Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Cardiovascular Disorders 1/2013

Open Access 01.12.2013 | Research article

Cardiovascular fitness associated with cognitive performance in heart failure patients enrolled in cardiac rehabilitation

verfasst von: Sarah Garcia, Michael L Alosco, Mary Beth Spitznagel, Ronald Cohen, Naftali Raz, Lawrence Sweet, Richard Josephson, Joel Hughes, Jim Rosneck, Morgan L Oberle, John Gunstad

Erschienen in: BMC Cardiovascular Disorders | Ausgabe 1/2013

Abstract

Background

Reduced cognitive function is common in persons with heart failure (HF). Cardiovascular fitness is a known contributor to cognitive function in many patient populations, but has only been linked to cognition based on estimates of fitness in HF. The current study examined the relationship between fitness as measured by metabolic equivalents (METs) from a standardized stress test and cognition in persons with HF, as well as the validity of office-based predictors of fitness in this population.

Methods

Forty-one HF patients enrolled in cardiac rehabilitation completed a standardized exercise stress test protocol, a brief neuropsychological battery, the 2-minute step test (2MST), and a series of medical history and self-report questionnaires.

Results

Maximum METs from stress testing demonstrated incremental predictive validity for attention (β = .41, p = .03), executive function (β = .37, p = .04), and memory domains (β = .46, p = .04). Partial correlations accounting for key medical and demographic characteristics revealed greater METs was associated with the 2MST (r (32) = .41, p = .02) but not with the Duke Activity Status Index (DASI) (r(32) = .24, p = .17).

Conclusion

The current findings indicate that better fitness levels measured by METs is independently associated with better cognitive function in older adults with HF. Results also showed that METs was closely associated with one office-based measure of fitness (2MST), but not another (DASI). Prospective studies are needed to clarify the mechanisms linking fitness and cognitive function in HF.
Hinweise

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

SG contributed to study concept and design, acquisition of subjects, data analysis and interpretation, and manuscript preparation. MA participated in study concept and design, acquisition of subjects, data analysis and interpretation, and manuscript preparation. MS, RC, NR, LS, LC, RJ, JH, JR, LO, and JG contributed to contributed to study concept and design, data analysis and interpretation, and manuscript preparation. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Abkürzungen
HF
Heart failure
METs
Metabolic equivalents
2MST
2 minute step test
DASI
Duke activity status index
LVEF
Left ventricular ejection fraction
3MS
Modified mini mental status exam
LNS
Letter number sequencing
TMT-A
Trail making test A
TMT-B
Trail making test B
CVLT
California verbal learning test
CFT
Complex figure test
BNT-II
Boston naming test-II
CBF
Cerebral blood flow

Background

Heart failure (HF) affects nearly six million Americans and is a leading cause of hospitalization and mortality in older adults [1, 2]. Though less commonly examined than other consequences, cognitive impairment is also common in HF and believed to affect up to 75% of this population [3]. Deficits can be found in all cognitive domains, but are particularly pronounced on tests of executive function, attention, and memory [4, 5]. Deficits in these cognitive domains can have adverse impact on patient outcomes, including reducing the ability to comply with medical regimen (e.g. medications, exercise, diet) and recognize symptoms of decreasing health [4].
Past studies have shown that reduced cerebral blood flow [6, 7] and structural brain changes including greater global and region-specific atrophy [8, 9], are important factors for the development of cognitive impairment in HF. Low physical fitness is another likely contributor to poor cognitive performance in persons with HF [10, 11]. Only one previous study has examined the association between fitness and cognitive function in HF [12]. Findings were consistent with expectations, with poorer fitness levels being associated with greater cognitive impairment. However, that study utilized an office-based estimate of fitness, the 2 minute step test, rather than a more detailed assessment such as stress testing. Furthermore, past work has shown a strong relationship between cognitive dysfunction and decreased fitness in other populations, including chronic obstructive pulmonary disorder, [13] depression, [14] and healthy older adults [15].
Metabolic equivalents (METS) are often used to illustrate the intensity of tasks individuals are able to accomplish, providing a standardized definition of fitness. As such, METS are important constructs in understanding the fitness of HF patients and determining the types of activities they are able to do independently. Found to be valid estimates in other populations [1618], both the Duke Activity Status Index (DASI) and the 2-minute step test are commonly used with HF patients to estimate METS. While these estimates have good clinical function, it has yet to be determined how well they estimate fitness in the HF population. Likewise, no study has yet examined the independent contribution of METS on cognitive function in HF patients.
Given these findings, the proposed study had two primary goals. First, we sought to clarify the possible association between fitness levels as measured by stress testing and cognitive function in persons with HF. Second, to examine the validity of office-based estimates of fitness by correlating them to performance on stress testing. Specifically, we examined the association between stress testing and the self report Duke Activity Status Index (DASI) and the objective 2-minute step test. Based on past literature in other populations, we hypothesized that metabolic equivalents (METs) would independently predict cognitive function in multiple domains. Based on their association with other measures of fitness, we also expected that office-based measures of fitness, specifically the 2-minute step test (2MST) and the Duke Activity Status Index (DASI), would be correlated to cognitive performance.

Methods

Participants

A sample of 41 older adults with HF was recruited from Summa Health System in Akron, OH. See Table 1. All participants were between 50-85 years of age, English-speaking, and had an established diagnosis of New York Heart Association class II or III at the time of enrollment. Exclusion criteria included a history of significant neurological disorder (e.g. dementia, stroke), head injury, severe psychiatric disorder (e.g. schizophrenia, bipolar disorder), substance use, and renal failure.
Table 1
Demographic and clinical characteristics of 41 older adults with heart failure
Demographic characteristics
 
Age, mean (SD) years
68.34 (8.41)
Gender (% Women)
34.1
Race, (% Caucasian)
85.4
Years of Education, mean (SD)
13.59 (3.04)
Clinical characteristics
 
LVEF (%)
39.00 (10.79)
Hypertension (% positive history)
68.3
Diabetes (% positive history)
34.1
BDI-II, mean (SD)
6.83(8.44)
Physical fitness
 
METS, mean (SD)
6.24(311)
2MST, mean (SD)
69.56 (23.07)
DASI peak oxygen uptake (mL/min), mean (SD)
26.32 (6.17)
Note. LVEF Left Ventricular Ejection Fraction, BDI-II Beck Depression Inventory-II, METS Metabolic Equivalent Task; 2MST 2-Minute Step Test; DASI Duke Activity Status Index.

Instrumentation

Neuropsychological tests

A neuropsychological battery was conducted by trained research assistants under the supervision of a licensed clinical neuropsychologist. The following tests were selected as they are clinically useful and have been used to assess cognition in HF in previous studies [4, 5]. The following cognitive domains were included:
Global cognitive function
The Modified Mini-Mental State Examination (3MS) is brief screening measure that provides an estimate of global cognitive function [19].
Attention
Attention was assessed using Letter Number Sequencing (LNS), the Trail Making Test A (TMT-A), and the Symbol Digit Modalities Test. The LNS asks participants to repeat unordered strings of letters and numbers in both numerical and alphabetical order. Higher scores reflect better executive functioning and this task has been shown to correlate with other measures (r = -.36) [20, 21]. In the TMT-A participants are instructed to draw a connecting line between numbered circles as quickly as possible [19]. Completion time is a reliable and valid measure of complex visual scanning and psychomotor speed (Test-retest reliability estimated at r = 0.79) [22]. The Symbol Digit Modalities Test asks participants to code numbers to correspond with symbols as quickly as possible. Past studies have indicated this test is reliable and valid [23, 24], with a test-retest reliability of r = 0.70 [25].
Executive function
To test executive function performance, the Frontal Assessment Battery, the Trail Making Test B (TMT-B), and the Stroop Interference Task were used. The Frontal Assessment Battery contains six subtests which assess various aspects of executive function, including conceptualization, mental flexibility, inhibition, and sequencing. Higher scores indicate better test performance [26]. The FAB has been shown to have strong psychometric properties including good internal consistency (Cronbach’s a = 0.78), discriminant validity (89.1% of cases were correctly identified), and interrater reliability (= 0.87, p < 0.001) [27]. The TMT-B requires participants to draw a connecting line between alternating numbered and lettered circles as quickly as possible [19]. Test completion time is a widely used measures of executive function, with strong psychometric properties (Test-retest reliability estimated at r = 0.89) [22]. The SCWT consists of three-45 second trials. The first involves reading as many words written in black ink as possible (Word). The next test requires participants to identify the ink color of blue, red, or green X’s (Color). The final test has words written in opposite color; for example, the word “red” is written in green ink and the person must response “green” (Color-Word). An interference score can be calculated, which anchors for overall talking speed and color perception while quantifying an individual’s ability to suppress prepotent response [28, 29].
Memory
Memory tests included the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT) and the Complex Figure Test (CFT). The CVLT asks participants to learn, recall, and recognize items from a 16-item word list. This test incorporates indices of delayed recall and recognition, and the average of these scores were used for analysis. This task has strong psychometric properties, including a high correlation to other verbal memory measures (r =0.63 to r = 0.96) [30]. In the CFT, participants recall a complex geometric design [31]. Interrater reliability and internal consistency reliability for this measure are strong in geriatric populations [32].
Language
To assess language performance, the Boston Naming Test-II (BNT-II) and the Animal Naming task were used. The BNT-II requires individuals to name pictures of objects ranging from high to low familiarity [19]. The BNT is closely correlated with other verbal ability tests such as the McGintie Reading Test (r = .83) [33]. Likewise, the Animal Naming task asks participants to generate as many animal names as possible within 60 seconds [34]. This task has been widely used as a measure of semantic verbal fluency in older adults [35].

Cardiovascular fitness level

Estimated metabolic equivalents (METs) were estimated using a standardized symptom limited volitional maximal treadmill stress test. In this test, treadmill elevation increased every 60 seconds and an increase in speed every 3 minutes as to estimate an increase in workload of 15%. The stress test ended if either the participant requested or if the participant showed clinical signs of cardiovascular, neurologic, or musculoskeletal distress. Maximum METs were estimated through a combination of treadmill speed, grade, and duration [36].

Office-based estimates of cardiovascular fitness

The two minute step test (2MST) requires individuals to walk in place, lifting their knees to a target midway between their kneecap and the crest of the iliac [37]. Individuals may use a wall or chair for balance as needed. A higher step count indicates better cardiovascular fitness [12].
The Duke Activity Status Index (DASI) was also administered to estimate individual's estimated peak oxygen uptake. This measure is a 12-question self-report on various aspects of physical work capacity and quality of life, asking whether individuals can complete daily tasks that are weighted based on the task's difficulty. It has been used as a valid estimate of physical ability in older adults [38].

Depressive symptoms

The Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II) was used to measure the presence and severity of depression [39]. This measure is a 21-item self-report instrument and has good psychometric properties including strong internal reliability and validity [40]. Higher scores on the BDI-II indicate more severe depressive symptoms.

Procedure

All procedures were approved by the Kent State University and Summa Health Institutional Review Boards and written consent was obtained from all participants. METs were established with an exercise stress test protocol that was completed upon enrollment into cardiac rehabilitation. Within two weeks of the initial stress test, participants completed a research visit that consisted of a neuropsychological test battery, the 2MST, and series of medical history and self-report questionnaires, including the DASI. Ejection fraction was determined using medical record review.

Statistical analyses

To facilitate clinical interpretation and to maintain directionality within the scales, all raw scores of the neuropsychological measures were transformed to T-scores (a distribution with a mean of 50, and a standard deviation of 10) using normative data correcting for age, similar to the approach used in other studies [41]. Memory measures were also corrected for gender. Composite scores for attention, executive function, memory, and language were means of the T-scores within each cognitive domain. Consistent with convention in many clinical settings, impairment in these domains for the current study was defined as a T-score ≤ 35 relative to normative data.
To examine the independent association between METs and cognitive function, a series of hierarchical regression analyses were performed with composite scores for each cognitive domain. All analyses utilized IBM SPSS Statistics version 21. For each model, age, sex (0 = female; 1 = male), years of education, LVEF, depressive symptomatology (as assessed by the BDI-II), and diagnostic history of hypertension and diabetes (1 = positive history; 0 = negative history), were entered into the first block. METs was then entered into the second block of each model to determine its incremental predictive validity above and beyond demographic and medical characteristics. Finally, a series of partial correlations adjusting for the aforementioned demographic and medical characteristics was conducted to examine the association of METs with the 2MST and peak oxygen uptake as estimated by the DASI.

Results

METs and cognitive performance

The current sample obtained an average maximum of 6.24 (SD = 3.11) METs during the stress test. See Table 1. On a global measure of cognitive function (3MS), average performance was 92.98 (SD = 5.36), with 22.0% of the sample scoring below 90, 41.5% between 90 and 95 and 36.6% above 95. See Table 2.
Table 2
Neuropsychological test performance in older adults with HF ( N= 41)
 
Raw test performance, mean (SD)
T-score, mean (SD)
Global cognitive function
92.98 (5.36)
--
Attention
  
TMTA (seconds)
40.22 (14.47)
50.46 (8.65)
Letter number sequence
8.93(2.49)
51.37 (8.42)
Digit symbol
53.78 (11.78)
50.07 (7.88)
Executive function
  
TMTB (seconds)
121.44 (77.24)
43.20 (22.44)
Frontal assessment battery
15.88 (2.46)
46.98 (20.78)
Stroop interference
1.04 (7.39)
51.05 (7.26)
Memory
  
CVLT LDFR
7.85 (3.22)
46.95 (10.18)
CVLT total hits
13.59 (2.22)
44.88 (13.11)
CVLT false positive
4.46 (3.23)
48.66 (9.94)
CFT delayed recall
12.77 (6.13)
49.41 (8.82)
Language
  
Boston naming test
53.61 (6.41)
51.38 (16.85)
Animals
19.93 (4.99)
56.27 (11.11)
TMTA Trail Making Test A; TMTB Trail Making Test B; LNS Letter Number Sequencing; CVLT California Verbal Learning Test; LDFR Long Delay Free Recall; CFT Complex Figure Test.

METs is independently associated with cognitive function

Demographic and medical factors were associated with test performance for attention (F(7,33) = 2.39, p = .04) and executive function (F(7,33) = 2.91, p = .02). After controlling for age, sex, education, LVEF, depressive symptomatology (as assessed by the BDI-II), and history of hypertension and diabetes, METs demonstrated incremental predictive validity for attention (β = .41, p = .03), executive function (β = .37, p = .04), and memory domains (β = .46, p = .04). In each case, higher METs levels were associated with better cognitive test performance. No such pattern emerged for language abilities (β = .27, p = .23). Notably, bivariate correlations also revealed higher METs were associated with better performance on the 3MS (r(39) = .38, p = .01). See Table 3 for a full summary of hierarchical regression analyses.
Table 3
METs independently predict cognitive function in older adults with heart failure ( N= 41)
 
Attention
Executive
Memory
Language
 
b(SE b)
b(SE b)
b(SE b)
b(SE b)
Block 1
Age
.27(.14)
.65(.27)*
.25(.18)
-.13(.25)
Sex
2.51(2.47)
5.80(4.77)
.27(3.25)
4.38(4.50)
Education
.72(.38)
1.28(.73)
.36(.50)
1.07(.69)
LVEF
.00(.10)
.18(.10)
.03(.13)
-.05(.17)
BDI-II
.03(.14)
-.49(.27)
.08(.18)
-.32(.25)
Hypertension
−4.98(2.52)
−2.74(4.86)
−1.95(3.32)
−1.80(4.59)
Diabetes
−2.36(2.29)
−3.74(4.41)
−1.42(3.01)
−1.22(4.17)
R 2
.34
.38
.11
.15
F
2.39*
2.91*
.58
.84
Block 2
METs
.92(.41)*
1.67(.80)*
1.16(.54)*
.95(.78)
R 2
.43
.46
.22
.19
F for ΔR2
5.10*
4.39*
4.57*
1.48
Note. *denotes p < 0.05; **denotes p < .01.
Abbreviations: b unstandardized regression coefficients; SE standard error; LVEF Left Ventricular Ejection Fraction; BDI-II Beck Depression Inventory-II; METs Metabolic Equivalent Task.

Association between METs with the 2MST and DASI

To clarify the association between different estimates of fitness level, partial correlations accounting for the effect of medical and demographic characteristics revealed increased METs was associated with better 2MST performance (r(32) = .41, p = .02). No relationship emerged between METs estimated from stress test and peak oxygen uptake as estimated by the DASI (r(32) = .24, p = .17).
Consistent with this and the above pattern, partial correlations showed lower 2MST performance was also associated with worse attention (r(32) = .34, p = .047) and executive function (r (32) = .40, p = .02) even after accounting for age, sex, education, LVEF, depressive symptomatology, and diagnostic history of hypertension and diabetes. The 2MST was not associated with memory (r(32) = .13, p = .46) or language performance (r(32) = .15, p = .41). Peak oxygen uptake as estimated by the DASI was not independently associated with cognitive function in any of the domains: attention (r(32) = .02, p = .90), executive function (r(32) = .05, p = .78), memory (r(32) = -.02, p = .91), language (r(32) = .17, p = .34).

Discussion

Results from the current study indicate that a better fitness level as estimated by METs is associated with better cognitive function in older adults with HF. This relationship remained significant after controlling for key demographic and medical factors, including LVEF. A similar pattern emerged for one office-based estimate of fitness (i.e. 2MST) but not another (i.e. DASI). Several aspects of these findings warrant brief discussion.
Results showing that METs predicted cognitive function above and beyond ejection fraction are consistent with past work showing fitness is an important contribution to cognition in other healthy and medical populations [42, 43]. There are several mechanisms by which better fitness levels may promote cognitive function in older adults with HF. A likely mechanism is increased cerebral blood flow (CBF). Greater CBF is associated with better cognition in HF [6, 8] and greater CBF has also been associated with increased METs in other populations [44]. Similarly, better fitness levels may also be associated with better brain integrity in HF. For instance, lower physical activity levels have been associated with greater atrophy in white [45] and grey matter in older adults [46]. In turn, this and other forms of brain pathology have been linked to poorer cognitive function in persons with HF [47, 48]. Future studies are needed to more clearly determine the mechanisms for the neuroprotective effect of fitness in HF patients, particularly prospective studies involving neuroimaging.
As noted above, HF patients frequently experience cognitive decline [3], which can lead to negative outcomes such as increased mortality and hospitalization, as well as a decrease in quality of life [1, 2, 49]. As fitness level is at least partly modifiable in HF patients, future work should further investigate the potential cognitive benefits of structured exercise in HF patients. Past work has shown that participation in a cardiac rehabilitation program was associated with cognitive improvements in a mixed sample of persons with cardiovascular disease [50], and it is possible that HF patients may show similar improvements. If so, structured exercise programs may protect against cognitive decline. Furthermore, cognitive impairment has been linked to hospitalization [2], and thus a decrease in cognitive deficits via exercise programs may decrease hospitalization rates and also ultimately reduce medical costs in HF.
We also examined the relationship between METs and two office-based measures of fitness to help clinicians identify those HF patients at risk for cognitive impairment. Found to be valid estimates in other populations [1618], both the DASI and the 2MST have been suggested for use in HF patients. Contrary to expectations, although METs level was closely associated with 2MST, it was unrelated to the DASI in the current sample. The exact reason for the lack of association between METs and DASI is unclear, but may involve the cognitive impairment found in persons with HF. It is possible that HF-related cognitive impairment limits patients’ ability to provide accurate information about fitness levels. This is similar to work on the self report of sleep and functional status in which patients were unable to remember and make accurate judgments about their sleep length and quality as well as independent functioning [51, 52]. As such, HF patients who suffer from executive dysfunction may be unable to accurately judge how much activity they engage in or how fit they truly are. Furthermore, participants may also have wanted to appear more functioning on the DASI than they may actually are, a common inaccuracy in self-report measures. Future work is needed to determine whether a similar amount of error is found in the report of fitness levels in persons with HF.
The current findings are limited in several ways. Studies are needed to clarify the mechanisms that link fitness levels and cognitive function in older adults with HF, particularly studies utilizing neuroimaging and direct measures of CBF. Future research would also benefit from prospective examination of fitness levels and cognitive function to better understand this relationship, particularly the possibility that programs like cardiac rehabilitation that may improve cognitive function in persons with HF. An additional limitation to the current work includes relatively small sample size and larger studies are needed to confirm the current findings.

Conclusion

In summary, maximum METs is an independent predictor of cognitive function in HF patients. Future work is needed to clarify the underlying mechanisms for this relationship, particularly prospective studies to determine whether structured exercise programs may improve cognitive function in persons with HF.

Acknowledgements

Data collection supported by HL089311. Manuscript supported in part by DK075119 & AG011230.
Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

SG contributed to study concept and design, acquisition of subjects, data analysis and interpretation, and manuscript preparation. MA participated in study concept and design, acquisition of subjects, data analysis and interpretation, and manuscript preparation. MS, RC, NR, LS, LC, RJ, JH, JR, LO, and JG contributed to contributed to study concept and design, data analysis and interpretation, and manuscript preparation. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Roger VL, Go AS, Lloyd-Jones DM: Heart disease and stroke statistics—2012 update. Circulation. 2012, 125: e2-e220.CrossRefPubMed Roger VL, Go AS, Lloyd-Jones DM: Heart disease and stroke statistics—2012 update. Circulation. 2012, 125: e2-e220.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Jencks SF, Williams MV, Coleman EA: Rehospitalization among patients in Medicare fee-for- service program. N Engl J Med. 2009, 360: 1418-1428. 10.1056/NEJMsa0803563.CrossRefPubMed Jencks SF, Williams MV, Coleman EA: Rehospitalization among patients in Medicare fee-for- service program. N Engl J Med. 2009, 360: 1418-1428. 10.1056/NEJMsa0803563.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Vogels RL, Scheltens P, Schroeder-Tanka JM: Cognitive impairment in heart failure: a systematic review of the literature. Eur J Heart Fail. 2007, 9 (5): 440-449. 10.1016/j.ejheart.2006.11.001.CrossRefPubMed Vogels RL, Scheltens P, Schroeder-Tanka JM: Cognitive impairment in heart failure: a systematic review of the literature. Eur J Heart Fail. 2007, 9 (5): 440-449. 10.1016/j.ejheart.2006.11.001.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Pressler SJ, Jinshil K, Riley P, Ronis D, Gradus-Pizlo I: Memory dysfunction, psychomotor slowing, and decreased executive dysfunction predict mortality in patients with heart failure and low ejection fraction. J Card Fail. 2010, 16: 750-760. 10.1016/j.cardfail.2010.04.007.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pressler SJ, Jinshil K, Riley P, Ronis D, Gradus-Pizlo I: Memory dysfunction, psychomotor slowing, and decreased executive dysfunction predict mortality in patients with heart failure and low ejection fraction. J Card Fail. 2010, 16: 750-760. 10.1016/j.cardfail.2010.04.007.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Vogels RL, Oosterman JM, van Harten B: Profile of cognitive impairment in chronic heart failure. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007, 55: 1764-1770. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01395.x.CrossRefPubMed Vogels RL, Oosterman JM, van Harten B: Profile of cognitive impairment in chronic heart failure. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007, 55: 1764-1770. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01395.x.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Alves TCTF, Rays J, Fráguas R: Localized cerebral blood flow reductions in patients with heart failure: a study using 99mTc-HMPAO SPECT. J Neuroimaging. 2005, 15 (2): 150-156.PubMed Alves TCTF, Rays J, Fráguas R: Localized cerebral blood flow reductions in patients with heart failure: a study using 99mTc-HMPAO SPECT. J Neuroimaging. 2005, 15 (2): 150-156.PubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Zuccalà G, Onder G, Pedone C: Hypotension and cognitive impairment: selective association in patients with heart failure. Neurology. 2001, 57 (11): 1986-1992.CrossRefPubMed Zuccalà G, Onder G, Pedone C: Hypotension and cognitive impairment: selective association in patients with heart failure. Neurology. 2001, 57 (11): 1986-1992.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Jefferson AL, Himali JJ, Beiser AS: Cardiac index is associated with brain aging: the framingham heart study. Circulation. 2010, 122 (7): 690-697. 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.109.905091.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Jefferson AL, Himali JJ, Beiser AS: Cardiac index is associated with brain aging: the framingham heart study. Circulation. 2010, 122 (7): 690-697. 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.109.905091.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Woo MA, Macey PM, Fonarow GC, Hamilton MA, Harper RM: Regional brain gray matter loss in heart failure. J Appl Physiol. 2003, 95 (2): 677-684.CrossRefPubMed Woo MA, Macey PM, Fonarow GC, Hamilton MA, Harper RM: Regional brain gray matter loss in heart failure. J Appl Physiol. 2003, 95 (2): 677-684.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosano C, Venkatraman VK, Guralnik J: Psychomotor speed and functional brain MRI 2 years after completing a physical activity treatment. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2010, 65 (6): 639-647.CrossRefPubMed Rosano C, Venkatraman VK, Guralnik J: Psychomotor speed and functional brain MRI 2 years after completing a physical activity treatment. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2010, 65 (6): 639-647.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Tanne D, Freimark D, Poreh A: Cognitive functions in severe congestive heart failure before and after an exercise training program. Int J Cardiol. 2004, 103: 145-149.CrossRef Tanne D, Freimark D, Poreh A: Cognitive functions in severe congestive heart failure before and after an exercise training program. Int J Cardiol. 2004, 103: 145-149.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Alosco ML, Spitznagel MB, Raz N: The 2-Minute Step Test is independently associated with cognitive function in older adults with heart failure. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2012, 24 (5): 468-474.PubMed Alosco ML, Spitznagel MB, Raz N: The 2-Minute Step Test is independently associated with cognitive function in older adults with heart failure. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2012, 24 (5): 468-474.PubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Emery CF, Shermer RL, Hauck ER, Hsiao ET, MacIntyre NR: Cognitive and psychological outcomes of exercise in a 1-year follow-up study of patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Health Psychol. 2003, 22 (6): 598-604.CrossRefPubMed Emery CF, Shermer RL, Hauck ER, Hsiao ET, MacIntyre NR: Cognitive and psychological outcomes of exercise in a 1-year follow-up study of patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Health Psychol. 2003, 22 (6): 598-604.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Vasquez PE, Moraes H, Silveira H, Deslandes AC, Laks J: Acute exercise improves cognition in the depressed elderly: the effect of dual-tasks. Clinics. 2011, 66 (9): 1553-1557. 10.1590/S1807-59322011000900008.CrossRef Vasquez PE, Moraes H, Silveira H, Deslandes AC, Laks J: Acute exercise improves cognition in the depressed elderly: the effect of dual-tasks. Clinics. 2011, 66 (9): 1553-1557. 10.1590/S1807-59322011000900008.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Buchman AS, Boyle PA, Yu L, Shah RC, Wilson RS, Bennett DA: Total daily physical activity and the risk of AD and cognitive decline in older adults. Neurology. 2012, 78 (17): 1323-1329. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182535d35.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Buchman AS, Boyle PA, Yu L, Shah RC, Wilson RS, Bennett DA: Total daily physical activity and the risk of AD and cognitive decline in older adults. Neurology. 2012, 78 (17): 1323-1329. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182535d35.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Bairey Merz C, Olson M, McGorray S: Physical activity and functional capacity measurement in women: a report from the NHLBI-sponsored WISE study. J Women's Health Gend Based Med. 2000, 9 (7): 769-777. 10.1089/15246090050147745.CrossRef Bairey Merz C, Olson M, McGorray S: Physical activity and functional capacity measurement in women: a report from the NHLBI-sponsored WISE study. J Women's Health Gend Based Med. 2000, 9 (7): 769-777. 10.1089/15246090050147745.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang J, Zhang B, Deng D, Tu W, Gao C, Zhang Y: The relationship between functional capacity (FC) and cardiovascular risk factors (CVRFs) in senile patients after noncardiac surgery. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2010, 51 (1): 92-94. 10.1016/j.archger.2009.08.006.CrossRefPubMed Zhang J, Zhang B, Deng D, Tu W, Gao C, Zhang Y: The relationship between functional capacity (FC) and cardiovascular risk factors (CVRFs) in senile patients after noncardiac surgery. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2010, 51 (1): 92-94. 10.1016/j.archger.2009.08.006.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Rikli RE, Jones CJ: Senior Fitness Test Manual. 2001, Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Rikli RE, Jones CJ: Senior Fitness Test Manual. 2001, Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O: A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. 1991, New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 3 Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O: A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. 1991, New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 3
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Gifford DR, Cummings JL: Evaluating dementia screening tests: methodological standard to rate their performance. Neurology. 1999, 52: 224-227. 10.1212/WNL.52.2.224.CrossRefPubMed Gifford DR, Cummings JL: Evaluating dementia screening tests: methodological standard to rate their performance. Neurology. 1999, 52: 224-227. 10.1212/WNL.52.2.224.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Titus JB, Retzlaff PD, Dean RS: Predicting scores of the Halstead category test with the WAIS-III. Int J Neurosci. 2002, 112: 1099-1114. 10.1080/00207450290026085.CrossRefPubMed Titus JB, Retzlaff PD, Dean RS: Predicting scores of the Halstead category test with the WAIS-III. Int J Neurosci. 2002, 112: 1099-1114. 10.1080/00207450290026085.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Dikmen S, Heaton R, Grant I, Temkin N: Test-retest reliability of the expanded halstead-reitan neuropsychological test battery. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1999, 5 (4): 346-356.CrossRefPubMed Dikmen S, Heaton R, Grant I, Temkin N: Test-retest reliability of the expanded halstead-reitan neuropsychological test battery. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1999, 5 (4): 346-356.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Morgan SF, Wheelock J: Comparability of WAIS-R digit symbol and the symbol digit modalities test. Percept Mot Skills. 1995, 80 (2): 631-634. 10.2466/pms.1995.80.2.631.CrossRefPubMed Morgan SF, Wheelock J: Comparability of WAIS-R digit symbol and the symbol digit modalities test. Percept Mot Skills. 1995, 80 (2): 631-634. 10.2466/pms.1995.80.2.631.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith A: Symbol Digit Modalities Test: Manual. 1982, Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services Smith A: Symbol Digit Modalities Test: Manual. 1982, Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Echemendia RJ, Lovell MR, Collins MW, Prigatano GP: Presented at: 107th Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Association. 1999, Washington, DC: Echemendia RJ, Lovell MR, Collins MW, Prigatano GP: Presented at: 107th Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Association. 1999, Washington, DC:
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Royall DR: The FAB: a frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology. 2011, 57 (3): 565-CrossRef Royall DR: The FAB: a frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology. 2011, 57 (3): 565-CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, Pillon B: The FAB: a frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology. 2000, 55: 1621-1626. 10.1212/WNL.55.11.1621.CrossRefPubMed Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, Pillon B: The FAB: a frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology. 2000, 55: 1621-1626. 10.1212/WNL.55.11.1621.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Golden CJ: Stroop Color and Word Test: A Manual for Clinical and Experimental Uses. 1978, Chicago, IL: Skoelting Golden CJ: Stroop Color and Word Test: A Manual for Clinical and Experimental Uses. 1978, Chicago, IL: Skoelting
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Stroop JR: Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psych. 1935, 18: 643-662.CrossRef Stroop JR: Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psych. 1935, 18: 643-662.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Delis D, Kramer J, Kaplan E, Ober B: California Verbal Learning Test- Second Edition: Adult Version. 2000, : Psychological Corporation Delis D, Kramer J, Kaplan E, Ober B: California Verbal Learning Test- Second Edition: Adult Version. 2000, : Psychological Corporation
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Loring D, Martin R, Meador K, Lee G: Psychometric construction of the Rey-osterieth complex figure: methodological considerations and interrater reliability. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1990, 5: 1-14.CrossRefPubMed Loring D, Martin R, Meador K, Lee G: Psychometric construction of the Rey-osterieth complex figure: methodological considerations and interrater reliability. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1990, 5: 1-14.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Berry DTR, Allen RS, Schmitt FA: Rey-osterrieth complex figure: psychometric characteristics in a geriatric sample. Clin Neuropsychol. 1991, 5 (2): 143-153. 10.1080/13854049108403298.CrossRef Berry DTR, Allen RS, Schmitt FA: Rey-osterrieth complex figure: psychometric characteristics in a geriatric sample. Clin Neuropsychol. 1991, 5 (2): 143-153. 10.1080/13854049108403298.CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Hawkins KA, Sledge WH, Orleans JE, Quinlan DM, Rakfeldt J, Huffman RE: Normative implications of the relationship between reading vocabulary and Boston Naming Test performance. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1993, 8: 525-537.CrossRefPubMed Hawkins KA, Sledge WH, Orleans JE, Quinlan DM, Rakfeldt J, Huffman RE: Normative implications of the relationship between reading vocabulary and Boston Naming Test performance. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1993, 8: 525-537.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Cooper D, Epker M, Lacritz L: Effects of practice on category fluency in Alzheimer's disease. Clin Neuropsychol. 2001, 15: 125-128. 10.1076/clin.15.1.125.1914.CrossRefPubMed Cooper D, Epker M, Lacritz L: Effects of practice on category fluency in Alzheimer's disease. Clin Neuropsychol. 2001, 15: 125-128. 10.1076/clin.15.1.125.1914.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, Rees L: Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1999, 14 (2): 167-177.PubMed Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, Rees L: Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1999, 14 (2): 167-177.PubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat American College of Sports Medicine: ACSM’s Health Related Fitness Assessment Manual. 2005, New York: Lippincott, Williams, & Wilkins American College of Sports Medicine: ACSM’s Health Related Fitness Assessment Manual. 2005, New York: Lippincott, Williams, & Wilkins
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Jones CJ, Rikli RE: Measuring functional fitness of older adults. The Journal on Active Aging. 2002, 2: 24-30. Jones CJ, Rikli RE: Measuring functional fitness of older adults. The Journal on Active Aging. 2002, 2: 24-30.
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Hlatky MA, Boineau RE, Higginbotham MB: A brief self-administered questionnaire to determine functional capacity (The Duke Activity Status Index). Am J Cardiol. 1989, 64 (10): 651-654. 10.1016/0002-9149(89)90496-7.CrossRefPubMed Hlatky MA, Boineau RE, Higginbotham MB: A brief self-administered questionnaire to determine functional capacity (The Duke Activity Status Index). Am J Cardiol. 1989, 64 (10): 651-654. 10.1016/0002-9149(89)90496-7.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK: Beck Depression Inventory. 1996, San Antonio. TX: The Psychological Corporation, 2 Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK: Beck Depression Inventory. 1996, San Antonio. TX: The Psychological Corporation, 2
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Segal DL, Coolidge FL, Cahill BS, O'Riley AA: Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II) among community-dwelling older adults. Behav Modif. 2008, 32 (1): 3-20. 10.1177/0145445507303833.CrossRefPubMed Segal DL, Coolidge FL, Cahill BS, O'Riley AA: Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II) among community-dwelling older adults. Behav Modif. 2008, 32 (1): 3-20. 10.1177/0145445507303833.CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Knox MR, Lacritz LH, Chandler MJ, Munro CC: Association between dementia rating scale performance and neurocognitive domains in Alzheimer's disease. Clin Neuropsychol. 2003, 17 (2): 216-219. 10.1076/clin.17.2.216.16496.CrossRefPubMed Knox MR, Lacritz LH, Chandler MJ, Munro CC: Association between dementia rating scale performance and neurocognitive domains in Alzheimer's disease. Clin Neuropsychol. 2003, 17 (2): 216-219. 10.1076/clin.17.2.216.16496.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Gunstad J, MacGregor KL, Paul RH: Cardiac rehabilitation improves cognitive performance in older adults with cardiovascular disease. J Cardiopulm Rehabil. 2005, 25 (3): 173-176. 10.1097/00008483-200505000-00009.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Gunstad J, MacGregor KL, Paul RH: Cardiac rehabilitation improves cognitive performance in older adults with cardiovascular disease. J Cardiopulm Rehabil. 2005, 25 (3): 173-176. 10.1097/00008483-200505000-00009.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Weuve J, Keng JH, Manson JE: Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women. JAMA. 2004, 292 (12): 1454-1461. 10.1001/jama.292.12.1454.CrossRefPubMed Weuve J, Keng JH, Manson JE: Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women. JAMA. 2004, 292 (12): 1454-1461. 10.1001/jama.292.12.1454.CrossRefPubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Brown AD, McMorris CA, Longman S: Effects of cardiorespiratory fitness and cerebral blood flow on cognitive outcomes in older women. Neurobiol Aging. 2010, 31 (12): 2047-2057. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.11.002.CrossRefPubMed Brown AD, McMorris CA, Longman S: Effects of cardiorespiratory fitness and cerebral blood flow on cognitive outcomes in older women. Neurobiol Aging. 2010, 31 (12): 2047-2057. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.11.002.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Ho AJ, Raji CA, Becker JT: The effects of physical activity, education, and body mass index on the aging brain. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010, 32 (9): 1371-1382.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ho AJ, Raji CA, Becker JT: The effects of physical activity, education, and body mass index on the aging brain. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010, 32 (9): 1371-1382.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Erickson KI, Raji CA, Lopez OL: Physical activity predicts grey matter volume in late adulthood: the cardiovascular health study. Neurology. 2010, 75 (16): 1415-1422. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f88359.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Erickson KI, Raji CA, Lopez OL: Physical activity predicts grey matter volume in late adulthood: the cardiovascular health study. Neurology. 2010, 75 (16): 1415-1422. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f88359.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Oosterman JM, Vogels R, van Harten B: The role of white matter hyperintensities and medial temporal lobe atrophy in age-related executive dysfunctioning. Brain Cogn. 2008, 68: 128-133. 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.03.006.CrossRefPubMed Oosterman JM, Vogels R, van Harten B: The role of white matter hyperintensities and medial temporal lobe atrophy in age-related executive dysfunctioning. Brain Cogn. 2008, 68: 128-133. 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.03.006.CrossRefPubMed
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Vogels RL, Oosterman JM, van Harten B: Neuroimaging and correlates of cognitive function among patients with heart failure. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2007, 24: 418-423. 10.1159/000109811.CrossRefPubMed Vogels RL, Oosterman JM, van Harten B: Neuroimaging and correlates of cognitive function among patients with heart failure. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2007, 24: 418-423. 10.1159/000109811.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Allen LA, Stevenson LW, Grady KL: Decision making in advanced heart failure: a scientific statement from the American heart association. Circulation. 2012, 125: 1928-1952. 10.1161/CIR.0b013e31824f2173.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Allen LA, Stevenson LW, Grady KL: Decision making in advanced heart failure: a scientific statement from the American heart association. Circulation. 2012, 125: 1928-1952. 10.1161/CIR.0b013e31824f2173.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Stanek KM, Gunstad J, Spitznagel MB: Improvements in cognitive function following cardiac rehabilitation for older adults with cardiovascular disease. Int J Neurosci. 2011, 121: 86-93. 10.3109/00207454.2010.531893.CrossRefPubMed Stanek KM, Gunstad J, Spitznagel MB: Improvements in cognitive function following cardiac rehabilitation for older adults with cardiovascular disease. Int J Neurosci. 2011, 121: 86-93. 10.3109/00207454.2010.531893.CrossRefPubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang MY, Hung HL, Tsai PS: The sleep log and actigraphy: congruency of measurement results for heart failure patients. J Nurs Res. 2011, 19: 173-180. 10.1097/JNR.0b013e318229c42f.CrossRefPubMed Wang MY, Hung HL, Tsai PS: The sleep log and actigraphy: congruency of measurement results for heart failure patients. J Nurs Res. 2011, 19: 173-180. 10.1097/JNR.0b013e318229c42f.CrossRefPubMed
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Skotzko CE, Krichten C, Zietowski G: Depression is common and precludes accurate assessment of functional status in elderly patients with congestive heart failure. J Card Fail. 2000, 6 (4): 300-305. 10.1054/jcaf.2000.19222.CrossRefPubMed Skotzko CE, Krichten C, Zietowski G: Depression is common and precludes accurate assessment of functional status in elderly patients with congestive heart failure. J Card Fail. 2000, 6 (4): 300-305. 10.1054/jcaf.2000.19222.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Cardiovascular fitness associated with cognitive performance in heart failure patients enrolled in cardiac rehabilitation
verfasst von
Sarah Garcia
Michael L Alosco
Mary Beth Spitznagel
Ronald Cohen
Naftali Raz
Lawrence Sweet
Richard Josephson
Joel Hughes
Jim Rosneck
Morgan L Oberle
John Gunstad
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2013
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Cardiovascular Disorders / Ausgabe 1/2013
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2261
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2261-13-29

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2013

BMC Cardiovascular Disorders 1/2013 Zur Ausgabe

Update Kardiologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.