Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics 1/2022

Open Access 01.12.2022 | Research

Impact of early term and late preterm birth on infants’ neurodevelopment: evidence from a cohort study in Wuhan, China

verfasst von: Zhong Chen, Chao Xiong, Hua Liu, Junyu Duan, Chun Kang, Cong Yao, Kai Chen, Yawen Chen, Yan Liu, Mingzhu Liu, Aifen Zhou

Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics | Ausgabe 1/2022

Abstract

Background

The incidences of early term and late preterm birth have increased worldwide during recent years. However, there is a lack of prospective study about the influence of early term and late preterm birth on infants’ neurodevelopment, especially at the early stage. Therefore, we conducted this cohort study to investigate the impact of early term and late preterm birth on infants’ neurodevelopment within 6 months.

Methods

This cohort study was conducted in Wuhan, China, between October 2012 and September 2013. A total of 4243 singleton infants born within 34-41 weeks of gestation at Wuhan Children’s Hospital were included. The Gesell Developmental Scale (GDS) was utilized to evaluate the neurodevelopment of infants.

Results

Among the 4243 included participants, 155 (3.65%) were late preterm infants, 1288 (30.36%) were early term infants, and 2800 (65.99%) were full term infants. After adjusted for potential confounders, significant negative relationship was shown between late preterm birth and development quotient (DQ) in all domains of neurodevelopment: gross motor (β = − 17.42, 95% CI: − 21.15 to − 13.69), fine motor (β = − 23.61, 95% CI: − 28.52 to − 18.69), adaptability (β = − 10.10, 95% CI: − 13.82 to − 6.38), language (β = − 6.28, 95% CI: − 9.82 to − 2.74) and social behavior (β = − 5.99, 95% CI: − 9.59 to − 2.39). There was a significant negative trend for early term birth in DQ of fine motor (β = − 2.01, 95% CI: − 3.93 to − 0.09). Late preterm infants had a significantly elevated risk of neurodevelopmental delay in domains of gross motor (adjusted OR = 3.82, 95% CI: 2.67 to 5.46), fine motor (adjusted OR = 3.51, 95% CI: 2.47 to 5.01), and adaptability (adjusted OR = 1.60, 95% CI: 1.12 to 2.29), whereas early term birth was significantly associated with neurodevelopmental delay of fine motor (adjusted OR = 1.22, 95% CI: 1.05 to 1.42).

Conclusions

This study suggested that late preterm birth mainly elevated the risk of neurodevelopmental delay of gross motor, fine motor, and adaptability, whereas early term birth was associated with the developmental delay of fine motor within 6 months. Further research is needed to determine the effectiveness and necessity of the interventions at the early stage for early term and late preterm infants who had suspected neurodevelopmental delay.
Hinweise
Zhong Chen and Chao Xiong contributed equally to this work.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
OR
Odds ratio
CI
Confidence interval
GDS
Gesell Developmental Scale
DQ
Development quotient
GA
Gestational age
ANOVA
One-way analysis of variance
GLM
Generalized linear model

Introduction

Preterm birth (less than 37 weeks of gestation) is generally recognized as a major cause of neonatal morbidity and mortality [1]. However, the conventional 37-week cut-off for preterm and term birth has been considered relatively arbitrary in recent years. A growing number of researchers argue that gestational age over 37 weeks does not mean fetal maturity is enough to avoid morbidity [2]. It is indicated that early term infants (37–38+ 6 weeks of gestation) have an increased risk of health problems than full term infants (39–41+ 6 weeks of gestation). Risks that have been revealed in previous studies include respiratory morbidity, diabetes, obesity-related disorders, long-term developmental outcomes, and even mortality in infancy, childhood, and young adulthood [37]. Late preterm birth (34–36+ 6 weeks of gestation) is also under-recognized, while previous studies are mainly focused on health outcomes of extremely preterm infants (less than 28 weeks of gestation) [8]. Growing evidence has indicated that late preterm infants have a higher risk of morbidity and mortality than more mature infants [9, 10].
Notably, early term and late preterm births have been prevalent worldwide in recent years. It is estimated that early term and late preterm births account for 15 to 31% and 3 to 6% of all live-born infants, respectively [11, 12]. Despite the ascending proportion of late preterm and early term births, the prospective studies about their short-term and long-term health outcomes are insufficient, especially their impact on infants’ neurocognitive and motor developments. Although a few studies have investigated the association between late preterm birth and delayed neurodevelopment [1315], the results are still elusive [10]. Moreover, even fewer studies have investigated the relationship between early term birth and neurodevelopment [16, 17]. In addition, previous studies have mainly been devoted to exploring the long-term impacts of late preterm and early term birth on neurodevelopment [14, 18, 19]. In contrast, few have focused on their impact on neurodevelopment at the early stage of infancy [20], which is the optimum period for interventions, as targeted inventions in the early stages of neurodevelopmental delay will achieve the best benefits.
Given the limited reports and the existing inconclusive results on the impact of early term and late preterm birth on neurodevelopment during early infancy, this study aimed to investigate the effect of early term and late preterm birth on infants’ neurodevelopment within 6 months through a cohort study in Wuhan, China.

Methods

Study population

Participants of this study were derived from a prospective birth cohort study conducted in Wuhan, China, which was designed to investigate prenatal and postnatal risk factors related to child health. Live singleton infants born within 34–41+ 6 weeks of gestation at Wuhan Children’s Hospital between October 2012 and September 2013, without congenital anomalies, were eligible for inclusion in this study.
Initially, a total of 4439 infants met the eligibility criteria after excluding those who refused to accept this routine neurodevelopmental survey (n = 8). In the statistical analysis stage, 4243 infants were incorporated after excluding 196 participants who had incomplete demographic information or health records within 6 months. The Medical Ethics Committee of Wuhan Children’s Hospital (Wuhan Maternal and Child Healthcare Hospital) approved the research protocol, and all the procedures involved in this study were executed following the approved guidelines. Informed consent was obtained from the mothers of infants at their first antenatal examination.

Gestational age classification

Gestational age (GA) was extracted from the delivery record, calculated by the date of delivery and the last recorded menstrual period, and was ascertained by ultrasound examination. According to the GA, infants were categorized as late preterm birth (34 weeks 0 day to 36 weeks 6 days gestation), early term birth (37 weeks 0 day to 38 weeks 6 days gestation), and full term birth (39 weeks 0 day to 41 weeks 6 days gestation) [2].

Infant development

In this study, the Gesell Developmental Scale (GDS) was conducted to evaluate the neurodevelopment of the infants. The GDS, developed by Gesell and Amatruda, is widely used for child neurodevelopmental diagnosis and intellectual evaluation worldwide [21]. The Chinese version of GDS modified by the Beijing Mental Development Cooperative Group was verified to have good validity and reliability in the Chinese population [22] and has been extensively used to diagnose neurodevelopmental delay among Chinese infants and children (0–6 years) [20, 23]. The Chinese version of GDS items is classified into five domains: gross motor, fine motor, adaptive, language, and social behavior. In brief, the gross motor includes raising head, rolling over, sitting, crawling, standing and walking; fine motor includes finger control and balance; adaptability contains sensation, perception, imitation, and discriminative performance; language is evaluated by pronunciation, word comprehension, and conversation; social behavior contains reactions to people and acquired information. For example, test items of language assessment within 6 months include burbling, calling loudly, vocalizing to people and objects, turning head when names are called, making “da-da” or “ma-ma” sounds, and imitating sounds, etc.
The score in each domain is expressed by developmental quotient (DQ), calculated as below: children’s estimated developmental age/ chronologic age× 100. According to the Chinese norm, the neurodevelopmental delay of each domain was defined as the DQ score below 85 [24]. Four standardized trained clinicians assessed neurodevelopmental tests of this study at the Developmental Neuropsychology Laboratory within 6 months. The evaluators were blind to the prenatal and perinatal characteristics of the infants.

Covariables

Information about the number of prenatal examinations, parity, mode of delivery, infant’s gender, birth weight, and neonatal asphyxia situation were derived from the delivery record. The socioeconomic factors such as maternal and paternal education level and infant’s feeding pattern were attained by questionnaire.

Statistical analysis

Mean and standard deviation or frequency and percentage were used to describe the characteristics of the study participants. For each infant, the DQ of the five domains was calculated, and neurodevelopment of each domain was classified into delay (DQ < 85) and normal (DQ ≥ 85) accordingly. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to compare the mean of DQ of each domain among different GA groups. The generalized linear model (GLM) was used to analyze the prediction of DQ associated with gestational age with adjustment potential confounding factors. Unconditional logistic regression was utilized to calculate the odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for the associations between GA and neurodevelopmental delay in each domain. In GLM and logistic regression analyses, we adjusted for the potential confounders, including parental education level, number of prenatal examinations, parity, mode of delivery, gender of infants, birth weight, neonatal asphyxia, and feeding pattern. In the further stratified analyses by infant’s gender, we evaluated the potential interactions between gender and GA by supplementing the relevant cross-product terms in the logistic regression models. All statistical analyses in this study were conducted using SAS, version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, North Carolina), and P-value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

The distribution of demographic characteristics of participants in this study is shown in Table 1. Among the 4243 participants, 155 (3.65%) were late preterm, 1288 (30.36%) were early term, and 2800 (65.99%) were full term. Of the infants, 2.88% were low birth weight (< 2500 g), and 5.87% were macrosomia (≥ 4000 g). 56.02% of the infants were boys, and 43.98% were girls. A total of 48 (1.13%) infants had asphyxia at birth. There were significant differences in parental education level, the number of prenatal examinations, parity, mode of delivery, infant’s gender, and birth weight among the three groups of late preterm, early term, and full term infants (P < 0.01).
Table 1
Distribution of demographic characteristics of participants
Variables
Late Preterm (34–36wks)
Early Term (37–38wks)
Full Term (39–41wks)
Total
P
n/mean
%/SD
n/mean
%/SD
n/mean
%/SD
n/mean
%/SD
Mothers’ education
        
< 0.01
 Below bachelor
113
72.90
847
65.76
1731
61.82
2691
63.42
 
 Bachelor or above
42
27.10
441
34.24
1069
38.18
1552
36.58
 
Fathers’ education
        
< 0.01
 Below bachelor
108
69.68
830
64.44
1641
58.61
2579
60.78
 
 Bachelor or above
47
30.32
458
35.56
1159
41.39
1664
39.22
 
Number of prenatal examinations
7.86
2.47
9.04
2.34
9.28
2.48
9.15
2.45
< 0.01
Parity
        
< 0.01
 1
127
81.94
1126
87.42
2630
93.93
3883
91.52
 
  ≥ 2
28
18.06
162
12.58
170
6.07
360
8.48
 
Mode of delivery
        
< 0.01
 Vaginal
54
34.84
289
22.44
717
25.61
1060
24.98
 
 Caesarean
101
65.16
999
77.56
2083
74.39
3183
75.02
 
Gender
        
< 0.01
 Boy
105
67.74
759
58.93
1513
54.04
2377
56.02
 
 Girl
50
32.26
529
41.07
1287
45.96
1866
43.98
 
Birth Weight (g)
        
< 0.01
  < 2500
64
41.29
39
3.03
19
0.68
122
2.88
 
 2500–3999.9
91
58.71
1213
94.18
2568
91.71
3872
91.26
 
  ≥ 4000
0
0.00
36
2.80
213
7.61
249
5.87
 
Neonatal asphyxia
         
 No
149
96.13
1275
98.99
2771
98.96
4195
98.87
< 0.01
 Yes
6
3.87
13
1.01
29
1.04
48
1.13
 
Feeding Pattern
        
0.09
 Breast feeding
86
55.48
784
60.87
1741
62.18
2611
61.54
 
 Mixed feeding
49
31.61
369
28.65
823
29.39
1241
29.25
 
 Powder milk
20
12.90
135
10.48
236
8.43
391
9.22
 
Table 2 presents late preterm birth and early term birth associations with DQ of different neurodevelopmental domains. The mean DQ of early term birth were 100.87, 105.63, 85.88, 87.63, and 88.13 in domains of gross motor, fine motor, adaptability, language, and social behavior, respectively, which were higher than those of late preterm infants (P < 0.01), and lower than full term infants (P < 0.01). The GLM analysis indicated that late preterm birth was significantly associated with decreased DQ in all domains: gross motor (β = − 17.42, 95% CI: − 21.15 to − 13.69), fine motor (β = − 23.61, 95% CI: − 28.52 to − 18.69), adaptability (β = − 10.10, 95% CI: − 13.82 to − 6.38), language (β = − 6.28, 95% CI: − 9.82 to − 2.74) and social behavior (β = − 5.99, 95% CI: − 9.59 to − 2.39). For early term birth, the statistically significant negative trend was only shown in the DQ of fine motor (β = − 2.01, 95% CI − 3.93 to − 0.09).
Table 2
Associations of late preterm birth and early term birth with DQ of different neurodevelopmental domains
Variables
DQ
P*
β
95% CI for β
P**
mean
SD
Lower
Upper
Gross motor DQ
 Late Preterm
81.66
22.48
< 0.01
−17.42
−21.15
−13.69
< 0.01
 Early Term
100.87
21.64
 
−0.25
−1.70
1.21
0.74
 Full Term
101.64
21.82
 
0.00
   
Fine motor DQ
 Late Preterm
82.54
20.89
< 0.01
−23.61
−28.52
−18.69
< 0.01
 Early Term
105.63
28.93
 
−2.01
−3.93
−0.09
0.04
 Full Term
108.14
28.95
 
0.00
   
Adaptability DQ
 Late Preterm
75.94
21.55
< 0.01
−10.10
−13.82
−6.38
< 0.01
 Early Term
85.88
21.54
 
−0.41
−1.87
1.04
0.58
 Full Term
86.49
21.75
 
0.00
   
Language DQ
 Late Preterm
81.21
22.78
< 0.01
−6.28
−9.82
−2.74
< 0.01
 Early Term
87.63
20.17
 
−0.59
−1.98
0.79
0.40
 Full Term
88.40
20.73
 
0.00
   
Social behavior development DQ
 Late Preterm
81.75
22.56
< 0.01
−5.99
−9.59
−2.39
< 0.01
 Early Term
88.13
20.12
 
−0.89
−2.29
0.52
0.22
 Full Term
89.16
21.28
 
0.00
   
P*, one-way analysis of variance; P**, generalized linear model analysis, adjusted for parental education level, the number of prenatal examinations, parity, mode of delivery, infant’s gender, birth weight, neonatal asphyxia and feeding pattern
Results of neurodevelopmental delay in different domains and the association with late preterm birth/early term birth are presented in Table 3. The neurodevelopmental delay rates among late preterm birth and early term birth infants were 56.77 and 22.67% in the gross motor domain, 56.13 and 29.04% in fine motor domain, 64.52 and 55.75% in adaptability domain, 52.26, and 50.85% in the language domain, 49.03 and 48.84% in social behavior development domain, respectively. They were all higher than the corresponding rates among full term infants. According to the results of the binary logistic regression model adjusted for potential confounders, late preterm birth infants had a significantly elevated risk of neurodevelopmental delay in domains of gross motor (adjusted OR = 3.82, 95% CI: 2.67 to 5.46), fine motor (adjusted OR = 3.51, 95% CI: 2.47 to 5.01) and adaptability (adjusted OR = 1.60, 95% CI: 1.12 to 2.29), whereas early term birth was significantly associated with delayed neurodevelopment in fine motor (adjusted OR = 1.22, 95% CI: 1.05 to 1.42).
Table 3
Associations of late preterm birth and early term birth with neurodevelopmental delay of different domains
Variables
Neurodevelopment
Crude OR (95% CI)
Adjusted ORa
(95% CI)
Normal
Delay
n
%
n
%
Gross motor
 Late Preterm
67
43.23
88
56.77
4.74 (3.40–6.59)
3.82 (2.67–5.46)
 Early Term
996
77.33
292
22.67
1.06 (0.90–1.24)
1.01 (0.86–1.19)
 Full Term
2192
78.29
608
21.71
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
Fine motor
 Late Preterm
68
43.87
87
56.13
3.88 (2.80–5.39)
3.51 (2.47–5.01)
 Early Term
914
70.96
374
29.04
1.24 (1.07–1.44)
1.22 (1.05–1.42)
 Full Term
2106
75.21
694
24.79
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
Adaptability
 Late Preterm
55
35.48
100
64.52
1.55 (1.10–2.17)
1.60 (1.12–2.29)
 Early Term
570
44.25
718
55.75
1.07 (0.94–1.22)
1.08 (0.94–1.23)
 Full Term
1287
45.96
1513
54.04
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
Language
 Late Preterm
74
47.74
81
52.26
1.10 (0.79–1.52)
1.12 (0.79–1.58)
 Early Term
633
49.15
655
50.85
1.04 (0.91–1.18)
1.05 (0.92–1.20)
 Full Term
1402
50.07
1398
49.93
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
Social behavior development
 Late Preterm
79
50.97
76
49.03
1.04 (0.75–1.43)
1.04 (0.74–1.47)
 Early Term
659
51.16
629
48.84
1.03 (0.90–1.17)
1.05 (0.91–1.20)
 Full Term
1451
51.82
1349
48.18
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
aAdjusted for parental education level, number of prenatal examinations, parity, mode of delivery, infant’s gender, birth weight, neonatal asphyxia and feeding pattern
We further explored the associations of late preterm birth and early term birth with neurodevelopment delay of different domains stratified by infants’ gender. As shown in Table 4, late preterm boys had a higher risk of neurodevelopment delay in three domains: gross motor (adjusted OR = 4.30, 95% CI: 2.75 to 6.71), fine motor (adjusted OR = 3.86, 95% CI: 2.49 to 5.99), and adaptability (adjusted OR = 1.64, 95% CI: 1.06 to 2.55); late preterm girls had a higher risk of neurodevelopment delay in 2 domains: gross motor (adjusted OR = 2.94, 95% CI: 1.59 to 5.44) and fine motor (adjusted OR = 2.91, 95% CI: 1.59 to 5.34). As for early term infants, the significant association was only found with fine motor development delay among girls (adjusted OR = 1.32, 95% CI: 1.05 to 1.65). The crude OR showed that late preterm birth was significantly associated with language delay among girls (crude OR = 1.81, 95% CI: 1.00 to 3.28). However, the association was no longer significant after adjusting for the potential confounding factors.
Table 4
Associations of late preterm birth and early term birth with neurodevelopment delay of different domains stratified by gender
Variables
Boys
Girls
P for heterogeneity
Crude OR
(95% CI)
Adjusted ORa
(95% CI)
Crude OR
(95% CI)
Adjusted ORa
(95% CI)
Gross motor
 Late Preterm
5.25 (3.49–7.89)
4.30 (2.75–6.71)
3.86 (2.18–6.83)
2.94 (1.59–5.44)
0.41
 Early Term
1.02 (0.83–1.26)
0.97 (0.79–1.21)
1.11 (0.88–1.42)
1.07 (0.84–1.36)
 
 Full Term
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
 
Fine motor
 Late Preterm
4.19 (2.80–6.28)
3.86 (2.49–5.99)
3.42 (1.93–6.05)
2.91 (1.59–5.34)
0.96
 Early Term
1.16 (0.95–1.42)
1.15 (0.94–1.41)
1.37 (1.10–1.71)
1.32 (1.05–1.65)
 
 Full Term
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
 
Adaptability
 Late Preterm
1.64 (1.08–2.48)
1.64 (1.06–2.55)
1.38 (0.77–2.47)
1.51 (0.82–2.79)
0.89
 Early Term
1.05 (0.88–1.25)
1.05 (0.87–1.25)
1.10 (0.90–1.35)
1.12 (0.91–1.37)
 
 Full Term
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
 
Language
 Late Preterm
0.90 (0.60–1.34)
0.93 (0.61–1.42)
1.81 (1.00–3.28)
1.68 (0.90–3.13)
0.05
 Early Term
0.98 (0.82–1.16)
0.98 (0.82–1.17)
1.15 (0.94–1.41)
1.15 (0.94–1.42)
 
 Full Term
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
 
Social behavior development
 Late Preterm
1.19 (0.80–1.76)
1.10 (0.72–1.68)
0.81 (0.46–1.43)
0.88 (0.48–1.60)
0.76
 Early Term
1.01 (0.85–1.20)
1.01 (0.84–1.20)
1.06 (0.87–1.30)
1.10 (0.89–1.35)
 
 Full Term
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
 
aAdjusted for parental education level, number of prenatal examinations, parity, mode of delivery, infant’s gender, birth weight, neonatal asphyxia and feeding pattern
Table 5 shows the association of the specific gestational week with neurodevelopment delay of different domains. After adjusting for potential confounders, infants born between 34 and 37 weeks of gestation showed a higher risk of gross motor and fine motor development delay, and infants born between 34 and 35 weeks of gestation also had a higher risk of adaptability development delay. No significant association was found for language or social behavior development delay.
Table 5
Associations of gestational weeks with neurodevelopment delay of different domainsa
Variables
Gross motor
Fine motor
Adaptability
Language
Social behavior development
34 wks
9.45 (3.69–24.21)
14.78 (4.98–43.84)
5.03 (1.70–14.86)
1.90 (0.83–4.35)
2.00 (0.87–4.59)
35 wks
3.83 (2.07–7.08)
4.24 (2.29–7.84)
2.92 (1.48–5.78)
1.79 (0.97–3.31)
1.19 (0.65–2.15)
36 wks
3.14 (1.99–4.96)
2.38 (1.51–3.75)
0.95 (0.60–1.49)
0.76 (0.48–1.19)
0.81 (0.51–1.27)
37 wks
1.49 (1.14–1.96)
1.72 (1.33–2.24)
1.28 (0.99–1.65)
1.18 (0.92–1.51)
1.10 (0.86–1.40)
38 wks
0.90 (0.75–1.07)
1.11 (0.94–1.31)
1.03 (0.89–1.19)
1.02 (0.89–1.19)
1.04 (0.89–1.20)
39-41wks
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
1.00 (reference)
aAdjusted for parental education level, number of prenatal examinations, parity, mode of delivery, infant’s gender, birth weight, neonatal asphyxia and feeding pattern

Discussion

In this study, we explored the impact of early term and late preterm birth on the neurodevelopment of infants within 6 months. Our results indicated that late preterm birth was associated with decreased DQ in all neurological domains, and early term birth was associated with decreased DQ in the fine motor. Moreover, late preterm birth had an elevated risk of neurodevelopmental delay in gross motor, fine motor, and adaptability domains. In contrast, early term birth only showed an increased risk of developmental delay in the fine motor.
According to the Fetal Origins Hypothesis, the fetal intrauterine environment, associated with gestational length, has profound and long-term effects on children’s health and development [25]. Our current study showed that late preterm and early term infants exhibited poorer neurodevelopmental levels within 6 months compared with full term infants, enriching the evidence of the association between gestational length and neurodevelopment among children at the early stage. This result is consistent with previous studies conducted among older children [14, 26, 27]. A prospective longitudinal cohort study in Australia found that compared with term infants, moderate and late preterm children had a higher risk of developmental delay in cognitive, language, and motor domains at the age of 2 years [14]. A cohort study in China also suggested that early term birth was associated with an increased risk of delayed neurodevelopment in the psychomotor domain at 2 years old [17]. Neurodevelopmental delay in late preterm infants may be related to brain morphology and structure [28]. Late preterm infants have been reported to have less brain volume and lower differentiated myelination and neural connectivity than full term infants [29, 30]. It has been suggested that the period between 340/7 and 400/7 gestational weeks is a critical period for immature brain growth, in which cortical and white matter volumes are multiplied by two and five, respectively [31]. While early term birth between 370/7 and 380/7 gestational weeks is included in the critical period, the association between early term birth and neurodevelopmental delay may be also explained [31].
Additionally, the prior study has shown a linear correlation between gestational age and birth fetal brain volume [32], supporting the present findings that longer gestation was associated with improved neurodevelopment at the early stage in infants. In line with these findings, a prospective study investigating 232 full term infants in Germany also found that infants with longer gestation had better cognitive and motor development within 1 year old [26]. Moreover, another study in Chilean also reported that the longer pregnancy was associated with higher mental and motor scores of BSID among term infants at 1 year of age [33]. It is suggested that longer gestation can benefit the infants’ neurodevelopment. Specifically, our results showed that infants born at 37 gestational weeks had a significantly elevated risk of neurodevelopment delay in gross motor and fine motor, compared with full term birth infants. This result supports the arguments of the conventional cut-off of 37 gestational weeks for preterm birth. However, taking gestational age as a continuum representing the risk level connected with adverse outcomes may be more appropriate than taking the term and preterm birth as a dichotomy [34]. More attention should be paid to the consequences of gestational length among term infants.
The present study found that late preterm birth mainly influenced infants’ neurodevelopmental delay of gross motor, fine motor, and adaptability during the early stage. In contrast, early term birth was associated with neurodevelopmental delay of fine motor. These findings may suggest that there may be different potential mechanisms under the impact of gestational age for different neurodevelopmental domains, and different neurodevelopmental modules may have different developmental critical periods. It has been shown that sensory axons are usually myelinated before motor axons, and myelination generally extends from the center to the periphery, from the dorsal to the ventral [35]. It seems that the sensory module may develop before the motor module and the gross motor module before the fine motor module. Our results suggest no significant difference between gestational age and neurodevelopmental delay of language at the early stage, which was inconsistent with results of some previous studies [36]. The inconsistency may be related to the physiological age of infants. Unlike most studies focused on the neurodevelopment of infants at least one-year-old, the present study was devoted to assessing the neurodevelopmental level within 6 months. In fact, to the best of our knowledge, there are a growing number of studies evaluating neurodevelopment in infants around 6 months of age [3739]. A study had shown that infants with neurodevelopmental delays would have better neurodevelopmental outcomes in future if interventions were given at 6 months of age [40]. This might highlight the importance of early identification and intervention of high-risk infants to promote an improved neurodevelopmental outcome.
Further, we investigated the influence of late preterm birth and early term birth with neurodevelopmental delay stratified by infants’ gender. Late preterm birth was associated with a higher risk of delay in 3 domains (gross motor, fine motor, and adaptability) in boys. In contrast, adaptability developmental delay was not observed among late preterm girls. Moreover, the association of early term birth with fine motor developmental delay was only detected in girls. Prior studies have shown that fetal gender may play an essential role in regulating the influence of prenatal exposure on neurodevelopment [41, 42]. Male fetuses are more sensitive to early exposure to risk factors than female fetuses [43]. Wu et al. found that the association between early term birth and the neurodevelopmental delay was more significant in male infants [17]. The results indicated that the impact and mechanism of gestational age on neurodevelopment at an early stage could be different for boys and girls, which needs to be examined and identified in the future.
The main strength of this study is the relatively large sample size compared with previous studies, which included information collected from pregnancy through 6 months after birth. Moreover, we were able to adjust for some potential confounding factors that may impact infants’ neurodevelopment in the present study. Furthermore, the assessment of infants’ neurodevelopment was under strict quality control, making the results more reliable. However, several limitations of this study need to be concerned. First, infants’ neurodevelopment was only assessed within 6 months in this study. As individual neuropsychological development is a long-term process and can be affected by various biological and psychosocial factors, further follow-up is required to identify the neurodevelopmental status of early term and late preterm birth infants in the future. Second, the participants in this study came from a single center of tertiary hospital, limiting our findings’ generalizability to the broader population. Third, although the revised GDS is widely used for neurodevelopmental assessment and diagnosis in China, it is not commonly used in western countries, which increases the difficulty of comparing the results among different studies worldwide.

Conclusion

In conclusion, we conducted a prospective cohort study in Wuhan, China to evaluate the impact of early term and late preterm birth on infants’ neurodevelopment at the early stage, and found the evidence that late preterm birth mainly elevated the risk of neurodevelopmental delay of gross motor, fine motor, and adaptability. In contrast, early term birth was associated with an increased risk of fine motor developmental delay within 6 months. We found that longer gestation benefits the infants’ gross and fine motor development at an early stage even among term infants. Further research is needed to determine the effectiveness and necessity of the interventions at the early stage for early term and late preterm infants who had suspected neurodevelopmental delay.

Acknowledgements

We are extremely grateful to all the participating families, as well as the pediatricians, nurses and other staffs involved in this study.

Permission and/or credit for reproduced images

Not applicable.

Declarations

This study was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of Wuhan Children’s Hospital (Wuhan Maternal and Child Healthcare Hospital) (No.2010009).
Informed consent was obtained from the mothers of infants at their first time of antenatal examination.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Spong CY. Defining "term" pregnancy: recommendations from the defining "term" pregnancy workgroup. JAMA. 2013;309(23):2445–6.PubMedCrossRef Spong CY. Defining "term" pregnancy: recommendations from the defining "term" pregnancy workgroup. JAMA. 2013;309(23):2445–6.PubMedCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Stewart DL, Barfield WD, Committee On F. Updates on an at-risk population: late-preterm and early-term infants. Pediatrics. 2019;144(5):e20192760.PubMedCrossRef Stewart DL, Barfield WD, Committee On F. Updates on an at-risk population: late-preterm and early-term infants. Pediatrics. 2019;144(5):e20192760.PubMedCrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Odibo IN, Bird TM, McKelvey SS, et al. Childhood respiratory morbidity after late preterm and early term delivery: a study of Medicaid patients in South Carolina. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2016;30(1):67–75.PubMedCrossRef Odibo IN, Bird TM, McKelvey SS, et al. Childhood respiratory morbidity after late preterm and early term delivery: a study of Medicaid patients in South Carolina. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2016;30(1):67–75.PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Paz LD, Sheiner E, Wainstock T, et al. Evidence that children born at early term (37-38 6/7 weeks) are at increased risk for diabetes and obesity-related disorders. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2017;217(5):588. Paz LD, Sheiner E, Wainstock T, et al. Evidence that children born at early term (37-38 6/7 weeks) are at increased risk for diabetes and obesity-related disorders. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2017;217(5):588.
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Dong Y, Chen SJ, Yu JL. A systematic review and meta-analysis of long-term development of early term infants. Neonatology. 2012;102(3):212–21.PubMedCrossRef Dong Y, Chen SJ, Yu JL. A systematic review and meta-analysis of long-term development of early term infants. Neonatology. 2012;102(3):212–21.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Crump C, Sundquist K, Winkleby MA, et al. Early-term birth (37-38 weeks) and mortality in young adulthood. Epidemiology. 2013;24(2):270–6.PubMedCrossRef Crump C, Sundquist K, Winkleby MA, et al. Early-term birth (37-38 weeks) and mortality in young adulthood. Epidemiology. 2013;24(2):270–6.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Stoll BJ, Hansen NI, Bell EF, et al. Trends in care practices, morbidity, and mortality of extremely preterm neonates, 1993-2012. JAMA. 2015;314(10):1039–51.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Stoll BJ, Hansen NI, Bell EF, et al. Trends in care practices, morbidity, and mortality of extremely preterm neonates, 1993-2012. JAMA. 2015;314(10):1039–51.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Huff K, Rose RS, Engle WA. Late preterm infants: morbidities, mortality, and management recommendations. Pediatr Clin N Am. 2019;66(2):387–402.CrossRef Huff K, Rose RS, Engle WA. Late preterm infants: morbidities, mortality, and management recommendations. Pediatr Clin N Am. 2019;66(2):387–402.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Woythaler M. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of the late preterm infant. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2019;24(1):54–9.PubMedCrossRef Woythaler M. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of the late preterm infant. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2019;24(1):54–9.PubMedCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Chawanpaiboon S, Vogel JP, Moller AB, et al. Global, regional, and national estimates of levels of preterm birth in 2014: a systematic review and modelling analysis. Lancet Glob Health. 2019;7(1):e37–46.PubMedCrossRef Chawanpaiboon S, Vogel JP, Moller AB, et al. Global, regional, and national estimates of levels of preterm birth in 2014: a systematic review and modelling analysis. Lancet Glob Health. 2019;7(1):e37–46.PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Delnord M, Zeitlin J. Epidemiology of late preterm and early term births - an international perspective. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2019;24(1):3–10.PubMedCrossRef Delnord M, Zeitlin J. Epidemiology of late preterm and early term births - an international perspective. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2019;24(1):3–10.PubMedCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Woythaler MA, McCormick MC, Smith VC. Late preterm infants have worse 24-month neurodevelopmental outcomes than term infants. Pediatrics. 2011;127(3):e622–9.PubMedCrossRef Woythaler MA, McCormick MC, Smith VC. Late preterm infants have worse 24-month neurodevelopmental outcomes than term infants. Pediatrics. 2011;127(3):e622–9.PubMedCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Cheong JL, Doyle LW, Burnett AC, et al. Association between moderate and late preterm birth and neurodevelopment and social-emotional development at age 2 years. JAMA Pediatr. 2017;171(4):e164805.PubMedCrossRef Cheong JL, Doyle LW, Burnett AC, et al. Association between moderate and late preterm birth and neurodevelopment and social-emotional development at age 2 years. JAMA Pediatr. 2017;171(4):e164805.PubMedCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat McGowan EC, Vohr BR. Neurodevelopmental follow-up of preterm infants: what is new? Pediatr Clin N Am. 2019;66(2):509–23.CrossRef McGowan EC, Vohr BR. Neurodevelopmental follow-up of preterm infants: what is new? Pediatr Clin N Am. 2019;66(2):509–23.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Stene-Larsen K, Brandlistuen RE, Lang AM, et al. Communication impairments in early term and late preterm children: a prospective cohort study following children to age 36 months. J Pediatr. 2014;165(6):1123–8.PubMedCrossRef Stene-Larsen K, Brandlistuen RE, Lang AM, et al. Communication impairments in early term and late preterm children: a prospective cohort study following children to age 36 months. J Pediatr. 2014;165(6):1123–8.PubMedCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Wu M, Wang L, Liu Y, et al. Association between early-term birth and delayed neurodevelopment at the age of 2 years: results from a cohort study in China. Eur J Pediatr. 2021;180(12):3509–17.PubMedCrossRef Wu M, Wang L, Liu Y, et al. Association between early-term birth and delayed neurodevelopment at the age of 2 years: results from a cohort study in China. Eur J Pediatr. 2021;180(12):3509–17.PubMedCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Vohr B. Long-term outcomes of moderately preterm, late preterm, and early term infants. Clin Perinatol. 2013;40(4):739–51.PubMedCrossRef Vohr B. Long-term outcomes of moderately preterm, late preterm, and early term infants. Clin Perinatol. 2013;40(4):739–51.PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Kajantie E, Strang-Karlsson S, Evensen KAI, et al. Adult outcomes of being born late preterm or early term - what do we know? Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2019;24(1):66–83.PubMedCrossRef Kajantie E, Strang-Karlsson S, Evensen KAI, et al. Adult outcomes of being born late preterm or early term - what do we know? Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2019;24(1):66–83.PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Doll E, Gesell A, Amatruda CS. Developmental diagnosis: Normal and abnormal child development—clinical methods and pediatric applications. Ann Intern Med. 1948;28:195. Doll E, Gesell A, Amatruda CS. Developmental diagnosis: Normal and abnormal child development—clinical methods and pediatric applications. Ann Intern Med. 1948;28:195.
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Xiuling Z, L J. The revise of Gesell developmental scale on ~ 6 years of age in Beijing. Chinese. J Clin Psychol. 1994;3:03. Xiuling Z, L J. The revise of Gesell developmental scale on ~ 6 years of age in Beijing. Chinese. J Clin Psychol. 1994;3:03.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang M, Gazimbi MM, Chen Z, et al. Association between birth weight and neurodevelopment at age 1-6 months: results from the Wuhan healthy baby cohort. BMJ Open. 2020;10(1):e031916.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Zhang M, Gazimbi MM, Chen Z, et al. Association between birth weight and neurodevelopment at age 1-6 months: results from the Wuhan healthy baby cohort. BMJ Open. 2020;10(1):e031916.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Yang YF. Rating scales for Children’s developmental behavior and mental health. 1st ed. Peking: People’s Medical Publishing House; 2016. Yang YF. Rating scales for Children’s developmental behavior and mental health. 1st ed. Peking: People’s Medical Publishing House; 2016.
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Sandman CA, Davis EP, Buss C, et al. Exposure to prenatal psychobiological stress exerts programming influences on the mother and her fetus. Neuroendocrinology. 2012;95(1):7–21.PubMedCrossRef Sandman CA, Davis EP, Buss C, et al. Exposure to prenatal psychobiological stress exerts programming influences on the mother and her fetus. Neuroendocrinology. 2012;95(1):7–21.PubMedCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Espel EV, Glynn LM, Sandman CA, et al. Longer gestation among children born full term influences cognitive and motor development. PLoS One. 2014;9(11):e113758.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Espel EV, Glynn LM, Sandman CA, et al. Longer gestation among children born full term influences cognitive and motor development. PLoS One. 2014;9(11):e113758.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Shah P, Kaciroti N, Richards B, et al. Developmental outcomes of late preterm infants from infancy to kindergarten. Pediatrics. 2016;138(2):e20153496.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Shah P, Kaciroti N, Richards B, et al. Developmental outcomes of late preterm infants from infancy to kindergarten. Pediatrics. 2016;138(2):e20153496.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Kelly CE, Thompson DK, Spittle AJ, et al. Regional brain volumes, microstructure and neurodevelopment in moderate-late preterm children. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2020;105(6):593–9.PubMedCrossRef Kelly CE, Thompson DK, Spittle AJ, et al. Regional brain volumes, microstructure and neurodevelopment in moderate-late preterm children. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2020;105(6):593–9.PubMedCrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Walsh JM, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, et al. Moderate and late preterm birth: effect on brain size and maturation at term-equivalent age. Radiology. 2014;273(1):232–40.PubMedCrossRef Walsh JM, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, et al. Moderate and late preterm birth: effect on brain size and maturation at term-equivalent age. Radiology. 2014;273(1):232–40.PubMedCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Oros D, Altermir I, Elia N, et al. Pathways of neuronal and cognitive development in children born small-for-gestational age or late preterm. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2014;43(1):41–7.PubMedCrossRef Oros D, Altermir I, Elia N, et al. Pathways of neuronal and cognitive development in children born small-for-gestational age or late preterm. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2014;43(1):41–7.PubMedCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Hüppi PS, Warfield S, Kikinis R, et al. Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of brain development in premature and mature newborns. Ann Neurol. 1998;43(2):224–35.PubMedCrossRef Hüppi PS, Warfield S, Kikinis R, et al. Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of brain development in premature and mature newborns. Ann Neurol. 1998;43(2):224–35.PubMedCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat El Marroun H, Zou R, Leeuwenburg MF, et al. Association of Gestational age at birth with brain Morphometry. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(12):1149–58.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef El Marroun H, Zou R, Leeuwenburg MF, et al. Association of Gestational age at birth with brain Morphometry. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(12):1149–58.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Rose O, Blanco E, Martinez SM, et al. Developmental scores at 1 year with increasing gestational age, 37-41 weeks. Pediatrics. 2013;131(5):e1475–81.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Rose O, Blanco E, Martinez SM, et al. Developmental scores at 1 year with increasing gestational age, 37-41 weeks. Pediatrics. 2013;131(5):e1475–81.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
34.
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Barkovich MJ, Barkovich AJ. MR imaging of Normal brain development. Neuroimaging Clin N Am. 2019;29(3):325–37.PubMedCrossRef Barkovich MJ, Barkovich AJ. MR imaging of Normal brain development. Neuroimaging Clin N Am. 2019;29(3):325–37.PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Zerbeto AB, Cortelo FM, EB CF. Association between gestational age and birth weight on the language development of Brazilian children: a systematic review. J Pediatr. 2015;91(4):326–32.CrossRef Zerbeto AB, Cortelo FM, EB CF. Association between gestational age and birth weight on the language development of Brazilian children: a systematic review. J Pediatr. 2015;91(4):326–32.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Yim G, Minatoya M, Kioumourtzoglou MA, et al. The associations of prenatal exposure to dioxins and polychlorinated biphenyls with neurodevelopment at 6 months of age: multi-pollutant approaches. Environ Res. 2022;209:112757.PubMedCrossRef Yim G, Minatoya M, Kioumourtzoglou MA, et al. The associations of prenatal exposure to dioxins and polychlorinated biphenyls with neurodevelopment at 6 months of age: multi-pollutant approaches. Environ Res. 2022;209:112757.PubMedCrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Shekhawat DS, Janu VC, Singh P, et al. Association of newborn blood lead concentration with neurodevelopment outcome in early infancy. J Trace Elem Med Biol. 2021;68:126853.PubMedCrossRef Shekhawat DS, Janu VC, Singh P, et al. Association of newborn blood lead concentration with neurodevelopment outcome in early infancy. J Trace Elem Med Biol. 2021;68:126853.PubMedCrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Sanefuji M, Sonoda Y, Ito Y, et al. Physical growth and neurodevelopment during the first year of life: a cohort study of the Japan environment and Children's study. BMC Pediatr. 2021;21(1):360.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Sanefuji M, Sonoda Y, Ito Y, et al. Physical growth and neurodevelopment during the first year of life: a cohort study of the Japan environment and Children's study. BMC Pediatr. 2021;21(1):360.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu TY, Chang JH, Peng CC, et al. Predictive validity of the Bayley-III cognitive scores at 6 months for cognitive outcomes at 24 months in very-low-birth-weight infants. Front Pediatr. 2021;9:638449.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Liu TY, Chang JH, Peng CC, et al. Predictive validity of the Bayley-III cognitive scores at 6 months for cognitive outcomes at 24 months in very-low-birth-weight infants. Front Pediatr. 2021;9:638449.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Massey SH, Mroczek DK, Reiss D, et al. Additive drug-specific and sex-specific risks associated with co-use of marijuana and tobacco during pregnancy: evidence from 3 recent developmental cohorts (2003-2015). Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2018;68:97–106.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Massey SH, Mroczek DK, Reiss D, et al. Additive drug-specific and sex-specific risks associated with co-use of marijuana and tobacco during pregnancy: evidence from 3 recent developmental cohorts (2003-2015). Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2018;68:97–106.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Benevenuto SG, Domenico MD, Martins MA, et al. Recreational use of marijuana during pregnancy and negative gestational and fetal outcomes: an experimental study in mice. Toxicology. 2017;376:94–101.PubMedCrossRef Benevenuto SG, Domenico MD, Martins MA, et al. Recreational use of marijuana during pregnancy and negative gestational and fetal outcomes: an experimental study in mice. Toxicology. 2017;376:94–101.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Impact of early term and late preterm birth on infants’ neurodevelopment: evidence from a cohort study in Wuhan, China
verfasst von
Zhong Chen
Chao Xiong
Hua Liu
Junyu Duan
Chun Kang
Cong Yao
Kai Chen
Yawen Chen
Yan Liu
Mingzhu Liu
Aifen Zhou
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2022
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Pediatrics / Ausgabe 1/2022
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2431
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-022-03312-3

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2022

BMC Pediatrics 1/2022 Zur Ausgabe

Darf man die Behandlung eines Neonazis ablehnen?

08.05.2024 Gesellschaft Nachrichten

In einer Leseranfrage in der Zeitschrift Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology möchte ein anonymer Dermatologe bzw. eine anonyme Dermatologin wissen, ob er oder sie einen Patienten behandeln muss, der eine rassistische Tätowierung trägt.

Ein Drittel der jungen Ärztinnen und Ärzte erwägt abzuwandern

07.05.2024 Klinik aktuell Nachrichten

Extreme Arbeitsverdichtung und kaum Supervision: Dr. Andrea Martini, Sprecherin des Bündnisses Junge Ärztinnen und Ärzte (BJÄ) über den Frust des ärztlichen Nachwuchses und die Vorteile des Rucksack-Modells.

Häufigste Gründe für Brustschmerzen bei Kindern

06.05.2024 Pädiatrische Diagnostik Nachrichten

Akute Brustschmerzen sind ein Alarmsymptom par exellence, schließlich sind manche Auslöser lebensbedrohlich. Auch Kinder klagen oft über Schmerzen in der Brust. Ein Studienteam ist den Ursachen nachgegangen.

Endlich: Zi zeigt, mit welchen PVS Praxen zufrieden sind

IT für Ärzte Nachrichten

Darauf haben viele Praxen gewartet: Das Zi hat eine Liste von Praxisverwaltungssystemen veröffentlicht, die von Nutzern positiv bewertet werden. Eine gute Grundlage für wechselwillige Ärztinnen und Psychotherapeuten.

Update Pädiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.