Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Public Health 1/2019

Open Access 01.12.2019 | Research article

Predicting hazardous drinking in late adolescence/young adulthood from early and excessive adolescent drinking - a longitudinal cross-national study of Norwegian and Australian adolescents

verfasst von: Frøydis Enstad, Tracy Evans-Whipp, Anne Kjeldsen, John W. Toumbourou, Tilmann von Soest

Erschienen in: BMC Public Health | Ausgabe 1/2019

Abstract

Background

Research has consistently shown that early onset of drinking (EOD) is associated with alcohol-related problems in adulthood. However, recent reviews have identified several limitations in the early onset literature, including the use of retrospective reports, insufficient control for potential confounders, ambiguous definitions of the concept, and an assumption that early onset is independent of cultural norms and national alcohol policies. This study addresses these limitations by examining whether EOD, independent of early onset of excessive drinking (EOE), prospectively predicts hazardous drinking in late adolescence/young adulthood in Norway and Australia, two countries with different drinking cultures.

Methods

Data were drawn from two population-based longitudinal studies; the Norwegian Tracking Opportunities and Problems Study (n = 329) and the Australian International Youth Development Study (n = 786). Data were collected prospectively from mid adolescence (14–16 years) to late adolescence/young adulthood (18–25 years) and a modified Poisson regression approach was used to estimate prevalence ratios. Adolescent self-reports included measures of EOD and EOE. Young adults completed the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT). The results were adjusted for adolescent factors; age, gender, impulsivity, hyperactivity, conduct problems, smoking, early sexual intercourse and friends’ substance use, and family factors; alcohol and drug use in the family, maternal education, family management and monitoring.

Results

Hazardous drinking was identified in 46.8 and 38.9% of young adults in Norway and Australia, respectively. Both EOD and EOE in adolescence were significantly related to an increased risk of alcohol-related problems in late adolescence/young adulthood in both studies, even when adjusting for possible confounders.

Conclusion

Our findings indicate that adolescent drinking behaviour is an indicator of alcohol-related problems in late adolescence/young adulthood, even when controlling for a variety of covariates. This finding is in contrast to previous research on older adults, where no association between adolescent drinking and later alcohol-related problems were found when controlling for covariates. The divergence in findings may suggest that the impact of EOD/EOE is limited to the late adolescent and young adult period. Preventing drinking in early adolescence may thus have some impact on the drinking patterns in late adolescence/young adulthood.
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
AUDIT
Alcohol Use Identification Test
AUS
Australia/Australian
CI
Confidence interval
EOD
Early onset of drinking
EOE
Early onset of excessive drinking
NOR
Norway/Norwegian
PR
Prevalence ratios

Background

Empirical studies have consistently shown that early onset of drinking (EOD) is related to later adverse alcohol-related outcomes [116]. Many of these studies suggest a causal relationship between EOD and subsequent problems [16, 10, 14]. However, recent reviews of the literature have questioned the assumption of a causal link [1719]. In particular, it has been noted that: (a) research on EOD and its association with future adverse outcomes is largely based on adult samples asking participants to recall retrospectively their age of drinking onset, potentially introducing recall biases; (b) the few longitudinal studies measuring drinking onset prospectively typically suffer from lack of control for important covariates that may serve as confounders, such as externalising behaviours; (c) most studies do not provide information about whether having consumed small amounts of alcohol at an early age are related to adverse outcomes, or whether such associations are first seen when more substantial amounts are consumed; and (d) the association between early onset and adverse outcomes may vary according to the cultural context [17, 18, 20]. By using longitudinal data from two different countries, this study aims to provide a better understanding of the relationship between early drinking behaviours and hazardous drinking in late adolescence/young adulthood.
As thoroughly demonstrated in a review by Kuntsche et al. [17] the literature in this field suffers from poor reliability of the early drinking measurements as many studies are based on retrospective reports introducing potential bias. The concerns are related partly to the tendency of forward telescoping [21], as respondents tend to report the age of onset closer to their current age when interviewed. Moreover, heavy drinkers are to a greater degree biased to report an early onset of drinking than light drinkers when age of drinking is assessed retrospectively [22]. The association between retrospectively reported onset of drinking and adult drinking behaviour may thus be explained by methodological artefacts. It has therefore been argued that prospective studies from adolescence, close in time to when onset typically occurs, are more suited to examine EOD and its relation to later alcohol-related problems [16]. Furthermore, individual level variables (e.g., conduct problems, impulsivity-hyperactivity, antisocial peer environment, health risk behaviour), and family level variables (e.g., alcohol and drug use in the family) need to be included as covariates. Developmental theories have pointed to their relevance for the development of substance use disorders [23] and empirical research link these same factors to early onset of alcohol use [3, 8, 16, 24]. Such factors are potential confounders, as they may reflect shared vulnerability to both early onset and adult alcohol-related problems.
Our review of the literature identified 15 prospective studies addressing the impact of EOD on later drinking patterns or alcohol-related problems. Among these, four followed the participants only to mid adolescence [7, 2527] and five studies did not control for important confounding variables, such as conduct problems or parental alcohol and substance use [3, 14, 2830]. Results from the remaining six studies following participants from early adolescence to adulthood, and with comprehensive control for possible confounders, were mixed: Whereas four studies found that the association between EOD and adult alcohol-related problems was completely explained by relevant covariates [16, 3133], two other studies found that the association between EOD and heavy alcohol consumption remained, even after comprehensive control for potential explanatory factors, including conduct problems and parental drinking patterns [4, 11]. Thus, despite the growing number of prospective studies in this field, findings are still diverging, which may be related to varying operationalisations of early drinking by researchers, and different cultural contexts across studies.
Most of the studies in the early onset literature conceptualize onset as having had more than a few sips on one or more occasions [1, 4, 6, 14, 15, 27]. However, scholars have recently challenged the notion that consuming small amounts of alcohol in itself should have such profound impact on adolescent development [17, 34]. If the association between early drinking and adult alcohol-related problems is based on biological mechanisms, for example by early alcohol use affecting the cognitive functioning of the brain (i.e. [35]) or disrupting maturational processes [36], it is more plausible to assume that such changes would occur only if the amount of alcohol consumed had a potential neurotoxic effect. Likewise, if the mechanism is of a social nature, for example through changing the identity or social role of those drinking early (i.e. [14]), one could assume that such changes would be triggered only if the amount consumed violates a cultural expectation of acceptable behaviour. If the adolescents’ first encounter with alcohol is in a cultural setting where moderate alcohol use is the norm, e.g., a family dinner, it may be rather improbable that such behaviour would trigger a different developmental trajectory. Underage excessive drinking on the other hand, is a more clear breach of socially accepted behaviour, and as such has more potential to trigger a change in identity or role. A recent review supports this explanation, concluding that there are larger and more important effects observed in relation to the onset of regular drinking and experiences of intoxication than age of first drink [18].
To our knowledge, only two longitudinal studies have directly addressed whether EOD independently of early onset of excessive drinking (EOE) serves as a risk factor for later alcohol problems. First, Warner and White [33] found that the only onset related variable associated with adult alcohol problems, after a rigorous control of possible confounders, was feeling drunk at initiation. This study however only addresses different aspects of the very first drinking episode (i.e., in or outside a family context, early versus late onset, experience pleasure or not, and feelings of drunkenness). Second, Morean et al. [12] examined age of onset and delay to first intoxication as independent predictors of heavy drinking in college students. The results showed that both an early onset and a short delay independently predicted heavy drinking, indicating that EOE confers unique risk relative to EOD, but that both factors are related to increased risk. However, the study was based on retrospective reports of drinking onset and did not control for indicators of externalizing problems or other risk taking behaviour. Thus, knowledge on whether EOE foreshadows a different drinking pattern in young adulthood from EOD, or whether any early exposure to alcohol is detrimental, is limited.
Cultural norms and national alcohol policies may also be of importance in understanding the relationship between early drinking behaviours and adult alcohol use [37]. Whilst excessive drinking and drunkenness in adolescents is likely to be viewed as a social problem in most cultures, the degree to which low levels of adolescent drinking, particularly within adult-supervised contexts, is viewed as problematic varies largely across countries and cultures. Research has shown that in permissive cultures where moderate drinking among adolescents is culturally normative, EOD is to a lesser degree linked to other markers of deviance, such as conduct problems, than in cultures were EOD is considered a greater social problem, such as the United States, where most of the research on EOD has been conducted [20]. Underage excessive drinking, on the other hand, may be more consistently related to negative outcomes across cultures. Although EOD and EOE take on meaning in the larger cultural and political context, no previous study has applied a cross-national design when examining possible associations. The present study uses data from Norway and Australia, countries with different alcohol policies and drinking cultures.
Of particular interest, prevention strategies aimed at youths differ considerably in the two countries. Norway has one of Europe’s most restrictive alcohol policies [38] with an emphasis on reducing use through reducing the demand and availability [39]. As a consequence, underage drinking is illegal with no exceptions, and the prohibition also applies to suppling minors with alcohol. Information material aimed at youth highlights activities to increase factual knowledge about alcohol, strengthening the individual’s ability to resist peer pressure and facilitating get-togethers where no alcohol is served [40]. In contrast, the overarching goal of Australia’s strategy during the study period was to minimize harm associated with alcohol use [41]. Information strategies aimed at adolescents included advice on how to stay safe and ‘in control’ when drinking [42]. The per capita consumption in Norway is among the lowest in the Western countries both among adolescents and in the adult population [43, 44], whereas Australia is among the highest among the adult population [44]. The prevalence of heavy episodic drinking is about the same in the two countries. However, somewhat paradoxically, the Norwegian drinking pattern, i.e., how they drink rather than how much, is characterized as more risky than the Australian [44]. Cultural attitudes towards underage drinking may influence associations between early drinking behaviours and later detrimental outcomes. One could hypothesize that the relationship between EOD and later problem drinking is weaker in Australia than in Norway, because EOD in Norway may represent a greater violation of formal rules and as such is more closely related to other problem behaviours which are associated with hazardous drinking in adulthood.
In summary, the present study’s primary aim is to examine the association between EOD and the association with risk of alcohol-related problems in late adolescence/young adulthood. In particular, this study addresses several limitations of most previous research by examining (a) whether EOD (14–16 years) prospectively predicts risk of alcohol-related problems in late adolescence/young adulthood (18–25 years), (b) whether this relationship remains upon adjustment for a comprehensive number of covariates that may function as confounders, (c) whether early onset of more than a few sips of alcohol is predictive of later alcohol problems or whether only the consumption of more excessive amounts of alcohol in early age is related to such risk, and (d) whether relationships between EOD and subsequent risk of alcohol-related problems are similar in samples drawn from two different countries with different alcohol policies and drinking cultures.

Methods

Study design and setting

Data were obtained from the Tracking Opportunities and Problems Study (TOPP) and the International Youth Development Study (IYDS); two independent longitudinal samples of adolescents and young adults in the eastern part of Norway (henceforth termed NOR) and Victoria, a southern state in Australia (henceforth termed AUS), respectively.
The participants in TOPP were recruited in 1993 when the families attended their toddlers’ 18 month vaccination at child health clinics. Originally 1081 families from 19 geographical health care districts in eastern Norway (28% living in large cities, 55% in densely populated areas and 17% in rural areas), were invited to the study, of whom 86.9% participated in the first wave. Details of the study are described elsewhere [45]. Data used in this paper were obtained from surveys of the participants and their mothers conducted in 2006 (t6; henceforth termed adolescence, median age = 14.6, range 14.0–15.8) and 2011 (t8; henceforth termed late adolescence/young adulthood, median age = 18.9, range 18.3–19.9). The overall response rate at t6 and t8 was 49% (n = 458) and 47% (n = 441) respectively, based on participants at t1 (n = 939, 87% of the eligible sample). The final sample, with data at both time points of interest were available for 329 young people (131 males and 198 females). Thus, from the 1081 families invited to the study, 30.4% were included in the present study. Previous attrition analyses have shown that dropout was predicted by low maternal education level and being male [46, 47]. The study was approved by the Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics.
The IYDS is an ongoing binational longitudinal study initiated in 2002 of three cohorts (t1 in grade 5, grade 7 and grade 9) of young people in Victoria, Australia and Washington State, United States. This study uses data from the Australian sample only as data on young adults in the US was not available at the time of analysis. The oldest age (grade 9) cohort was used as this best matched the ages at which TOPP data collection was conducted. The IYDS is a school-based survey with a state wide representative sample recruited and surveyed in 2002 and followed up to 8 subsequent waves. Details of the study and school recruitment procedures has been described elsewhere [48]. In the IYDS oldest cohort, 1288 students were eligible to participate, of whom 973 (76%) participated at t1 and 788 in the follow-up (81% retention). Honesty criteria based on responses to the t1 survey was used to remove 2 participants and so the final sample with data at both time points was 786. The surveys included in this study were conducted in 2002 (t1; henceforth termed adolescence, median age = 14.9, range 13.8–16.2) and 2010 (t7; henceforth termed late adolescence/young adulthood, median age = 22.9, range 21.6–24.6). The IYDS also includes mother report on education level from a phone survey conducted in 2002.

Measures

In NOR and AUS, hazardous drinking in late adolescence/young adulthood was assessed using the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification test (AUDIT) [49]. The AUDIT is a widely used 10 item screening tool to identify persons with hazardous (i.e., those who are at risk of alcohol-related problems) and harmful (i.e., those experiencing some alcohol-related problems) patterns of alcohol consumption. The scores of the AUDIT range from 0 to 40, and the generally accepted cut-off for hazardous drinking is 8 and above, and a cut-off for harmful use is 16 and above [4951]. The present study examines only hazardous use. In the NOR questionnaire, Item 3 differs from the original AUDIT in that it asks “How often do you have five or more drinks on one occasion” instead of six. Cronbach’s alpha for the whole scale was .77 (NOR) and .83 (AUS). In the NOR sample, drinking smaller amounts of alcohol and excessive use in adolescence were measured by adolescent self-report using two items; “Have you ever tasted more than a few sips of alcohol?” and “During the past 12 months, have you had so much to drink that you felt clearly intoxicated?” with five response categories (Never, Once, 2–5 times, 6–10 times and, More than 10 times). Responses were reclassified into two dummy variables; EOD relative to abstinent (have tasted one or more times, but not been intoxicated past 12 months, EOE (tasted one or more times and been intoxicated one or more times past 12 months). In the AUS sample these concepts were measured by adolescent self-report using three items; “In your lifetime, have you ever had more than just a few sips of an alcoholic beverage (like beer, wine or spirits)?” with five response categories (Never, 1 or 2 times, 3 to 5 times, 6 to 9 times, 10 or more times), “Think back over the past 2 weeks. How many times have you had five or more alcoholic drinks in a row?” (None, Once, Twice, 3–5 times, 6–9 times, 10 or more times) and “How often over the past year has your alcohol use caused you to get so drunk you were sick or passed out?” with items rated on an 8-point scale from “never” to “40 times or more”. Responses were reclassified into two dummy variables; EOD relative to abstinent (one or more times of lifetime alcohol use, but never binged and never sick or passed out), EOE (one or more times of lifetime alcohol use and one or more times binged and/or one or more times sick or passed out).
Hyperactivity (NOR) was measured with a subscale of the Strengths and Difficulties questionnaire (SDQ) [52] with five items (sample items: “I am restless”, “I find it hard to sit down for long”, “I think before I do things”) with four response categories ranging from 3 (Fits very well) to 0 (Doesn’t fit at all) (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.71). Impulsivity (AUS) was measured by three items describing typical ways to act (i.e., thinking before acting, rushing into things, answering before thinking). Response options were NO! (1), no (2), yes (3), YES! (4) (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.57, Cronbach’s alpha based on polychoric correlation = 0.63). Mother’s education level (NOR) was measured by asking the mothers to indicate their highest level of education on a five point scale ranging from 1, “9 year primary school or less” to 5, “More than 4 years at college or university” and by mother report (AUS) on a three point scale of “Less than secondary school (Year 12)” (1), “Completed secondary school (Year 12)” (2) to “Completed post-secondary education” (3). Poor family management (NOR) was assessed using adolescent-report on the short version of the “Keeping tabs” questionnaire developed for the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (SECCYD). The 6-items are questions regarding their parents’ supervision and monitoring (i.e. “how much does a parent know about…”; “who you spend time with?”, “…how you spend your money?”, “…where you go after school?”). Responses ranged from 1 to 4 (“knows everything” to “doesn’t know at all”) and indicate the extent to which the parent is thought to know about different aspects of the child’s whereabouts and day to day activates (NOR, Cronbach’s alpha = .85). In AUS, poor family management was measured by a nine item scale asking adolescents’ agreement to statements such as “My parents ask if I’ve gotten my homework done”, “The rules in my family are clear”, “If you skipped school without your parents’ permission, would you be caught by your parents?”. Response options were: NO! (4), no (3), yes (2), YES! (1) (AUS, Cronbach’s alpha = 0.77). Alcohol and drug use in the family was defined to have occurred if the adolescents reported having experienced that “one of the people closest to me uses too much alcohol, pills or other drugs” one or more times during the past 12 months at t5 and/or t6 (0 = No, 1 = Yes) (NOR) and "Has anyone in your family ever had a severe alcohol or drug problem?" (0 = No, 1 = Yes) (AUS). Past year conduct problems was assessed by a sum score of 19 adolescent-report items taken from three Scandinavian scales of antisocial behaviour (NOR). The items covered stealing, verbally and physically aggressive behaviours, loitering, vandalism, and questions about carrying weapons. The construction of the scale is described in detail elsewhere [53]. The answers were given on an ordinal frequency scale and for this study transformed to a dichotomous (yes/no) variable, creating a variable ranging from 0 to 18 (Cronbach’s alpha = .84). In the AUS sample, conduct problems were measured with nine items covering stealing, physically aggressive behaviour, suspension from school, arrests and questions about carrying weapons, selling drugs and being drunk or high at school. The response options ranged from “never” (1) to “40+ times” (8), (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.64). Early sexual intercourse was assessed by one item asking if the adolescents had ever had sexual intercourse (yes/no) (NOR and AUS). Smoking was assessed by a single item asking if adolescents had smoked at least once in their life (yes/no) (NOR and AUS). Friends’ substance use was assessed by adolescent report on how many of their most important friends; “drinks alcohol approximately once a week”, “have tried hashish, marijuana or other illegal drugs” (NOR) and (in the past 12 months) “how many of your best friends have”; “tried alcohol (like beer, wine, or liquor/spirits) when their parents didn’t know?”, “used marijuana (pot, weed, grass)?”, “used other illegal drugs (like cocaine, heroin, LSD/acid, or amphetamine/speed)?” (AUS). A dummy variable was constructed contrasting those who reported at least one friend with substance use experiences with all others. Gender and the age of the child at the time of responding to the survey were assessed. Gender was dummy-coded (female coded 0 and male coded 1) (NOR and AUS).

Statistical analysis

The relationships of adolescent EOD and EOE with hazardous drinking in late adolescence/young adulthood were examined by means of a modified Poisson regression approach where Poisson regression with robust error variances were estimated. This approach has been recommended instead of binary logistic regression analysis because prevalence ratios (PR) or risk ratios are obtained, thereby providing a more intuitive estimate of the association between predictors and outcomes than odds ratios from logistic regression models [54, 55]. Moreover, Poisson regressions can easily be conducted without difficulties converging. In the present study, the two dummy variables for EOD and EOE were simultaneously included as predictors of hazardous drinking (AUDIT ≥8) in modified Poisson regression analyses together with age and gender. Moreover, a series of Poisson regression analyses was performed to identify associations between each potential confounder with hazardous drinking, controlling for age and gender. Finally, EOD, EOE and all potential confounders were included simultaneously in Poisson regression analyses to examine the unique associations of adolescent EOD and EOE with late adolescent/young adult hazardous drinking. To examine the robustness of our results, we re-ran all analyses by using logistic models. All continuous predictor variables were standardized. Results thus indicate the change in the outcome associated with one standard deviation change in the predictor.
The overall proportions of missing data among those who participated was low. In the NOR sample, the variable with highest level of missing was hyperactivity (4.3%), with proportions of missingness varying from 0.3 to 1.8% for the remaining variables. In the AUS sample, early sexual intercourse (26.4%), and mother’s education (7.1%) showed rather high proportions of missingness, whereas the remaining variables had between 0.3 and 2.5% missing data. Multiple imputation (MI) was used to handle missing data in the predictor and confounder variables, thereby providing missing data routines that are considered the state of art and to be appropriate under missing at random (MAR) conditions [56]. As recommended, 20 complete datasets were created by imputation, incorporating all variables of interest [56]. All analysis and MI were conducted separately for the Norwegian and Australian sample. To test the robustness of our results, all analyses were additionally re-run by handling missing data by listwise deletion.

Results

Table 1 provides the descriptive statistics for the study variables. As shown in the table, a total of 60.2% (NOR) and 54.8% (AUS) of the adolescents in the study were girls. At the time of the study, 13.4% of the Australian adolescents and 10.3% of the Norwegian adolescents’ reported alcohol and drug use in the family.
Table 1
Descriptives of study variables
 
Mean
Standard deviation
Norway (n = 329)
Girls (a), %
60.2
 Age (m)
14.58
0.31
 Hyperactivity (a)
1.13
0.53
 Conduct problems (a)
2.60
2.87
 Early sexual intercourse (a), %
8.3
 Smoking (a), %
25.4
 Friends’ substance use (a), %
43.3
Mother’s education (m)
 Primary school (9 years or less), %
3.4
 Secondary school (1–3 years), %
37.5
 Higher education (≤4 years), %
25.7
 Higher education (> 4 years), %
33.4
Poor family management (a)
1.85
0.58
Alcohol and drug use in the family (a), %
10.3
Australia (n = 786)
 Girls (a), %
54.8
 Age (m)
14.89
0.38
 Impulsivity (a)
2.06
0.53
 Conduct problems (a)
1.12
0.30
 Early sexual intercourse (a), %
11.1
 Smoking (a), %
53.0
 Friends’ substance use (a), %
80.0
Mother’s education (m)
 Primary school, %
42.2
 Completed secondary school, %
31.9
 Higher education, %
26.1
Poor family management (a)
1.86
0.48
Alcohol and drug use in the family (a), %
13.4
Note. Variables are measured differently in the Norwegian and Australian samples and values are therefore not directly comparable
(a) = Adolescent self-report; (m) = Mother’s report
The frequency of drinking behaviours in adolescence and late adolescence/young adulthood for both samples is presented in Table 2. In the AUS sample, over half of the adolescents were classified as EOD, whereas 32.2% were classified as EOE. In the NOR sample, about one quarter of the sample was classified as EOD and 13.4% as EOE. Norwegian late adolescents/young adults reported higher rates of hazardous drinking (46.8%) than the Australian late adolescents/young adults (38.9%), with a slight overlap of the confidence intervals of the proportions. As there was a modification in the wording of one of the ten items in the AUDIT (Item 3) in the NOR sample, we also compared AUDIT scores between the NOR and AUS sample when excluding this item. The difference between the samples did not change substantially (mean score for the AUDIT with 10 items: NOR: 7.59 versus AUS: 7.13; mean score when excluding Item 3: NOR: 6.07 versus AUS: 5.68). A further inspection of single items of the AUDIT revealed that late adolescents/young adults in Australia reported a higher frequency of drinking (AUDIT Item 1; 2–3 times a week or more) compared to the Norwegian late adolescents/young adults (AUS: 29.7% versus NOR: 6.5%), while a higher proportion of the Norwegian late adolescents/young adults reported drinking larger amounts of alcohol per drinking occasion (AUDIT Item 2; five drinks or more; NOR: 64.2% versus AUS: 36.6%).
Table 2
Proportion of early onset drinking (EOD), early onset excessive drinking (EOE), abstinent (14–16 years) and hazardous and non-hazardous drinking in late adolescence/young adulthood (18–25) in the Norwegian and Australian sample
 
Norway
Australia
 
n
%
95% CI
n
%
95% CI
Alcohol use in adolescence
 EOD
84
25.5
20.8–30.2
442
56.2
52.7–59.7
 EOE
44
13.4
9.7–17.1
253
32.2
28.9–35.5
 Abstinent
201
61.1
55.8–66.4
91
11.6
9.4–13.8
Alcohol use in late adolescence/young adulthood
 Non-hazardous drinking (AUDIT < 8)
173
53.2
47.8–58.6
477
61.1
57.7–64.5
 Hazardous drinking (AUDIT ≥8)
152
46.8
41.4–52.2
304
38.9
35.5–42.3
Note. EOD, EOE and abstinence are measured differently in the Norwegian and Australian samples and prevalences are therefore not directly comparable
95% CI = 95% confidence interval of percentage estimate
AUDIT Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test
Modified Poisson regression analyses showed a statistically significant association of both EOD and EOE with hazardous drinking while adjusting for gender and age (Table 3, Model 1). EOE was a stronger predictor than EOD of hazardous drinking in both the NOR and the AUS sample: Prevalence ratios indicated a two to three times higher risk of hazardous drinking among those with EOE, compared to those with no early drinking experiences. EOD was also significantly associated with an increasing risk of hazardous drinking, with an about two-fold increased risk of reporting hazardous drinking, compared to abstinence. Among the possible confounding variables, significant relationships between predictors and outcome were revealed in the individual and the family domain for both samples.
Table 3
Results of modified Poisson regressions predicting hazardous drinking (AUDIT ≥8)
 
Hazardous drinking
Hazardous drinking
(AUDIT ≥8) Norway
(AUDIT ≥8) Australia
Model 1a
Model 2b
Model 1a
Model 2b
PR (95% CI)
PR (95% CI)
PR (95% CI)
PR (95% CI)
Individual variables
 EOD relative to abstinent (a)
1.78 (1.39–2.30)***
1.63 (1.25–2.13)***
2.18 (1.35–3.51)**
1.69 (1.03–2.78)*
 EOE relative to abstinent (a)
2.10 (1.52–2.66)***
1.76 (1.19–2.60)**
2.97 (1.84–4.78)***
1.89 (1.12–3.16)*
 Impulsivity (a) (z)
1.18 (1.09–1.29)***
1.07 (0.97–1.17)
 Hyperactivity (a) (z)
1.26 (1.13–1.42)***
1.17 (1.03–1.33)*
 Conduct problems (a) (z)
1.24 (1.14–1.34)***
1.11 (0.97–1.26)
1.10 (1.04–1.17)**
1.00 (0.93–1.07)
 Early sexual intercourse (a)
1.16 (0.79–1.71)
0.72 (0.48–1.08)
1.31 (1.01–1.70)*
1.06 (0.79–1.43)
 Smoking (a)
1.28 (1.01–1.63)*
0.92 (0.71–2.18)
1.66 (1.36–1.99)***
1.31 (1.06–1.61)*
 Friends’ substance use (a)
1.31 (1.04–1.65)*
0.90 (0.69–1.19)
1.72 (1.28–2.33)***
1.24 (0.89–1.72)
 Gender (girl = 0, boy =1) (a)
1.37 (1.09–1.72)**
1.36 (1.07–1.71)*
1.63 (1.36–1.95)***
1.59 (1.33–1.89)***
 Age (m) (z)
1.00 (0.89–1.13)
0.98 (0.88–1.00)
1.00 (0.93–1.10)
0.92 (0.92–1.08)
Family variables
 Mother’s education (m) (z)
1.03 (0.91–1.16)
1.03 (0.92–1.16)
1.03 (0.94–1.13)
1.06 (0.97–1.16)
 Poor family management (a) (z)
1.20 (1.07–1.34)**
1.02 (0.90–1.17)
1.24 (1.15–1.35)***
1.11 (1.01–1.22)*
 Alcohol and drug use in the family (a)
1.51 (1.14–2.01)**
1.13 (0.82–1.53)
1.09 (0.85–1.39)
0.89 (0.69–1.15)
Note. Variables are measured differently in the Norwegian and Australian samples and values are therefore not directly comparable
*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001. PR = Prevalence Ratio; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval of PR
(a) = adolescent self-report; (m) = mother’s report; (z) = standardized variables
aSeparate analyses are conducted for each predictor, with control for age and gender. PR of age and gender are not controlled for covariates
bAll predictor variables are included simultaneously
When conducting Poisson regression analyses with adjustment for all confounders, EOE, although somewhat reduced, remained a significant predictor of hazardous drinking in both samples and prevalence ratios remained marginally higher than for the relationship between EOD and the outcome (see Table 3, Model 2). Likewise, the association between EOD and hazardous drinking remained statistically significant in both samples, even though prevalence ratios were somewhat reduced when including covariates. To further examine the role of EOD and EOE relative to each other, we conducted a new set of analysis where the dummy variables for early onset of drinking behaviour were recoded to set EOD as the reference group. We could thus test whether EOD increased the risk of hazardous drinking compared to EOE, when adjusting for all covariates, but no significant difference between EOD and EOE was found (NOR: PR = 1.08, 95% CI = 0.78–1.50; AUS: PR = 1.11, 95% CI = 0.92–1.35). To explore potential differences in the predictive potential of EOD versus EOE in greater detail, a new set of regression analyses was conducted, where any form for early use (including both drinking moderate amounts of alcohol and drinking excessively) was included as one dummy variable, together with all covariates. Comparable to results where EOD and EOE were included as separate variables in the regression analyses, any form for early alcohol use was significantly related to hazardous drinking in both countries (NOR: PR = 1.65, 95% CI = 1.27–2.14; AUS: PR = 1.72, 95% CI = 1.04–2.82). Moreover, to examine whether EOE predicted later hazardous drinking over and above any form of early use, EOE was included as an additional predictor in the regression equations. In line with the previous findings, results showed early use to significantly predict hazardous drinking (NOR: PR = 1.63, 95% CI =1.25–2.13; AUS: PR = 1.69, 95% CI = 1.03–2.78), whereas EOE did not significantly increase the risk of hazardous drinking over and above any early use (NOR: PR = 1.08, 95% CI = 0.77–1.49; AUS: PR = 1.11, 95% CI = 0.92–1.35).
Few significant relationships were identified among the possible confounding variables in the multivariate analysis (see Table 3, Model 2). In the NOR sample, only hyperactivity and male gender significantly increased the risk of hazardous drinking. In the AUS sample poor family management, smoking and male gender remained as statistically significant predictors of hazardous drinking. We also examined whether the relationship between early drinking behaviours and hazardous drinking was gender-specific, but we found no significant interactions.
Finally, all regression analyses were re-run by handling missing data by listwise deletion and results were compared to analyses when MI was used. The analyses showed no substantial differences in the results for the two ways of handling missing data. Moreover, as another robustness check, all modified Poisson models were re-ran by using logistic regressions. The results showed similar results, but with somewhat higher odds ratios compared to prevalence ratios. Such a difference was expected, as odds ratios in general yield higher values than prevalence rates, particularly when highly prevalent phenomena, such as alcohol use, are investigated [55].

Discussion

This study used prospective, cross-national data to examine the relationship of EOD and EOE with hazardous drinking. The results showed both EOD and EOE in adolescence to be related to an increased risk for alcohol-related problems in late adolescence/young adulthood, with a somewhat stronger association for EOE than EOD. Moreover, the associations were reduced, but remained statistically significant when controlling for a comprehensive number of potential confounders, including risk factors such as conduct problems, alcohol and drug use in the family and friends’ substance use. Even though most risk factors were operationalized somewhat differently in the two samples, the same pattern of findings was identified in both NOR and AUS – two countries with different alcohol policies and drinking cultures.
Our findings support previous longitudinal studies demonstrating that EOD/EOE is related to a high risk alcohol consumption pattern [7, 11, 14, 2830, 33, 57]. Moreover, the marked attenuation of the relationship between EOD/EOE and alcohol-related problems when including potential confounders is also in accordance with previous empirical studies (for a review, see [18]). However, somewhat surprisingly, the association between early drinking behaviours and hazardous drinking remained statistically significant, even after control for a comprehensive number of possible confounders. Our results thus indicate that adolescents with EOE/EOD are two to three times more likely to be high risk drinkers in late adolescence/young adulthood. Such findings are in contrast to previous prospective studies in which associations between early drinking behaviour and later high risk drinking patterns were completely explained by relevant covariates [16, 31, 32], but well in line with the findings of Buchmann et al. [4] and Irons et al. [11].
The divergent findings may be explained by variations in the age at which problematic drinking patterns were assessed. Typically, in studies assessing adult outcomes at ages 25 and above [16, 31, 32], associations between early drinking behaviour and later high risk drinking were not detected. In contrast, the study of Buchmann et al. [4], Irons et al. [11], and the present study, which detected associations between early drinking behaviour and later high risk drinking assessed later age drinking at 18 to 25 years. This pattern of findings may suggest that the effect of EOD/EOE on later high risk drinking may be particularly potent in late adolescence/young adulthood.
Excessive drinking typically reaches a peak in late adolescence/young adulthood, and then gradually decreases with increasing age, accompanied by changing roles and obligations as one moves through different life phases [5860]. A high risk drinking pattern, in the late adolescence/young adult life phase, where excessive drinking is “normative” and highly prevalent, may to a larger degree be based on social and environmental factors – including early alcohol exposure, relative to later high risk drinking. Maintenance of an excessive drinking pattern throughout adulthood is typically restricted to a smaller group of individuals characterized by more severe and long-lasting problems [60]. This drinking behaviour may be better understood in the framework of underlying genetic liability, than in light of social and environmental factors. Such a notion is supported by several genetically informed studies showing that heritability estimates increase with increasing age of the individuals and increasing severity of alcohol-related problems [61, 62]. EOD may become a non-significant predictor of adult alcohol problems when adjusting for factors thought to reflect genetic transmission or vulnerability, such as externalizing behaviours and parental substance use. In contrast, early drinking experiences in themselves may function as an early transition to a drinking culture, and may thus increase the risk for hazardous drinking in social contexts, particularly in late adolescence/young adulthood where such behaviour is considered normative.
The scientific community has over the past couple of decades discussed whether more than just a few sips of alcohol per se or having consumed more substantial amounts of alcohol at an early age are related to adverse outcomes (e.g., [12, 33, 63]). The present study shows rather minor differences in how EOD versus EOE prospectively predicts later hazardous drinking behaviour. There could be several explanations for such a finding. First, early drinking behaviours, regardless of level of alcohol consumption, may be indicators of the same proneness to risk-taking behaviours, including drinking. However, previous analyses of predictors of EOD and EOE in the NOR sample suggest that adolescents with EOD may be less inclined to take risks than adolescents with EOE, as lower levels of shyness, conduct problems and high levels of risk exposure in friendship networks were unrelated to EOD, but prospectively predicted EOE [64]. Thus, adolescents with EOE are in fact more likely to show characteristics indicating higher proneness to risk taking behaviour than EOD adolescents.
Alternatively, drawing on neurobiological and psychosocial development literature, there may be similar biological mechanisms whereby alcohol exposure leads to later alcohol-related problems [35, 36]. Based on our findings, we cannot dismiss such a biological basis, although further investigations of these relationships with a more fine-grained measure of different levels of early alcohol exposure is warranted. Furthermore, this hypothesis would be strengthened if the relationship between early onset and alcohol-related problems more consistently demonstrated a robust relationship across different studies. Our findings are more in line with an understanding of early onset, regardless of level consumed, as an introduction to a “wet” environment, enabling a progression to a hazardous drinking pattern. Interestingly, similar patterns in the relationship between early onset and risk of alcohol-related problems were identified in both the NOR and AUS samples, despite the very different proportions of adolescent drinking. The results indicate that early alcohol use seems be cross-nationally invariant and to a lesser degree is influenced by differences in cultural or social norms and national alcohol policies. Furthermore, rates of hazardous drinking were higher in the NOR sample despite the much greater adolescent abstinence. This finding indicates that EOD/EOE cannot fully explain hazardous drinking in late adolescence/young adulthood and that there are likely to be important contextual correlates that affect this behaviour.

Strengths and limitations

By using prospective studies, we minimized the potential bias caused by retrospective recall and forward telescoping that are often noted as major limitations in the literature on early onset of alcohol use [65]. The inclusion of a wide range of potential confounding factors, consideration of the independent importance of different early drinking behaviours and the use of two samples from different cultural contexts, make this study an important contribution to the literature on consequences of early drinking patterns. However, several considerations are important to note. First, the measures of early drinking behaviours give us no indication of the actual volume of alcohol consumed, although the better differentiation of early drinking experiences into EOD and EOE improves upon previous studies. Second, the NOR and AUS studies were designed as two separate studies. Consequently, the predictor variables and covariates were assessed differently in the Norwegian and the Australian samples and are not directly comparable. For example, the higher prevalence of early adolescent drinking in the AUS sample may not necessarily reflect different drinking patterns between the countries, because drinking was assessed differently in the two samples. Another limitation is that the two samples differed in the age when hazardous drinking was assessed in “late adolescence/young adulthood”. While the Norwegian sample had just reached the legal drinking age (mean age 18.9 years), the Australian participants were on average 22.9 years old. Such differences may explain different rates of hazardous drinking in the two samples, and it is possible that the Australian participants have “settled down” somewhat as compared to the Norwegian adolescents, who may just have moved away from home and take full advantage of their new won freedom. Still, the age at which most adolescents start to drink is very similar in many Western countries. Around 50% or more of 15–16 year olds have initiated alcohol use [66]. As such, many adolescents have several years of experience with alcohol before reaching young adulthood. Despite different measures and age ranges, the patterns identified across samples were similar, indicating a robust association across countries with different alcohol policies and drinking cultures. Third, even though a comprehensive number of potential confounders were included, we did not have access to information about parents’ drinking patterns, physiological sensitivity to the effects of alcohol or genetics. The relation between early drinking behaviour and hazardous drinking could thus be caused by unmeasured common risk factors. Another concern is that information about the adolescent drinking context was not available in this study. Fourth, some measures had somewhat low internal reliability. Particularly, the three item impulsivity scale used in the Australian sample displayed low Cronbach’s alpha, with a value of .57. Pearson’s correlations, which the Cronbach’s alpha is based on, holds an assumption of normally distributed continuous variables. The items in the impulsivity scale are on an ordinal scale, with few response categories, and a slightly skewed distribution. As such, calculation of alpha based on Pearson’s correlation may lead to underestimation of the true association. In such cases, alternative strategies to estimate reliability have been suggested, such as calculating Cronbach’s alpha based on polychoric correlations, providing a more accurate estimate of the relationship between the items on the scale [67]. In our case, such an alternative estimation strategy increased internal consistency somewhat to α = .63. Even though it is common to find low alphas in scales consisting of few items, we cannot rule out that a more reliable assessment of impulsivity could have affected the results in our study. Fifth, AUDIT scores for the adolescent waves were not available, thereby not providing us with the possibility to examine alcohol-related problems in greater detail or to account for already existing problems when examining the relationship between early onset of alcohol use and later alcohol-related problems. Lastly, the substantial attrition rate in the NOR sample is a major limitation of the study, as only data from 30.4% of the originally invited families were available at both time points of interest. Although attrition rates of 50% and more are not uncommon in longitudinal studies [68], such high attrition rates may be a source of bias. Of particular concern is the fact that attrition in the NOR sample was predicted by low maternal education level and male gender, resulting in an overrepresentation of girls and adolescents whose mothers have higher levels of education. This may represent a threat to the representativeness, and our study provides limited knowledge about how early drinking behaviours are related to later alcohol outcomes in adolescents whose mothers have a low level of education. Moreover, the selective drop-out warrants caution when interpreting the results regarding gender, maternal education and family variables, even though attrition and simulation studies based on the NOR sample have shown that selective attrition leads only to biased estimates of means of variables; estimates of associations between variables were not affected even with selective attrition and high attrition rates [69]. Still, research on these associations in low socio-economic strata will be of importance in future studies. The AUS sample is a Victoria sample, and so may be limited in its generalisability to the whole country.

Conclusion

In summary, this study used prospective, cross-national data to examine the relationship of EOD and EOE with hazardous drinking in late adolescence/young adulthood. Our findings indicate that early drinking behaviour, regardless of level, is an indicator of alcohol-related problems in late adolescence/young adulthood even when controlling for a variety of covariates. This pattern is consistent across samples drawn from two countries with different prevention policies and drinking cultures. Our findings are in contrast to previous research on older adults, where no association between adolescent drinking and later alcohol problems was found when controlling for covariates. The divergence in findings may suggest that the impact of EOD/EOE is limited to the late adolescent and young adult period. We suggest that EOD and EOE play a role first and foremost as an introduction to a drinking culture enabling a progression to a hazardous drinking pattern. Efforts to reduce early age drinking and reduce the progression from experimental/minor alcohol use to instances of EOE may have some impact on the risk of alcohol-related problems in late adolescence/young adulthood. Future research should examine the role of early drinking behaviours and indicators of alcohol-related problems in different life phases.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful for the voluntary effort of all the Norwegian and Australian families who have participated in the TOPP and IYDS studies over the years. We also want to give thanks to all the TOPP and IYDS project staff.
The Norwegian TOPP study has been approved by the Norwegian Data Protection Authority and the Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics (REC) South East Norway. The TOPP study started in 1992/1993 when written consent in research studies was not a requirement. However, in later years, REC has reviewed all information given to participants and concluded that the study fulfils essential requirements for informed consent. Study participation was voluntary. Information about the study, the possibility to skip questions, and the right to withdraw from the study at any point, was provided in writing to both the children and their mothers for each data collection. The children were invited through their mothers, who were given questionnaires and separate envelopes for themselves and their children. In the Australian IYDS study, ethics approval was first gained through the Ethics in Human Research Office at the Royal Children’s Hospital in Victoria. At time 1, permission was then gained from the Victorian Department of Education and Training for government schools and the Catholic Education Office for some private schools and then by school principals. Parents provided written consent for their child to participate in the study and students provided assent to complete the survey. At time 7, ethics approval was gained through The University of Melbourne Human Ethics in Research Committee and young adult participants provided online assent at the time of survey completion.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Grant BF, Dawson DA. Age at onset of alcohol use and its association with DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence: results from the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. J Subst Abus. 1997;9:103–10.CrossRef Grant BF, Dawson DA. Age at onset of alcohol use and its association with DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence: results from the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. J Subst Abus. 1997;9:103–10.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat DeWit DJ, Adlaf EM, Offord DR, Ogborne AC. Age at first alcohol use: a risk factor for the development of alcohol disorders. Am J Psychiatr. 2000;157(5):745–50.PubMedCrossRef DeWit DJ, Adlaf EM, Offord DR, Ogborne AC. Age at first alcohol use: a risk factor for the development of alcohol disorders. Am J Psychiatr. 2000;157(5):745–50.PubMedCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Hawkins JD, Graham JW, Maguin E, Abbott R, Hill KG, Catalano RF. Exploring the effects of age of alcohol use initiation and psychosocial risk factors on subsequent alcohol misuse. J Stud Alcohol. 1997;58(3):280.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hawkins JD, Graham JW, Maguin E, Abbott R, Hill KG, Catalano RF. Exploring the effects of age of alcohol use initiation and psychosocial risk factors on subsequent alcohol misuse. J Stud Alcohol. 1997;58(3):280.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Buchmann AF, Schmid B, Blomeyer D, Becker K, Treutlein J, Zimmermann US, Jennen-Steinmetz C, Schmidt MH, Esser G, Banaschewski T. Impact of age at first drink on vulnerability to alcohol-related problems: testing the marker hypothesis in a prospective study of young adults. J Psychiatr Res. 2009;43(15):1205–12.PubMedCrossRef Buchmann AF, Schmid B, Blomeyer D, Becker K, Treutlein J, Zimmermann US, Jennen-Steinmetz C, Schmidt MH, Esser G, Banaschewski T. Impact of age at first drink on vulnerability to alcohol-related problems: testing the marker hypothesis in a prospective study of young adults. J Psychiatr Res. 2009;43(15):1205–12.PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Hingson RW, Zha W. Age of drinking onset, alcohol use disorders, frequent heavy drinking, and unintentionally injuring oneself and others after drinking. Pediatrics. 2009;123(6):1477–84.PubMedCrossRef Hingson RW, Zha W. Age of drinking onset, alcohol use disorders, frequent heavy drinking, and unintentionally injuring oneself and others after drinking. Pediatrics. 2009;123(6):1477–84.PubMedCrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Hingson RW, Heeren T, Winter MR. Age at drinking onset and alcohol dependence: age at onset, duration, and severity. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2006;160(7):739–46.PubMedCrossRef Hingson RW, Heeren T, Winter MR. Age at drinking onset and alcohol dependence: age at onset, duration, and severity. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2006;160(7):739–46.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Fergusson DM, Lynskey MT, Horwood L. Childhood exposure to alcohol and adolescent drinking patterns. Addiction. 1994;89(8):1007–16.PubMedCrossRef Fergusson DM, Lynskey MT, Horwood L. Childhood exposure to alcohol and adolescent drinking patterns. Addiction. 1994;89(8):1007–16.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat McGue M, Iacono WG, Legrand LN, Malone S, Elkins I. Origins and consequences of age at first drink. I. Associations with substance-use disorders, disinhibitory behavior and psychopathology, and P3 amplitude. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2001;25(8):1156–65.PubMedCrossRef McGue M, Iacono WG, Legrand LN, Malone S, Elkins I. Origins and consequences of age at first drink. I. Associations with substance-use disorders, disinhibitory behavior and psychopathology, and P3 amplitude. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2001;25(8):1156–65.PubMedCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Prescott CA, Kendler KS. Age at first drink and risk for alcoholism: a noncausal association. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1999;23(1):101–7.PubMed Prescott CA, Kendler KS. Age at first drink and risk for alcoholism: a noncausal association. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1999;23(1):101–7.PubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Liang W, Chikritzhs T. Age at first use of alcohol predicts the risk of heavy alcohol use in early adulthood: a longitudinal study in the United States. Int J Drug Pol. 2015;26(2):131–4.CrossRef Liang W, Chikritzhs T. Age at first use of alcohol predicts the risk of heavy alcohol use in early adulthood: a longitudinal study in the United States. Int J Drug Pol. 2015;26(2):131–4.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Irons DE, Iacono WG, McGue M. Tests of the effects of adolescent early alcohol exposures on adult outcomes. Addiction. 2015;110(2):269–78.PubMedCrossRef Irons DE, Iacono WG, McGue M. Tests of the effects of adolescent early alcohol exposures on adult outcomes. Addiction. 2015;110(2):269–78.PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Morean ME, Corbin WR, Fromme K. Age of first use and delay to first intoxication in relation to trajectories of heavy drinking and alcohol-related problems during emerging adulthood. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2012;36(11):1991–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Morean ME, Corbin WR, Fromme K. Age of first use and delay to first intoxication in relation to trajectories of heavy drinking and alcohol-related problems during emerging adulthood. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2012;36(11):1991–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Asbridge M, Cartwright J, Wilson K, Langille D. Age at first frink, experiences of drunkenness, and alcohol-related problems in Canadian youth: is early onset bad if you are a moderate drinker? J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2016;77(6):974–9.PubMedCrossRef Asbridge M, Cartwright J, Wilson K, Langille D. Age at first frink, experiences of drunkenness, and alcohol-related problems in Canadian youth: is early onset bad if you are a moderate drinker? J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2016;77(6):974–9.PubMedCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Pedersen W, Skrondal A. Alcohol consumption debut: predictors and consequences. J Stud Alcohol. 1998;59(1):32–42.PubMedCrossRef Pedersen W, Skrondal A. Alcohol consumption debut: predictors and consequences. J Stud Alcohol. 1998;59(1):32–42.PubMedCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Dawson DA, Goldstein RB, Patricia Chou S, June Ruan W, Grant BF. Age at first drink and the first incidence of adult-onset DSM-IV alcohol use disorders. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32(12):2149–60.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Dawson DA, Goldstein RB, Patricia Chou S, June Ruan W, Grant BF. Age at first drink and the first incidence of adult-onset DSM-IV alcohol use disorders. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32(12):2149–60.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Rossow I, Kuntsche E. Early onset of drinking and risk of heavy drinking in young adulthood—a 13-year prospective study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2013;37(1):E297–304.PubMedCrossRef Rossow I, Kuntsche E. Early onset of drinking and risk of heavy drinking in young adulthood—a 13-year prospective study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2013;37(1):E297–304.PubMedCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuntsche E, Rossow I, Engels R, Kuntsche S. Is ‘age at first drink’a useful concept in alcohol research and prevention? We doubt that. Addiction. 2015. Kuntsche E, Rossow I, Engels R, Kuntsche S. Is ‘age at first drink’a useful concept in alcohol research and prevention? We doubt that. Addiction. 2015.
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Maimaris W, McCambridge J. Age of first drinking and adult alcohol problems: systematic review of prospective cohort studies. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2014;68(3):268–74.PubMedCrossRef Maimaris W, McCambridge J. Age of first drinking and adult alcohol problems: systematic review of prospective cohort studies. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2014;68(3):268–74.PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Magid V, Moreland AD. The role of substance use initiation in adolescent development of subsequent substance-related problems. J Child Adolesc Subst Abuse. 2014;23(2):78–86.CrossRef Magid V, Moreland AD. The role of substance use initiation in adolescent development of subsequent substance-related problems. J Child Adolesc Subst Abuse. 2014;23(2):78–86.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Geels LM, Vink JM, Van Beijsterveldt CE, Bartels M, Boomsma DI. Developmental prediction model for early alcohol initiation in Dutch adolescents. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2013;74(1):59–70.PubMedCrossRef Geels LM, Vink JM, Van Beijsterveldt CE, Bartels M, Boomsma DI. Developmental prediction model for early alcohol initiation in Dutch adolescents. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2013;74(1):59–70.PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Shillington AM, Woodruff SI, Clapp JD, Reed MB, Lemus H. Self-reported age of onset and telescoping for cigarettes, alcohol, and marijuana: across eight years of the National Longitudinal Survey of youth. J Child Adolesc Subst Abuse. 2012;21(4):333–48.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Shillington AM, Woodruff SI, Clapp JD, Reed MB, Lemus H. Self-reported age of onset and telescoping for cigarettes, alcohol, and marijuana: across eight years of the National Longitudinal Survey of youth. J Child Adolesc Subst Abuse. 2012;21(4):333–48.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Sartor CE, Bucholz KK, Nelson EC, Madden PA, Lynskey MT, Heath AC. Reporting bias in the association between age at first alcohol use and heavy episodic drinking. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35(8):1418–25.PubMedPubMedCentral Sartor CE, Bucholz KK, Nelson EC, Madden PA, Lynskey MT, Heath AC. Reporting bias in the association between age at first alcohol use and heavy episodic drinking. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35(8):1418–25.PubMedPubMedCentral
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Tarter R, Vanyukov M, Giancola P, Dawes M, Blackson T, Mezzich ADA, Clark DB. Etiology of early age onset substance use disorder: a maturational perspective. Dev Psychopathol. 1999;11(4):657–83.PubMedCrossRef Tarter R, Vanyukov M, Giancola P, Dawes M, Blackson T, Mezzich ADA, Clark DB. Etiology of early age onset substance use disorder: a maturational perspective. Dev Psychopathol. 1999;11(4):657–83.PubMedCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Scholes-Balog KE, Hemphill S, Reid S, Patton G, Toumbourou J. Predicting early initiation of alcohol use: a prospective study of Australian children. Subst Use Misuse. 2013;48(4):343–52.PubMedCrossRef Scholes-Balog KE, Hemphill S, Reid S, Patton G, Toumbourou J. Predicting early initiation of alcohol use: a prospective study of Australian children. Subst Use Misuse. 2013;48(4):343–52.PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuntsche E, van der Vorst H, Engels R. The earlier the more? Differences in the links between age at first drink and adolescent alcohol use and related problems according to quality of parent-child relationships. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2009;70(3):346–54.PubMedCrossRef Kuntsche E, van der Vorst H, Engels R. The earlier the more? Differences in the links between age at first drink and adolescent alcohol use and related problems according to quality of parent-child relationships. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2009;70(3):346–54.PubMedCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Mason WA, Toumbourou JW, Herrenkohl TI, Hemphill SA, Catalano RF, Patton GC. Early age alcohol use and later alcohol problems in adolescents: individual and peer mediators in a bi-national study. Psychol Addict Behav. 2011;25(4):625.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mason WA, Toumbourou JW, Herrenkohl TI, Hemphill SA, Catalano RF, Patton GC. Early age alcohol use and later alcohol problems in adolescents: individual and peer mediators in a bi-national study. Psychol Addict Behav. 2011;25(4):625.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim MJ, Mason WA, Herrenkohl TI, Catalano RF, Toumbourou JW, Hemphill SA. Influence of early onset of alcohol use on the development of adolescent alcohol problems: a longitudinal binational study. Prev Sci. 2017;18(1):1–11.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kim MJ, Mason WA, Herrenkohl TI, Catalano RF, Toumbourou JW, Hemphill SA. Influence of early onset of alcohol use on the development of adolescent alcohol problems: a longitudinal binational study. Prev Sci. 2017;18(1):1–11.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Guttmannova K, Bailey JA, Hill KG, Lee JO, Hawkins JD, Woods ML, Catalano RF. Sensitive periods for adolescent alcohol use initiation: predicting the lifetime occurrence and chronicity of alcohol problems in adulthood. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2011;72(2):221–31.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Guttmannova K, Bailey JA, Hill KG, Lee JO, Hawkins JD, Woods ML, Catalano RF. Sensitive periods for adolescent alcohol use initiation: predicting the lifetime occurrence and chronicity of alcohol problems in adulthood. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2011;72(2):221–31.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Pitkänen T, Lyyra AL, Pulkkinen L. Age of onset of drinking and the use of alcohol in adulthood: a follow-up study from age 8–42 for females and males. Addiction. 2005;100(5):652–61.PubMedCrossRef Pitkänen T, Lyyra AL, Pulkkinen L. Age of onset of drinking and the use of alcohol in adulthood: a follow-up study from age 8–42 for females and males. Addiction. 2005;100(5):652–61.PubMedCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Riala K, Hakko H, Isohanni M, Järvelin M-R, Räsänen P. Teenage smoking and substance use as predictors of severe alcohol problems in late adolescence and in young adulthood. J Adolesc Health. 2004;35(3):245–54.PubMedCrossRef Riala K, Hakko H, Isohanni M, Järvelin M-R, Räsänen P. Teenage smoking and substance use as predictors of severe alcohol problems in late adolescence and in young adulthood. J Adolesc Health. 2004;35(3):245–54.PubMedCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Newton-Howes G, Boden JM. Relation between age of first drinking and mental health and alcohol and drug disorders in adulthood: evidence from a 35-year cohort study. Addiction. 2015. Newton-Howes G, Boden JM. Relation between age of first drinking and mental health and alcohol and drug disorders in adulthood: evidence from a 35-year cohort study. Addiction. 2015.
32.
Zurück zum Zitat King KM, Chassin L. A prospective study of the effects of age of initiation of alcohol and drug use on young adult substance dependence. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2007;68(2):256–65.PubMedCrossRef King KM, Chassin L. A prospective study of the effects of age of initiation of alcohol and drug use on young adult substance dependence. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2007;68(2):256–65.PubMedCrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Warner LA, White HR. Longitudinal effects of age at onset and first drinking situations on problem drinking. Subst Use Misuse. 2003;38(14):1983–2016.PubMedCrossRef Warner LA, White HR. Longitudinal effects of age at onset and first drinking situations on problem drinking. Subst Use Misuse. 2003;38(14):1983–2016.PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuntsche E, Rossow I, Simons-Morton B, Bogt TT, Kokkevi A, Godeau E. Not early drinking but early drunkenness is a risk factor for problem behaviors among adolescents from 38 European and north American countries. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2013;37(2):308–14.PubMedCrossRef Kuntsche E, Rossow I, Simons-Morton B, Bogt TT, Kokkevi A, Godeau E. Not early drinking but early drunkenness is a risk factor for problem behaviors among adolescents from 38 European and north American countries. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2013;37(2):308–14.PubMedCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Weissman DG, Schriber RA, Fassbender C, Atherton O, Krafft C, Robins RW, Hastings PD, Guyer AE. Earlier adolescent substance use onset predicts stronger connectivity between reward and cognitive control brain networks. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2015;16:121–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Weissman DG, Schriber RA, Fassbender C, Atherton O, Krafft C, Robins RW, Hastings PD, Guyer AE. Earlier adolescent substance use onset predicts stronger connectivity between reward and cognitive control brain networks. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2015;16:121–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Spear LP. The adolescent brain and the college drinker: biological basis of propensity to use and misuse alcohol. J Stud Alcohol Suppl. 2002;(s14):71–81. Spear LP. The adolescent brain and the college drinker: biological basis of propensity to use and misuse alcohol. J Stud Alcohol Suppl. 2002;(s14):71–81.
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Bendtsen P, Damsgaard MT, Huckle T, Casswell S, Kuntsche E, Arnold P, Looze ME, Hofmann F, Hublet A, Simons-Morton B. Adolescent alcohol use: a reflection of national drinking patterns and policy? Addiction. 2014;109(11):1857–68.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Bendtsen P, Damsgaard MT, Huckle T, Casswell S, Kuntsche E, Arnold P, Looze ME, Hofmann F, Hublet A, Simons-Morton B. Adolescent alcohol use: a reflection of national drinking patterns and policy? Addiction. 2014;109(11):1857–68.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Österberg E, Karlsson T: Alcohol policies in EU member states and Norway. A collection of country reports 2002. Österberg E, Karlsson T: Alcohol policies in EU member states and Norway. A collection of country reports 2002.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Meld. St. 30 (2011–2012): Se meg! En helhetlig rusmiddelpolitikk. Alkohol - narkotika - doping. In.: Helse- og omsorgsdepartementet; 2012. Meld. St. 30 (2011–2012): Se meg! En helhetlig rusmiddelpolitikk. Alkohol - narkotika - doping. In.: Helse- og omsorgsdepartementet; 2012.
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Støttemateriell: Rusforebyggende arbeid i skolen - forslag til læringsaktiviteter In.: Helsedirektoratet.; 2012. Støttemateriell: Rusforebyggende arbeid i skolen - forslag til læringsaktiviteter In.: Helsedirektoratet.; 2012.
41.
Zurück zum Zitat National Drug Strategy 2010-2015. A framework for action on alcohol, tobacco and other drugs. Australia: Ministerial Council on Drug Strategy; 2011. National Drug Strategy 2010-2015. A framework for action on alcohol, tobacco and other drugs. Australia: Ministerial Council on Drug Strategy; 2011.
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Toumbourou JW, Williams IR, White VM, Snow PC, Munro GD, Schofield PE. Prediction of alcohol-related harm from controlled drinking strategies and alcohol consumption trajectories. Addiction. 2004;99(4):498–508.PubMedCrossRef Toumbourou JW, Williams IR, White VM, Snow PC, Munro GD, Schofield PE. Prediction of alcohol-related harm from controlled drinking strategies and alcohol consumption trajectories. Addiction. 2004;99(4):498–508.PubMedCrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Vedøy TF, Skretting A: Ungdom og rusmidler. Resultater fra spørreskjemaundersøkelser 1968–2008. In., vol. 5/2009. Oslo: Statens institutt for rusmiddelforskning; 2009. Vedøy TF, Skretting A: Ungdom og rusmidler. Resultater fra spørreskjemaundersøkelser 1968–2008. In., vol. 5/2009. Oslo: Statens institutt for rusmiddelforskning; 2009.
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Global status report on alcohol and health (2014). In. Geneva: World Helath Organization; 2014. Global status report on alcohol and health (2014). In. Geneva: World Helath Organization; 2014.
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Nilsen W, Kjeldsen A, Karevold EB, Skipstein A, Helland M, Gustavson K, Enstad F, Baardstu S, Røysamb E, von Soest T, et al. Cohort profile: the tracking opportunities and problems study (TOPP) – a study of Norwegian children and their parents followed from infancy to adulthood. Int J Epidemiol. 2017. Nilsen W, Kjeldsen A, Karevold EB, Skipstein A, Helland M, Gustavson K, Enstad F, Baardstu S, Røysamb E, von Soest T, et al. Cohort profile: the tracking opportunities and problems study (TOPP) – a study of Norwegian children and their parents followed from infancy to adulthood. Int J Epidemiol. 2017.
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Nilsen W, Gustavson K, Røysamb E, Kjeldsen A, Karevold E. Pathways from maternal distress and child problem behavior to adolescent depressive symptoms: a prospective examination from early childhood to adolescence. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2013;34(5):303–13.CrossRef Nilsen W, Gustavson K, Røysamb E, Kjeldsen A, Karevold E. Pathways from maternal distress and child problem behavior to adolescent depressive symptoms: a prospective examination from early childhood to adolescence. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2013;34(5):303–13.CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Kjeldsen A, Nilsen W, Gustavson K, Skipstein A, Melkevik O, Karevold EB. Predicting well-being and internalizing symptoms in late adolescence from trajectories of externalizing behavior starting in infancy. J Res Adolesc. 2016;26(4):991–1008.PubMedCrossRef Kjeldsen A, Nilsen W, Gustavson K, Skipstein A, Melkevik O, Karevold EB. Predicting well-being and internalizing symptoms in late adolescence from trajectories of externalizing behavior starting in infancy. J Res Adolesc. 2016;26(4):991–1008.PubMedCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat McMorris BJ, Hemphill SA, Toumbourou JW, Catalano RF, Patton GC. Prevalence of substance use and delinquent behavior in adolescents from Victoria, Australia and Washington state, United States. Health Educ Behav. 2007;34(4):634–50.PubMedCrossRef McMorris BJ, Hemphill SA, Toumbourou JW, Catalano RF, Patton GC. Prevalence of substance use and delinquent behavior in adolescents from Victoria, Australia and Washington state, United States. Health Educ Behav. 2007;34(4):634–50.PubMedCrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, De la Fuente JR, Grant M. Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption-II. Addiction. 1993;88(6):791–804.PubMedCrossRef Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, De la Fuente JR, Grant M. Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption-II. Addiction. 1993;88(6):791–804.PubMedCrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Reinert DF, Allen JP. The alcohol use disorders identification test: an update of research findings. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31(2):185–99.PubMedCrossRef Reinert DF, Allen JP. The alcohol use disorders identification test: an update of research findings. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31(2):185–99.PubMedCrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Babor TF, Higgins-Biddle JC, Saunders JB, Monteiro MG. AUDIT: the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): guidelines for use in primary care. 2nd ed. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2001. Babor TF, Higgins-Biddle JC, Saunders JB, Monteiro MG. AUDIT: the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): guidelines for use in primary care. 2nd ed. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2001.
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Goodman R, Meltzer H, Bailey V. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a pilot study on the validity of the self-report version. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatr. 1998;7(3):125–30.CrossRef Goodman R, Meltzer H, Bailey V. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a pilot study on the validity of the self-report version. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatr. 1998;7(3):125–30.CrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Kjeldsen A, Janson H, Stoolmiller M, Torgersen L, Mathiesen KS. Externalising behaviour from infancy to mid-adolescence: latent profiles and early predictors. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2014;35(1):25–34.CrossRef Kjeldsen A, Janson H, Stoolmiller M, Torgersen L, Mathiesen KS. Externalising behaviour from infancy to mid-adolescence: latent profiles and early predictors. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2014;35(1):25–34.CrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Zou G. A modified poisson regression approach to prospective studies with binary data. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;159(7):702–6.PubMedCrossRef Zou G. A modified poisson regression approach to prospective studies with binary data. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;159(7):702–6.PubMedCrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Espelt A, Marí-Dell’Olmo M, Penelo E, Bosque-Prous M. Applied prevalence ratio estimation with different regression models: an example from a cross-national study on substance use research. Adicciones. 2016;29(2):105–12.PubMedCrossRef Espelt A, Marí-Dell’Olmo M, Penelo E, Bosque-Prous M. Applied prevalence ratio estimation with different regression models: an example from a cross-national study on substance use research. Adicciones. 2016;29(2):105–12.PubMedCrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Schafer JL, Graham JW. Missing data: our view of the state of the art. Psychol Methods. 2002;7(2):147.PubMedCrossRef Schafer JL, Graham JW. Missing data: our view of the state of the art. Psychol Methods. 2002;7(2):147.PubMedCrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Warner LA, White HR, Johnson V. Alcohol initiation experiences and family history of alcoholism as predictors of problem-drinking trajectories. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2007;68(1):56–65.PubMedCrossRef Warner LA, White HR, Johnson V. Alcohol initiation experiences and family history of alcoholism as predictors of problem-drinking trajectories. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2007;68(1):56–65.PubMedCrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuntsche E, Rehm J, Gmel G. Characteristics of binge drinkers in Europe. Soc Sci Med. 2004;59(1):113–27.PubMedCrossRef Kuntsche E, Rehm J, Gmel G. Characteristics of binge drinkers in Europe. Soc Sci Med. 2004;59(1):113–27.PubMedCrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Chassin L, Sher KJ, Hussong A, Curran P. The developmental psychopathology of alcohol use and alcohol disorders: research achievements and future directions. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25(402):1567–84.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Chassin L, Sher KJ, Hussong A, Curran P. The developmental psychopathology of alcohol use and alcohol disorders: research achievements and future directions. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25(402):1567–84.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Schulenberg JE, Maggs JL. A developmental perspective on alcohol use and heavy drinking during adolescence and the transition to young adulthood. J Stud Alcohol Suppl. 2002;(s14):54–70. Schulenberg JE, Maggs JL. A developmental perspective on alcohol use and heavy drinking during adolescence and the transition to young adulthood. J Stud Alcohol Suppl. 2002;(s14):54–70.
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Kendler KS, Schmitt E, Aggen SH, Prescott CA. Genetic and environmental influences on alcohol, caffeine, cannabis, and nicotine use from early adolescence to middle adulthood. Arch Gen Psychiatr. 2008;65(6):674–82.PubMedCrossRef Kendler KS, Schmitt E, Aggen SH, Prescott CA. Genetic and environmental influences on alcohol, caffeine, cannabis, and nicotine use from early adolescence to middle adulthood. Arch Gen Psychiatr. 2008;65(6):674–82.PubMedCrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Walters GD. The heritability of alcohol abuse and dependence: a meta-analysis of behavior genetic research. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2002;28(3):557–84.PubMedCrossRef Walters GD. The heritability of alcohol abuse and dependence: a meta-analysis of behavior genetic research. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2002;28(3):557–84.PubMedCrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Monshouwer K, Smit F, de Zwart WM, Spruit I, van Ameijden EJ. Progress from a first drink to first intoxication: age of onset, time-windows and risk factors in a Dutch national sample of secondary school students. J Subst Use. 2003;8(3):155–63.CrossRef Monshouwer K, Smit F, de Zwart WM, Spruit I, van Ameijden EJ. Progress from a first drink to first intoxication: age of onset, time-windows and risk factors in a Dutch national sample of secondary school students. J Subst Use. 2003;8(3):155–63.CrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Enstad F, Pedersen W, Nilsen W, von Soest T. Predicting early onset of intoxication versus drinking—a population-based prospective study of Norwegian adolescents. Addict Behav Rep. 2017;6:1–7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Enstad F, Pedersen W, Nilsen W, von Soest T. Predicting early onset of intoxication versus drinking—a population-based prospective study of Norwegian adolescents. Addict Behav Rep. 2017;6:1–7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Rossow I. Inferences of associations and implications for prevention: the case of early drinking onset. Understanding Choice, Explaining Behaviour; 2006. Rossow I. Inferences of associations and implications for prevention: the case of early drinking onset. Understanding Choice, Explaining Behaviour; 2006.
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Kraus L, Nociar A: ESPAD report 2015: results from the European school survey project on alcohol and other drugs: European monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction; 2016. Kraus L, Nociar A: ESPAD report 2015: results from the European school survey project on alcohol and other drugs: European monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction; 2016.
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Gadermann AM, Guhn M, Zumbo BD. Estimating ordinal reliability for Likert-type and ordinal item response data&58; a conceptual, empirical, and practical guide. Practical Assessment. 2012;17(3):1–13. Gadermann AM, Guhn M, Zumbo BD. Estimating ordinal reliability for Likert-type and ordinal item response data&58; a conceptual, empirical, and practical guide. Practical Assessment. 2012;17(3):1–13.
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Van der Kamp LJT, Bijleveld, C. C. J. H. : Methodological issues in longitudinal research. In: Longitudinal data analysis: designs, models and methods. edn. Edited by C. C. J. H. Bijleveld LJTvdK, A. Mooijaart, W. A. van der Kloot, R. van der Leeden, & E. Van Der Burg. London, England Sage; 1998: 1–44. Van der Kamp LJT, Bijleveld, C. C. J. H. : Methodological issues in longitudinal research. In: Longitudinal data analysis: designs, models and methods. edn. Edited by C. C. J. H. Bijleveld LJTvdK, A. Mooijaart, W. A. van der Kloot, R. van der Leeden, & E. Van Der Burg. London, England Sage; 1998: 1–44.
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Gustavson K, von Soest T, Karevold E, Røysamb E. Attrition and generalizability in longitudinal studies: findings from a 15-year population-based study and a Monte Carlo simulation study. BMC Public Health. 2012;12(1):1.CrossRef Gustavson K, von Soest T, Karevold E, Røysamb E. Attrition and generalizability in longitudinal studies: findings from a 15-year population-based study and a Monte Carlo simulation study. BMC Public Health. 2012;12(1):1.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Predicting hazardous drinking in late adolescence/young adulthood from early and excessive adolescent drinking - a longitudinal cross-national study of Norwegian and Australian adolescents
verfasst von
Frøydis Enstad
Tracy Evans-Whipp
Anne Kjeldsen
John W. Toumbourou
Tilmann von Soest
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2019
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Public Health / Ausgabe 1/2019
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2458
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-019-7099-0

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2019

BMC Public Health 1/2019 Zur Ausgabe