM, Conrad M: Cross-validating the Berlin Affective Word List. Behav Res Methods. 2006, 38 (4): 606-609. 10.3758/BF03193892.CrossRefPubMed Vo ML, Jacobs AM, Conrad M: Cross-validating the Berlin Affective Word List. Behav Res Methods. 2006, 38 (4): 606-609. 10.3758/BF03193892.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Lundqvist D, Flykt A, Öhman A: The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces - KDEF, CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience. 1998, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet Lundqvist D, Flykt A, Öhman A: The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces - KDEF, CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience. 1998, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Mogg K, Mathews A, Weinman J: Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states: a replication. Behav Res Ther. 1989, 27 (4): 317-323. 10.1016/0005-7967(89)90001-6.CrossRefPubMed Mogg K, Mathews A, Weinman J: Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states: a replication. Behav Res Ther. 1989, 27 (4): 317-323. 10.1016/0005-7967(89)90001-6.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Constantine R, McNally RJ, Hornig CD: Snake fear and the pictorial emotional Stroop paradigm. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2001, 25: 757-764. 10.1023/A:1012923507617.CrossRef Constantine R, McNally RJ, Hornig CD: Snake fear and the pictorial emotional Stroop paradigm. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2001, 25: 757-764. 10.1023/A:1012923507617.CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Harris CR, Pashler H: Attention and the processing of emotional words and names: not so special after all. Psychol Sci. 2004, 15 (3): 171-178. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503005.x.CrossRefPubMed Harris CR, Pashler H: Attention and the processing of emotional words and names: not so special after all. Psychol Sci. 2004, 15 (3): 171-178. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503005.x.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Schimmack U, Derryberry D: Attentional interference effects of emotional pictures: threat, negativity, or arousal?. Emotion. 2005, 5 (1): 55-66.CrossRefPubMed Schimmack U, Derryberry D: Attentional interference effects of emotional pictures: threat, negativity, or arousal?. Emotion. 2005, 5 (1): 55-66.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Radat F, Lanteri-Minet M, Nachit-Ouinekh F, Massiou H, Lucas C, Pradalier A, Mercier F, El Hasnaoui A: The GRIM2005 study of migraine consultation in France. III: Psychological features of subjects with migraine. Cephalalgia. 2009, 29 (3): 338-350. 10.1111/j.1468-2982.2008.01718.x.CrossRefPubMed Radat F, Lanteri-Minet M, Nachit-Ouinekh F, Massiou H, Lucas C, Pradalier A, Mercier F, El Hasnaoui A: The GRIM2005 study of migraine consultation in France. III: Psychological features of subjects with migraine. Cephalalgia. 2009, 29 (3): 338-350. 10.1111/j.1468-2982.2008.01718.x.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin PR: Headache triggers: To avoid or not to avoid, that is the question. Psychol Health. 2000, 15: 801-809. 10.1080/08870440008405582.CrossRef Martin PR: Headache triggers: To avoid or not to avoid, that is the question. Psychol Health. 2000, 15: 801-809. 10.1080/08870440008405582.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin PR: How do trigger factors acquire the capacity to precipitate headaches?. Behav Res Ther. 2001, 39 (5): 545-554. 10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00032-2.CrossRefPubMed Martin PR: How do trigger factors acquire the capacity to precipitate headaches?. Behav Res Ther. 2001, 39 (5): 545-554. 10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00032-2.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin PR, Lae L, Reece J: Stress as a trigger for headaches: relationship between exposure and sensitivity. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2007, 20 (4): 393-407. 10.1080/10615800701628843.CrossRefPubMed Martin PR, Lae L, Reece J: Stress as a trigger for headaches: relationship between exposure and sensitivity. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2007, 20 (4): 393-407. 10.1080/10615800701628843.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin PR, Seneviratne HM: Effects of food deprivation and a stressor on head pain. Health Psychol. 1997, 16 (4): 310-318.CrossRefPubMed Martin PR, Seneviratne HM: Effects of food deprivation and a stressor on head pain. Health Psychol. 1997, 16 (4): 310-318.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Ouimet AJ, Gawronski B, Dozois DJ: Cognitive vulnerability to anxiety: A review and an integrative model. Clin Psychol Rev. 2009, 29 (6): 459-470. 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.05.004.CrossRefPubMed Ouimet AJ, Gawronski B, Dozois DJ: Cognitive vulnerability to anxiety: A review and an integrative model. Clin Psychol Rev. 2009, 29 (6): 459-470. 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.05.004.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Hypervigilance or avoidance of trigger related cues in migraineurs? - A case-control study using the emotional stroop task
verfasst von
Anne-Katrin Puschmann
Claudia Sommer
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2011
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Neurology / Ausgabe 1/2011
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2377
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2377-11-141

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2011

BMC Neurology 1/2011 Zur Ausgabe

Neu in den Fachgebieten Neurologie und Psychiatrie

Akuter Schwindel: Wann lohnt sich eine MRT?

28.04.2024 Schwindel Nachrichten

Akuter Schwindel stellt oft eine diagnostische Herausforderung dar. Wie nützlich dabei eine MRT ist, hat eine Studie aus Finnland untersucht. Immerhin einer von sechs Patienten wurde mit akutem ischämischem Schlaganfall diagnostiziert.

Niedriger diastolischer Blutdruck erhöht Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Komplikationen

25.04.2024 Hypotonie Nachrichten

Wenn unter einer medikamentösen Hochdrucktherapie der diastolische Blutdruck in den Keller geht, steigt das Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Ereignisse: Darauf deutet eine Sekundäranalyse der SPRINT-Studie hin.

Frühe Alzheimertherapie lohnt sich

25.04.2024 AAN-Jahrestagung 2024 Nachrichten

Ist die Tau-Last noch gering, scheint der Vorteil von Lecanemab besonders groß zu sein. Und beginnen Erkrankte verzögert mit der Behandlung, erreichen sie nicht mehr die kognitive Leistung wie bei einem früheren Start. Darauf deuten neue Analysen der Phase-3-Studie Clarity AD.

Viel Bewegung in der Parkinsonforschung

25.04.2024 Parkinson-Krankheit Nachrichten

Neue arznei- und zellbasierte Ansätze, Frühdiagnose mit Bewegungssensoren, Rückenmarkstimulation gegen Gehblockaden – in der Parkinsonforschung tut sich einiges. Auf dem Deutschen Parkinsonkongress ging es auch viel um technische Innovationen.