Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Public Health 1/2019

Open Access 01.12.2019 | Research article

Screen viewing behavior and sleep duration among children aged 2 and below

verfasst von: Bozhi Chen, Rob M. van Dam, Chuen Seng Tan, Hwee Ling Chua, Pey Gein Wong, Jonathan Y. Bernard, Falk Müller-Riemenschneider

Erschienen in: BMC Public Health | Ausgabe 1/2019

Abstract

Background

Few studies have investigated the association between screen viewing (SV) and sleep duration among young children. This study aims to examine the association between total and device-specific SV and sleep duration among children aged 2 and below.

Methods

We conducted a cross-sectional study of 714 Singaporean children aged 2 years and below. Parents were recruited during routine well-child clinic visits from two national polyclinics. In Singapore, all parents visit well-child clinics with their children at regular intervals for routine check-ups and vaccinations. Socio-demographic characteristics, duration of total and device-specific SV, and sleep duration were reported by parents via interviewer-administered questionnaires. Multiple linear regression analysis was used to assess associations between various types of SV and sleep duration, adjusted for socio-demographic variables. Due to significant interaction between SV and age, stratified analyses for children aged less than 6 months and those aged 7–24 months were performed.

Results

The prevalence of daily SV among children was 53.1%; 28.3% in children up to 6 months and 73.8% in children aged 7 to 24 months. TV viewing was reported for 44.3% of all children and mobile device SV for 30.1%. Children’s average sleep duration was 13.9 (SD = 3.5) hours daily and younger children had longer sleep duration than older ones (up to 6 months: mean = 15.6 h, SD = 3.9; 7–24 months: mean = 12.4 h, SD = 2.2; P < 0.01). In the regression analysis among all children, each 1 h per day increment in total SV was significantly associated with 0.26 h shorter sleep duration with similar significant associations for TV (β = − 0.28 h, 95%CI: -0.50, − 0.06) and mobile devices (β = − 0.35 h, 95%CI: -0.61, − 0.09). Stratified analysis revealed significantly greater reductions in sleep with higher SV among children aged 6 months and below (β = − 0.73 h, 95%CI: -1.12, − 0.34), while associations were weaker in older children (β = − 0.13 h, 95% CI: -0.24, − 0.01).

Conclusions

This study provides evidence for a substantial association between longer SV and shorter sleep duration among very young children. These associations appeared stronger among children aged 6 months and below as compared with those aged 7 to 24 months. Further studies are warranted to confirm our findings.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s12889-018-6385-6) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Abkürzungen
AAP
American Academy of Pediatrics
DSRB
Domain Specific Review Board
SV
Screen viewing
TV
Television
US
United States

Background

Screen media usage has become increasingly prevalent in daily life with children beginning to use screen media at early age. In Japan, by 18 months old, 86% of children spend over 1 h watching television (TV) a day [1]. In Australia, children under 4 years of age spend more than 2 h watching TV per day on average [2]. Similarly, in the United States (US), 90% of children at age 2 regularly watch TV or videos, with the average duration exceeding 1.5 h a day [3, 4].
Given the high prevalence of screen viewing (SV) among children worldwide, the impact of SV on children’s health has been of rising concern. However, the current information on associations between SV and health outcomes among children is mixed. Studies have shown that media exposure is more likely to have detrimental impacts on children’s health and development before the age of about 2.5 years than after [5, 6]. Potential adverse effects span several developmental domains including delayed cognitive and language development [7, 8], impaired social interaction [9, 10], attention deficits [8, 11, 12], and behavioral problems such as violent behavior and aggression [1315]. In addition to developmental delay, higher SV exposure has been associated with negative impacts on vision [1618] and a greater likelihood of being overweight or obese in late childhood and adolescence [19, 20]. On the other hand, results from some studies have suggested benefits of screen media usage among children. For example, children may benefit from virtual visits to their relatives through live video chat [21] and watching educational media with parents might contribute to general language development [22]. The American Academy of Paediatrics (AAP) has recently modified its SV guidelines. Instead of recommending no SV for children before age 2, it now emphasizes the importance of high-quality programming and parents’ accompanying [23] their children while SV. AAP recommends no SV of children before 18 months except for video-chatting, for instance to prompt interactions with distant family members and parental supervision and support are necessary for children to understand what they are seeing [23]. AAP has reported no evidence that infants can actually benefit from media the way they do from live social interactions.
Among adults, inadequate sleep has been independently associated with overweight and obesity and the development of type 2 diabetes as it may lead to higher energy intake, less physical activity, increased cortisol levels and reduced insulin sensitivity [2427]. The importance of sleep in memory-related domains (including verbal, emotional and procedural development) has been exhibited in both human and animal studies [28]. Children may be more vulnerable to negative effects of inadequate sleep as they gain complexity in domains as cognition and language skills during their neuro-development. There is increasing evidence that suggests inadequate sleep quantity in early life is associated with negative consequences at a later age including cognitive and behavioral deficits which, in turn, affect daytime functioning [29]. Other consequences relate to detrimental health outcomes such as diabetes [30], cardiovascular disease [31], obesity [32] and depression [33]. The current understanding of mechanisms of sleep in young children is bolstered by recent research suggesting that sleep is crucial for the brain to remember and learn by resetting the steady built-up synaptic connectivity during wake time [34].
Screen media usage may indirectly influence sleep because brightly lit screens may lead to hyperarousal resulting in automatic activation increase [35]. Screen media usage may directly limit sleep duration by delaying or interrupting sleep time [36]. There is evidence supporting the hypotheses that increased screen time is associated with shorter sleep duration [3740], night awakenings and disturbed sleep [4145]. Most studies investigating associations of SV behavior with inadequate sleep have focused on adults, adolescents or school-age children, while research on very young children is limited. A recent longitudinal study from the US reported that greater TV viewing during infancy was associated with shorter sleep duration [46] in mid-childhood. The study provided new evidence for the detrimental effects of TV viewing on sleep but did not explore the contribution of other forms of screen media (e.g. mobile phones) to total SV or their impact on sleep duration among very young children. Considering the potentially harmful effects of SV behavior, the scarcity of evidence related to the changing patterns of SV and how other types of screen media, including computers, video consoles and mobile devices may affect sleep and other health related outcomes in very young children is an important limitation of the existing scientific literature.
With a predominantly Asian population in one of the most wired countries in the world [47], this study in Singapore aims to address these gaps by examining the association of total and device-specific SV and sleep duration in children aged 2 years and below.

Methods

Study population

We conducted a cross-sectional study by recruiting parents of children aged up to 2 years in February 2014. The study has previously been described in detail [48]. Briefly, parents were recruited when they brought their children to government polyclinics for routine developmental assessments and mandatory vaccinations. They were recruited from two National Healthcare Group polyclinics, out of total 18 polyclinics in Singapore over a 7-day period. As Singapore’s National Childhood Immunization Program for children up to 2 years has a take-up rate of more than 97%, children attending this program are good representation of the children population in the region at a certain point of time [49]. Parent-child dyads were invited to participate in this study if they met the following inclusion criteria: 1) children were 2 years and below; 2) children were healthy and brought for well-child health visits to the two National Healthcare Group Polyclinics. Non-Singaporeans were excluded.

Ethics statement

On recruitment, verbal informed consents were obtained from all participating parents and questionnaires were administered using participants’ preferred language (English, Chinese, Malay or Tamil) by trained interviewers during clinic visits. The study was approved by the Singapore National Health Care Group Domain Specific Review Board (DSRB).

Measurements

The questionnaire was developed based on a comprehensive review of the literature on determinants and consequences of SV. Parents were asked to report the amount of time their children spent actively watching TV/DVD, computers, video games consoles and mobile devices (including mobile phones, tablets and handheld video game devices) at home on weekdays and weekends (see Additional file 1 for the items). This question emphasized foreground viewing, in which children are actively watching screen devices, to generate a conservative estimate of children’s screen media exposures. It is also believed that parents will report their children’s foreground exposure more accurately as compared with background viewing (for example, a TV is turned on but the children are not actively viewing it). Children’s foreground screen time recalled by parents has been shown to be accurate and reproducible [50]. Although parent reporting has more errors than objective measurements (i.e. video-recorder), it has been shown to exhibit no systematic bias [51]. The daily device-specific SV time was calculated as follows: ((weekdays × 5) + (weekends × 2))/7. Total SV was then determined by adding up the daily SV time for each type of SV device. We also classified total and device specific SV into 4 groups: 0 h, < 1 h, 1–2 h, ≥2 h.
Daily total sleep duration, including daytime and nighttime sleep, was assessed by one question: “How much time in a day does your child spend on sleeping?”. This question was derived from the Children’s Sleep Habit Questionnaire (CSHQ), a commonly used paediatric sleep screening tool, which has been validated among infants and children and used widely in other studies [5254]. Sleep assessed by CSHQ has shown to have a moderate correlation with that observed by the Infant Sleep Chronogram [54]. Parents’ education, marital status, ethnicity and housing type were recorded together with the child’s age and sex.
Cases beyond the lower inner fence, which equal 25th Percentile - (1.5 * Interquartile Range) were defined as outliers [55]. Thus, the participants who reported a sleep duration of less than 6 h per day were excluded from analysis.

Statistical analyses

Socio-demographic and baseline characteristics were expressed as means and standard deviations (SD) for continuous variables, and frequencies and percentages for categorical variables. Fisher’s exact test (for categorical variables) and Student’s t-test (for continuous variables) were performed to compare differences between two age groups (“up to 6 months” and “7–24 months”). The associations of categorical SV (total and device specific SV) with sleep duration were tested using linear regression models, with children having no corresponding SV as the reference groups. Linear effect of continuous SV (total and device-specific SV) on sleep duration was also examined. In multiple linear regression analysis, we further adjusted for potential confounders including child’s age, sex and race, caregiver, marital status of parents, housing type and paternal and maternal educational levels.
To test for possible interaction between SV and age, sex and ethnicity, an interaction term was added to the multivariable regression model one at a time. Only age was found to have significant interaction with SV. Therefore, stratified analyses by age group were conducted. All tests conducted were two-sided and considered significant if P < 0.05. All statistical analyses were performed using Stata, version 14.0 (StataCorp, 2015).

Results

Among the 1061 parent-child dyads approached during their visit to the polyclinics, 794 met the inclusion criteria and 722 (91.0%) provided verbal consent and completed the survey giving us data for 722 children. Eight parents who reported a sleep duration of less than 6 h per day were excluded from analysis. The current analysis includes 714 children.

Characteristics of the study population

Child and parental characteristics of the study population are presented in Table 1. Among all children, 55.0% were male and 58.7% were of Chinese descent, with the remainder being mostly of Indian or Malay descent. The majority of children (55.0%) were between 7 and 24 months. Nearly all parents were married (98.0%) and 73.4% participants reported that they took care of their children by themselves. 79.4% of fathers and 75.8% of mothers completed post-secondary education and above, and the majority of the study population (95.7%) lived in public government housing, where most Singaporeans reside [56]. In general, socio-demographic characteristics were well balanced between the two age groups.
Table 1
Characteristics of participants included in the overall analysis
 
Total (n = 714)
0–6 month
(n = 321)
7–24 months
(n = 393)
N
%
%
%
Sex
 Male
393
55.0
53.6
56.2
 Female
321
45.0
46.4
43.8
Race
 Chinese
419
58.7
57.3
59.8
 Malay
193
27.0
27.1
27.0
 Indian
80
11.2
12.2
10.4
 Others
22
3.1
3.4
2.8
Parent marital status
 Married
700
98.0
98.1
98.0
 Single
14
2.0
1.9
2.0
Father education
 Primary/secondary
147
20.6
17.8
22.9
 Post-secondary and abovea
567
79.4
82.2
77.1
Mother education
 Primary/secondary
173
24.2
22.7
25.4
 Post-secondary and above
541
75.8
77.3
74.6
Caregiver
 Parent
524
73.4
80.4
67.7
 Relative/othersb
190
26.6
19.6
32.3
Housing
 Government housing (1–3 rooms)
138
19.3
21.2
17.8
 Government housing 4 rooms
329
46.1
42.4
49.1
 Government housing ≥5 rooms
216
30.3
33.3
27.7
 Condominium/landed property
31
4.3
3.1
5.3
Sleep in hour (Mean, SD)
714
13.9(3.5)
15.6(3.9)
12.4(2.2)
aIncluding post-secondary, diploma and professional qualification, university and post-graduate
bIncluding siblings, relatives, home helpers and others
Average sleep duration of children was reported to be 13.9 (SD = 3.5) hours. For children aged 6 months and below, the average sleep duration was 15.6 (SD = 3.9) hours while 12.4 (SD = 2.2) hours for children aged between 7 to 24 months.

Screen viewing

Table 2 presents the prevalence of any corresponding screen viewing among total study population, and duration of daily total SV and device-specific SV among those exposed to screen devices. The prevalence of any daily SV was 53.1%. Prevalence of any daily TV (44.3%) and mobile devices (30.1%) viewing was higher than computers (6.6%) and video consoles (0.4%). Due to the low prevalence of video console use, it was not presented in the table.
Table 2
Prevalence and duration of total and device-specific screen viewing (SV) among children exposed to screen devices by age
 
Count n (%)a
Time (Hours/day)b
Median (IQR)
Total
Up to 6 months
7-24 months
Total
Up to 6 months
7-24 months
SV
379 (53.1)
91 (28.3)
288 (73.3)
1.00 (0.50–2.00)
1.00 (0.44–2.00)
1.04 (0.50–2.31)
TV
316 (44.3)
76 (23.7)
240 (61.1)
0.98 (0.43–2.00)
0.80 (0.36–1.86)
1.00 (0.50–2.00)
Computer
47 (6.6)
7 (2.2)
40 (10.2)
0.50 (0.36–1.00)
0.36 (0.17–0.71)
0.57 (0.36–1.00)
Mobile devices
215 (30.1)
42 (13.1)
173 (44.0)
0.50 (0.25–1.00)
0.39 (0.18–1.00)
0.50 (0.29–1.00)
aPrevalence of any corresponding screen viewing among total study population; b Screen viewing duration among children exposed to screen devices
Among children exposed to any screen devices, a median of 1.00 (IQR: 0.50–2.00) hour SV per day was observed. Across various devices, TV viewing time was the highest with a median of 0.98 (IQR: 0.43–2.00) hour per day among TV viewers followed by mobile devices (median = 0.50, IQR: 0.36–1.00) and computer viewing (median = 0.50, IQR: 0.25–1.00) hour per day among children exposed to these devices. Older children (7–24 months) had a much higher prevalence of SV when compared to younger children (up to 6 months): 73.3% vs 28.3%, and this persisted across device-specific SV. SV duration was similar among screen viewers from both groups.

Association of screen time and sleep duration

Table 3 shows the results of multiple linear regression analyses on the association of total and device- specific SV with sleep duration among the total study population. In univariate analysis (e.g. Model 1), total SV was observed to be negatively associated with sleep duration. The association remained statistically significant, although somewhat attenuated, after controlling for socio-demographic factors in Model 2 (0 h: β = ref.; < 1 h of SV: β = − 1.01 h, 95%CI: -1.63, − 0.40; 1–2 h of SV: β = − 1.42 h, 95%CI: -2.10, − 0.73; > 2 h of SV: β = − 1.73 h, 95%CI: -2.43, − 1.02). This association had a significant dose-response relationship. Similar associations were observed for total SV, TV and mobile devices.
Table 3
Associations of total and device-specific screen viewing (hours per day) with sleep duration (hours per day) among all participants
All participants
(n = 714)
β (95%CI)
P valuec
β (95%CI)
P for trendd
SV
0 h (n = 335)
< 1 h (n = 147)
1–2 h (n = 116)
≥ 2 h (n = 116)
 
Per Hour
 
 Model 1a
Ref
−2.03 (− 2.65,-1.41)
− 2.73 (− 3.41,-2.06)
−3.11 (− 3.78,-2.43)
< 0.01
− 0.54 (− 0.68,-0.39)
< 0.01
 Model 2b
Ref
− 1.01 (− 1.63,-0.40)
− 1.42 (− 2.10,-0.73)
−1.73 (− 2.43,-1.02)
< 0.01
− 0.26 (− 0.40,-0.12)
< 0.01
TV
0 h (n = 398)
< 1 h (n = 158)
1–2 h (n = 77)
≥ 2 h (n = 81)
 
Per Hour
 
 Model 1a
Ref
−1.84 (− 2.44,-1.23)
−2.41 (− 3.21,-1.61)
−2.58 (− 3.37,-1.80)
< 0.01
− 0.69 (− 0.92,-0.46)
< 0.01
 Model 2b
Ref
−0.78 (− 1.37,-0.20)
−1.27 (− 2.05,-0.48)
−1.32 (− 2.08,-0.56)
< 0.01
− 0.28 (− 0.50,-0.06)
0.01
Mobile devices
0 h (n = 499)
< 1 h (n = 141)
1–2 h (n = 47)
≥ 2 h (n = 27)
 
Per Hour
 
 Model 1a
Ref
−1.94 (− 2.57,-1.32)
−2.43 (− 3.43,-1.44)
−2.19 (− 3.48,-0.90)
< 0.01
−0.69 (− 0.97,-0.41)
< 0.01
 Model 2b
Ref
−0.85(− 1.45,-0.25)
−0.95 (− 1.90,-0.01)
−1.10 (− 2.32,0.11)
0.01
−0.35 (− 0.61,-0.09)
0.01
aUnadjusted model
bMultivariable model adjusted for children’s age race, sex, mother’s education, father’s education, marital status, caregiver, housing type
cOverall effect of categorical screen viewing variables; d Trend of association using continuous screen viewing variables
We observed significant interactions for age group and total SV, TV and SV on mobile devices as continuous variables in relation to sleep duration (P for interaction < 0.01, < 0.01, 0.03, respectively). Table 4 provides the results of the analysis stratified by age. Stronger associations between SV and sleep duration were reported among the younger children as compared to older children across different device types, after taking socio-demographic factors into account. Among the younger children, compared with no SV, a total SV time of up to 1 h (β = − 1.59 h, 95%CI: -2.84, − 0.34), 1 to 2 h (β = − 2.07 h, 95%CI: -3.74, − 0.40) and 2 or more hours (β = − 2.88 h, 95%CI: -4.50, − 1.26) per day were significantly associated with shorter sleep duration. Among the older children, a total SV of 1 to 2 h (β = − 0.84 h, 95% CI: -1.45, − 0.23) and 2 or more hours (β = − 0.91 h, 95% CI: -1.54, − 0.28) per day were associated with shorter sleep duration when compared with no SV. A significant dose-response relationship was detected for both age groups (up to 6 months: P for trend < 0.01, 7–24 months: P for trend = 0.03). Similarly, TV viewing of up to 1 h (β = − 1.76 h, 95% CI: -3.05, − 0.48), 1 to 2 h (β = − 2.02 h, 95% CI: -4.01, − 0.04) and 2 or more hours (β = − 2.47 h, 95% CI: -4.29, − 0.65) per day were significantly associated with shorter sleep duration when compared with no SV (P for trend < 0.01) among the younger children. However, among the older children, only TV viewing of 1 to 2 h (β = − 0.68 h, 95% CI: -1.33, − 0.02) was significantly associated with a shorter sleep duration when compared with no SV. With regard to SV on mobile devices among younger children, 1 to 2 h (β = − 3.21 h, 95%CI: -6.15, − 0.27) per day was significantly associated with shorter sleep duration (P for trend = 0.02) when compared with no SV. Among older children, a marginally significant association between SV on mobile devices of up to 1 h (β = − 0.62 h, 95%CI: -1.11, − 0.13, overall P = 0.07) per day and sleep duration was observed when compared with no SV.
Table 4
Associations of total and device-specific screen viewing (hours per day) with sleep duration (hours per day) stratified by age group
 
β (95%CI)
P value c
β (95% CI)
P for trend d
Up to 6 months
 SV
0 h (n = 230)
< 1 h (n = 43)
1–2 h (n = 23)
≥ 2 h (n = 25)
 
Per Hour
 
  Model 1a
Ref
−1.75 (− 2.99,-0.51)
−2.26 (− 3.89,-0.63)
−3.26 (− 4.83,-1.69)
< 0.01
− 0.82 (− 1.19,-0.44)
< 0.01
  Model 2b
Ref
− 1.59 (− 2.84,-0.34)
−2.07 (− 3.74,-0.40)
−2.88 (− 4.50,-1.26)
< 0.01
− 0.73 (− 1.12,-0.34)
< 0.01
 TV
0 h (n = 245)
< 1 h (n = 41)
1–2 h (n = 16)
≥ 2 h (n = 19)
 
Per Hour
 
  Model 1
Ref
−2.01 (−3.28,-0.74)
− 2.05 (− 3.99,-0.11)
−2.74 (− 4.54,-0.95)
< 0.01
− 1.03 (− 1.59,-0.46)
< 0.01
  Model 2
Ref
−1.76 (− 3.05,-0.48)
−2.02 (− 4.01,-0.04)
−2.47 (− 4.29,-0.65)
< 0.01
−0.85 (− 1.43,-0.27)
< 0.01
 Mobile devices
0 h (n = 279)
< 1 h (n = 29)
1–2 h (n = 7)
≥ 2 h (n = 6)
 
Per Hour
 
  Model 1
Ref
−1.63 (−3.11,-0.15)
−3.80 (− 6.71,-0.89)
− 2.79 (− 5.93,0.35)
< 0.01
−0.81 (− 1.39,-0.23)
0.01
  Model 2
Ref
−1.37 (−2.88,0.14)
−3.21 (− 6.15,-0.27)
− 2.62 (− 5.85,0.61)
0.03
− 0.73 (− 1.33,-0.14)
0.02
7–24 months
 SV
0 h (n = 105)
< 1 h (n = 104)
1–2 h (n = 93)
≥ 2 h (n = 91)
 
Per Hour
 
  Model 1
Ref
−0.35 (− 0.93,0.23)
−0.88 (− 1.48,-0.28)
− 1.12 (− 1.72,-0.52)
< 0.01
−0.17 (− 0.28,-0.06)
< 0.01
  Model 2
Ref
−0.29 (− 0.87,0.29)
−0.84 (− 1.45,-0.23)
−0.91 (− 1.54,-0.28)
0.01
− 0.13 (− 0.24,-0.01)
0.03
 TV
0 h (n = 153)
< 1 h (n = 117)
1–2 h (n = 61)
≥ 2 h (n = 62)
 
Per Hour
 
  Model 1
Ref
−0.16 (− 0.68,0.36)
−0.77 (− 1.41,-0.13)
−0.86 (− 1.50,-0.23)
0.01
− 0.17 (− 0.34,0.01)
0.06
  Model 2
Ref
− 0.08 (− 0.60,0.43)
−0.68 (− 1.33,-0.02)
−0.61 (− 1.26,0.05)
0.1
−0.09 (− 0.27,-0.08)
0.29
 Mobile devices
0 h (n = 220)
< 1 h (n = 112)
1–2 h (n = 40)
≥ 2 h (n = 21)
 
Per Hour
 
  Model 1
Ref
−0.62 (− 1.12,-0.13)
− 0.68 (− 1.41,-0.05)
−0.66 (− 1.62,0.31)
0.04
−0.23 (− 0.45,-0.02)
0.04
  Model 2
Ref
−0.62 (− 1.11,-0.13)
−0.54 (− 1.28,0.19)
−0.45 (− 1.43,0.53)
0.07
− 0.17 (− 0.40,-0.05)
0.12
aUnadjusted model
bMultivariable model adjusted for children’s age race, sex, maternal education, father’s education, mother’s education, caregiver, marital status, housing type
cOverall effect of categorical screen viewing variables
dTrend of association using screen viewing variables

Discussion

To our knowledge, our study is the first to investigate the association between device-specific SV and sleep in very young children from a multiethnic Asian population. We found that longer total and device-specific SV was associated with shorter sleep duration among children aged 2 years and below. Associations were stronger for children aged 6 months and below when compared to children aged 7 to 24 months.
In our study, TV was still the most commonly used screen device contributing the highest viewing time among these very young children in Singapore. However, more than half of children also used mobile devices actively. On average, this study reported an adequate daily sleep duration among the very young children. Compared with other high-income countries in Asia, sleep duration reported in this study is similar to that among Taiwanese young children (13.7 h/day) [57], and more than that among infants and toddlers in Japan (11.6 h/day) [58] and South Korea (11.9 h/day) [59]. However, unlike the high compliance reported among Australian (88.7%) [60] and Canadian (83.9%) toddlers [61], almost half of our study sample failed to meet the sleep recommendations [62, 63].
There was a strong inverse association between SV and sleep in children aged up to 2 years, to the extent that children who had more than 1 h of total SV per day slept 1.5 h less than those without any SV. Our findings suggested that associations were even stronger in younger children as compared to older ones. We hypothesize that screen devices may affect sleep through several pathways. Firstly, the use of screen devices may directly lead to a reduction of sleep duration by delaying in naptime and bedtime [64]. Secondly, viewed content may be psychologically stimulating thereby leading to delayed sleep or interruptions of nighttime sleep [65]. Thirdly, exposure to bright light of screen devices before sleep may interrupt the sleep-wake cycle by suppressing the release of the hormone melatonin [66]. Consequently, the onset of nighttime sleepiness may be postponed due to the increasing alertness caused by bright light [67]. These findings were consistent with existing evidence from previous studies where the exposure to and use of screen media was negatively associated with nighttime sleep duration among school-aged children [37, 42, 68] and adolescents [69, 70]. Our study found that SV from two common types of screen device use (TV and mobile devices) had negative associations with sleep duration, expanding on previous studies investigating the relationship of single SV device and napping and nighttime sleep among young children [64, 71]. In the present study, we observed 0.28 h’ shorter sleep duration per 1-h increase in TV viewing among all children, with a considerably stronger association among younger children. The magnitude of association in our study is similar to that reported in the recent longitudinal study of TV viewing and sleep duration among 1864 American children, each 1 h longer TV viewing per day from infancy to mid-childhood was associated with fewer hours of total sleep duration (including napping and nighttime sleep) during the same period (0.12 h per day, 95%CI: 0.07, 0.17) [46]. Consistent with our finding that SV on mobile devices was significantly associated with shorter sleep duration in young children, a previous systematic review reported a strong and consistent positive relationship between bedtime mobile device use and inadequate nighttime sleep (OR: 2.17; 95% CI: 1.42, 3.32) among school-age children [72]. In our study, increasing SV on mobile devices was associated with similar reductions in sleep duration as compared to TV viewing. Traditional SV devices such as TVs have been used by parents as a way for their children to rest, as a babysitter [73] and a behavior management tool [74]. In addition to these functions, mobile screen devices, including phones and tablets are portable with broader capabilities resulting in a different level and type of exposure than TVs, as these mobile screen devices enable continuous stimulation for children by allowing real-time interaction [72].
Our study makes a unique contribution to the existing literature. To our knowledge, existing studies on SV and sleep have not assessed multiple screen devices among very young children. However, the variety of SV exposures reflects modern SV patterns. Our study also has a very high participation rate and the characteristic of the children in our study was broadly comparable to the children population in Singapore [49]. However, there are also some methodological limitations to be acknowledged. Firstly, for logistical and administrative reasons, participants were recruited from 2 out of 18 polyclinics in Singapore. Although there is no information suggesting that there may be a difference between polyclinics, such differences may still exist which would decrease generalizability of our findings. Secondly, our study adopted a subjective measure, a proxy questionnaire, to determine SV and sleep duration. All data was collected by interviewers rather than self-administered and respondents did not have difficulty understanding the questions. Although our questionnaire was developed based on existing literature and piloted extensively, it was not validated in the target population. This may introduce measurement error which could attenuate the associations towards the null. Interviewer-administered questionnaires can be affected by various response biases, such as social desirability and recall bias. Although children’s demographics (i.e. age, sex, race) and family characteristics (i.e. caregivers, marital status of parents, housing type and paternal and maternal educational levels) were adjusted in the analysis, residual confounding might still exist due to unmeasured factors such as children’s anthropometrics and parenting style. Thirdly, since this was a cross-sectional study, it was unable to fully conclude the direction of association between SV time and sleep duration. Fourthly, a one-item question was used to determine sleep duration, which might be suboptimal for very young children who tend to take several naps throughout the day. Younger children have been shown to have more wakeups and shorter sleeping cycle when compared with older ones [75], however, the one-item question was not able to differentiate the different sleeping cycles. While this item was derived from a questionnaire that has previously been validated among an infant population and widely used in other studies among infants, toddlers and children [5254], we acknowledge that objective means of measuring sleep duration, for instance via actigraphy, may be advisable in future studies. In addition, the one-item question did not capture any information of sleep quality, which is another valuable measure of sleep [76]. Finally, young children may benefit from quality programmes or interactive media [77] while some content, such as media violence, can elevate psychological and physiological arousal, making it more difficult for children to fall asleep and resulting in impaired sleep quality of sleep [78]. We did not include more information of the content of media use in this study, which may be important to better explain the mechanisms by which SV impacts sleep patterns.
It is important that caregivers, teachers and healthcare professionals are educated about the potential detrimental influence of SV on sleep which may in turn affect children’s daytime function, heath behavior and lead to health risks in the long run. Based on literature and our findings, we encourage policy-led guidance to communities on the promotion of sleep hygiene. In addition, a checklist for monitoring sleeping behavior of children during routine clinic visits is important in order to explore screen device use as a possible risk factor. Given the evolving technology landscape, parents may believe in the educational and entertainment value of media devices and replace traditional paper books with mobile devices [73]. It is likely that the access and use of media screen devices will increase further among very young children. Our findings support the need for interventions to reduce the access and usage of mobile screen devices, especially among very young children.
Our study provides relevant new evidence for an increasingly important public health issue. The phenomenon of very young children using multiple screen devices is nowadays almost common practice, which could be motivated by parents’ beliefs in the educational values of using screen devices. However, more research is needed to address those beliefs. TVs are not recommended to be placed in a child’s bedroom [79]. Given that mobile devices may exert similar influences on sleep, further research should assess the presence of other screen devices in a child’s bedroom and its association with sleep and other health related outcomes among very young children.

Conclusions

Our study suggests strong adverse associations between SV time across different screen devices with sleep duration among Singaporean children aged 2 years and below. These associations were particularly strong among children below 6 months of age. Collaborative approaches from caregivers, teachers, and healthcare professionals are needed to reduce screen device access and use among very young children in order to minimize the negative impact on child sleep and potentially child health. Nevertheless, further research is warranted to corroborate our observations and to develop effective intervention strategies to reduce SV among very young children.

Acknowledgements

The authors acknowledge the contribution of all members of the iBaby Community Health Project group (Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore) who supported the data collection and the conducted of this study. The authors also want to acknowledge the National Healthcare Group for the support in data colletion at the polyclinics. The authors thank Imara Roychowdhury for copyeditting the manuscript.

Funding

The research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
The study was approved by the Singapore National Health Care Group, Domain Specific Review Board (DSRB). Verbal consent was approved by the ethics board and obtained from all participants.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

FM-R is a member of the editorial board (Associated Editor) of BMC public health. All other authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Cheng S, Maeda T, Yoichi S, Yamagata Z, Tomiwa K, Japan Children’s Study G. Early television exposure and children’s behavioral and social outcomes at age 30 months. J Epidemiol. 2010;20(Suppl 2):S482–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Cheng S, Maeda T, Yoichi S, Yamagata Z, Tomiwa K, Japan Children’s Study G. Early television exposure and children’s behavioral and social outcomes at age 30 months. J Epidemiol. 2010;20(Suppl 2):S482–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Australian Communications and Media Authority (ACMA). Children’s viewing patterns on commercial, free-to-air and subscription television: report analysing audience and ratings data for 2001, 2005 and 2006. Belconnen: ACT; 2007. Australian Communications and Media Authority (ACMA). Children’s viewing patterns on commercial, free-to-air and subscription television: report analysing audience and ratings data for 2001, 2005 and 2006. Belconnen: ACT; 2007.
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Common Sense Media. Zero to eight children’s media use in America. San Francisco, CA; 2013. Common Sense Media. Zero to eight children’s media use in America. San Francisco, CA; 2013.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA, Meltzoff AN. Television and DVD/video viewing in children younger than 2 years. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161(5):473–9.PubMedCrossRef Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA, Meltzoff AN. Television and DVD/video viewing in children younger than 2 years. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161(5):473–9.PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson DRPT. Television and very young children. Am Behav Sci. 2005;48:505–22.CrossRef Anderson DRPT. Television and very young children. Am Behav Sci. 2005;48:505–22.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Christakis DA, Zimmerman FJ. Viewing television before age 3 is not the same as viewing television at age 5. Pediatrics. 2006;118(1):435 author reply 435-436.PubMedCrossRef Christakis DA, Zimmerman FJ. Viewing television before age 3 is not the same as viewing television at age 5. Pediatrics. 2006;118(1):435 author reply 435-436.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Chonchaiya W, Pruksananonda C. Television viewing associates with delayed language development. Acta Paediatr. 2008;97(7):977–82.PubMedCrossRef Chonchaiya W, Pruksananonda C. Television viewing associates with delayed language development. Acta Paediatr. 2008;97(7):977–82.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA, Meltzoff AN. Associations between media viewing and language development in children under age 2 years. J Pediatr. 2007;151(4):364–8.PubMedCrossRef Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA, Meltzoff AN. Associations between media viewing and language development in children under age 2 years. J Pediatr. 2007;151(4):364–8.PubMedCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Kirkorian HL, Pempek TA, Murphy LA, Schmidt ME, Anderson DR. The impact of background television on parent-child interaction. Child Dev. 2009;80(5):1350–9.PubMedCrossRef Kirkorian HL, Pempek TA, Murphy LA, Schmidt ME, Anderson DR. The impact of background television on parent-child interaction. Child Dev. 2009;80(5):1350–9.PubMedCrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Mendelsohn AL, Brockmeyer CA, Dreyer BP, Fierman AH, Berkule-Silberman SB, Tomopoulos S. Do verbal interactions with infants during electronic media exposure mitigate adverse impacts on their language development as toddlers? Infant Child Dev. 2010;19(6):577–93.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mendelsohn AL, Brockmeyer CA, Dreyer BP, Fierman AH, Berkule-Silberman SB, Tomopoulos S. Do verbal interactions with infants during electronic media exposure mitigate adverse impacts on their language development as toddlers? Infant Child Dev. 2010;19(6):577–93.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Christakis DA, Zimmerman FJ, DiGiuseppe DL, McCarty CA. Early television exposure and subsequent attentional problems in children. Pediatrics. 2004;113(4):708–13.PubMedCrossRef Christakis DA, Zimmerman FJ, DiGiuseppe DL, McCarty CA. Early television exposure and subsequent attentional problems in children. Pediatrics. 2004;113(4):708–13.PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Verlinden M, Tiemeier H, Hudziak JJ, Jaddoe VW, Raat H, Guxens M, Hofman A, Verhulst FC, Jansen PW. Television viewing and externalizing problems in preschool children: the generation R study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166(10):919–25.PubMedCrossRef Verlinden M, Tiemeier H, Hudziak JJ, Jaddoe VW, Raat H, Guxens M, Hofman A, Verhulst FC, Jansen PW. Television viewing and externalizing problems in preschool children: the generation R study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166(10):919–25.PubMedCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Comstock G, Strasburger VC. Deceptive appearances: television violence and aggressive behavior. J Adolesc Health Care. 1990;11(1):31–44.PubMedCrossRef Comstock G, Strasburger VC. Deceptive appearances: television violence and aggressive behavior. J Adolesc Health Care. 1990;11(1):31–44.PubMedCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Singer MI, Slovak K, Frierson T, York P. Viewing preferences, symptoms of psychological trauma, and violent behaviors among children who watch television. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;37(10):1041–8.PubMedCrossRef Singer MI, Slovak K, Frierson T, York P. Viewing preferences, symptoms of psychological trauma, and violent behaviors among children who watch television. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;37(10):1041–8.PubMedCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnson JG, Cohen P, Smailes EM, Kasen S, Brook JS. Television viewing and aggressive behavior during adolescence and adulthood. Science (New York, NY). 2002;295(5564):2468–71.CrossRef Johnson JG, Cohen P, Smailes EM, Kasen S, Brook JS. Television viewing and aggressive behavior during adolescence and adulthood. Science (New York, NY). 2002;295(5564):2468–71.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat French AN, Morgan IG, Mitchell P, Rose KA. Risk factors for incident myopia in Australian schoolchildren: the Sydney adolescent vascular and eye study. Ophthalmology. 2013;120(10):2100–8.PubMedCrossRef French AN, Morgan IG, Mitchell P, Rose KA. Risk factors for incident myopia in Australian schoolchildren: the Sydney adolescent vascular and eye study. Ophthalmology. 2013;120(10):2100–8.PubMedCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Yingyong P. Risk factors for refractive errors in primary school children (6-12 years old) in Nakhon Pathom Province. J Med Assoc Thail. 2010;93(11):1288–93. Yingyong P. Risk factors for refractive errors in primary school children (6-12 years old) in Nakhon Pathom Province. J Med Assoc Thail. 2010;93(11):1288–93.
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Czepita D, Mojsa A, Ustianowska M, Czepita M, Lachowicz E. Reading, writing, working on a computer or watching television, and myopia. Klin Oczna. 2010;112(10–12):293–5.PubMed Czepita D, Mojsa A, Ustianowska M, Czepita M, Lachowicz E. Reading, writing, working on a computer or watching television, and myopia. Klin Oczna. 2010;112(10–12):293–5.PubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Mendoza JA, Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA. Television viewing, computer use, obesity, and adiposity in US preschool children. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2007;4:44.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mendoza JA, Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA. Television viewing, computer use, obesity, and adiposity in US preschool children. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2007;4:44.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Saldanha-Gomes C, Heude B, Charles MA, de Lauzon-Guillain B, Botton J, Carles S, Forhan A, Dargent-Molina P, Lioret S. Prospective associations between energy balance-related behaviors at 2 years of age and subsequent adiposity: the EDEN mother-child cohort. Int J Obes. 2017;41(1):38–45.CrossRef Saldanha-Gomes C, Heude B, Charles MA, de Lauzon-Guillain B, Botton J, Carles S, Forhan A, Dargent-Molina P, Lioret S. Prospective associations between energy balance-related behaviors at 2 years of age and subsequent adiposity: the EDEN mother-child cohort. Int J Obes. 2017;41(1):38–45.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat McClure EC-DY, Barr R, Holochwost S, Parrott W. “Facetime doesn’t count”: video chat as an exception to media restrictions for infants and toddlers. Int J Child-Comput Interact. 2015;6:1–6.CrossRef McClure EC-DY, Barr R, Holochwost S, Parrott W. “Facetime doesn’t count”: video chat as an exception to media restrictions for infants and toddlers. Int J Child-Comput Interact. 2015;6:1–6.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Richert RA, Robb MB, Fender JG, Wartella E. Word learning from baby videos. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164(5):432–7.PubMedCrossRef Richert RA, Robb MB, Fender JG, Wartella E. Word learning from baby videos. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164(5):432–7.PubMedCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Banks S, Dinges DF. Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. J Clin Sleep Med. 2007;3(5):519–28.PubMedPubMedCentral Banks S, Dinges DF. Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. J Clin Sleep Med. 2007;3(5):519–28.PubMedPubMedCentral
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Jean-Louis G, Williams NJ, Sarpong D, Pandey A, Youngstedt S, Zizi F, Ogedegbe G. Associations between inadequate sleep and obesity in the US adult population: analysis of the national health interview survey (1977-2009). BMC Public Health. 2014;14:290.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Jean-Louis G, Williams NJ, Sarpong D, Pandey A, Youngstedt S, Zizi F, Ogedegbe G. Associations between inadequate sleep and obesity in the US adult population: analysis of the national health interview survey (1977-2009). BMC Public Health. 2014;14:290.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Shlisky JD, Hartman TJ, Kris-Etherton PM, Rogers CJ, Sharkey NA, Nickols-Richardson SM. Partial sleep deprivation and energy balance in adults: an emerging issue for consideration by dietetics practitioners. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2012;112(11):1785–97.PubMedCrossRef Shlisky JD, Hartman TJ, Kris-Etherton PM, Rogers CJ, Sharkey NA, Nickols-Richardson SM. Partial sleep deprivation and energy balance in adults: an emerging issue for consideration by dietetics practitioners. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2012;112(11):1785–97.PubMedCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003;26(2):117–26.PubMedCrossRef Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003;26(2):117–26.PubMedCrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Payne JD. Learning, memory, and sleep in humans. Sleep Med Clin. 2011;6(1):15–30.CrossRef Payne JD. Learning, memory, and sleep in humans. Sleep Med Clin. 2011;6(1):15–30.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Beebe DW. Cognitive, behavioral, and functional consequences of inadequate sleep in children and adolescents. Pediatr Clin N Am. 2011;58(3):649–65.CrossRef Beebe DW. Cognitive, behavioral, and functional consequences of inadequate sleep in children and adolescents. Pediatr Clin N Am. 2011;58(3):649–65.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Knutson KL, Ryden AM, Mander BA, Van Cauter E. Role of sleep duration and quality in the risk and severity of type 2 diabetes mellitus. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166(16):1768–74.PubMedCrossRef Knutson KL, Ryden AM, Mander BA, Van Cauter E. Role of sleep duration and quality in the risk and severity of type 2 diabetes mellitus. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166(16):1768–74.PubMedCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Kasasbeh E, Chi DS, Krishnaswamy G. Inflammatory aspects of sleep apnea and their cardiovascular consequences. South Med J. 2006;99(1):58–67 quiz 68–59, 81.PubMedCrossRef Kasasbeh E, Chi DS, Krishnaswamy G. Inflammatory aspects of sleep apnea and their cardiovascular consequences. South Med J. 2006;99(1):58–67 quiz 68–59, 81.PubMedCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Bell JF, Zimmerman FJ. Shortened nighttime sleep duration in early life and subsequent childhood obesity. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164(9):840–5.PubMedCrossRef Bell JF, Zimmerman FJ. Shortened nighttime sleep duration in early life and subsequent childhood obesity. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164(9):840–5.PubMedCrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Zimmerman M, McGlinchey JB, Young D, Chelminski I. Diagnosing major depressive disorder I: a psychometric evaluation of the DSM-IV symptom criteria. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2006;194(3):158–63.PubMedCrossRef Zimmerman M, McGlinchey JB, Young D, Chelminski I. Diagnosing major depressive disorder I: a psychometric evaluation of the DSM-IV symptom criteria. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2006;194(3):158–63.PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Huber R, Born J. Sleep, synaptic connectivity, and hippocampal memory during early development. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18(3):141–52.PubMedCrossRef Huber R, Born J. Sleep, synaptic connectivity, and hippocampal memory during early development. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18(3):141–52.PubMedCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Garrison MM, Liekweg K, Christakis DA. Media use and child sleep: the impact of content, timing, and environment. Pediatrics. 2011;128(1):29–35.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Garrison MM, Liekweg K, Christakis DA. Media use and child sleep: the impact of content, timing, and environment. Pediatrics. 2011;128(1):29–35.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Chang AM, Aeschbach D, Duffy JF, Czeisler CA. Evening use of light-emitting eReaders negatively affects sleep, circadian timing, and next-morning alertness. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015;112(4):1232–7.PubMedCrossRef Chang AM, Aeschbach D, Duffy JF, Czeisler CA. Evening use of light-emitting eReaders negatively affects sleep, circadian timing, and next-morning alertness. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015;112(4):1232–7.PubMedCrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Calamaro CJ, Yang K, Ratcliffe S, Chasens ER. Wired at a young age: the effect of caffeine and technology on sleep duration and body mass index in school-aged children. J Pediatr Health Care. 2012;26(4):276–82.PubMedCrossRef Calamaro CJ, Yang K, Ratcliffe S, Chasens ER. Wired at a young age: the effect of caffeine and technology on sleep duration and body mass index in school-aged children. J Pediatr Health Care. 2012;26(4):276–82.PubMedCrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Hitze B, Bosy-Westphal A, Bielfeldt F, Settler U, Plachta-Danielzik S, Pfeuffer M, Schrezenmeir J, Monig H, Muller MJ. Determinants and impact of sleep duration in children and adolescents: data of the Kiel obesity prevention study. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2009;63(6):739–46.PubMedCrossRef Hitze B, Bosy-Westphal A, Bielfeldt F, Settler U, Plachta-Danielzik S, Pfeuffer M, Schrezenmeir J, Monig H, Muller MJ. Determinants and impact of sleep duration in children and adolescents: data of the Kiel obesity prevention study. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2009;63(6):739–46.PubMedCrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat King DL, Delfabbro PH, Zwaans T, et al. Sleep interference effects of pathological electronic media use during adolescence. Int J Ment Health Addiction. 2014;12(1):21–35.CrossRef King DL, Delfabbro PH, Zwaans T, et al. Sleep interference effects of pathological electronic media use during adolescence. Int J Ment Health Addiction. 2014;12(1):21–35.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Bartlett NDGD, Bartlett CP, Eisenmann JC, Walsh DA. Sleep as a mediator of screen time effects on US children’s health outcomes: a prospective study. J Child Media. 2012;6(1):37–50.CrossRef Bartlett NDGD, Bartlett CP, Eisenmann JC, Walsh DA. Sleep as a mediator of screen time effects on US children’s health outcomes: a prospective study. J Child Media. 2012;6(1):37–50.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: a review. Sleep Med. 2010;11(8):735–42.PubMedCrossRef Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: a review. Sleep Med. 2010;11(8):735–42.PubMedCrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Li S, Jin X, Wu S, Jiang F, Yan C, Shen X. The impact of media use on sleep patterns and sleep disorders among school-aged children in China. Sleep. 2007;30(3):361–7.PubMedCrossRef Li S, Jin X, Wu S, Jiang F, Yan C, Shen X. The impact of media use on sleep patterns and sleep disorders among school-aged children in China. Sleep. 2007;30(3):361–7.PubMedCrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Oka Y, Suzuki S, Inoue Y. Bedtime activities, sleep environment, and sleep/wake patterns of Japanese elementary school children. Behav Sleep Med. 2008;6(4):220–33.PubMedCrossRef Oka Y, Suzuki S, Inoue Y. Bedtime activities, sleep environment, and sleep/wake patterns of Japanese elementary school children. Behav Sleep Med. 2008;6(4):220–33.PubMedCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Calamaro CJ, Mason TB, Ratcliffe SJ. Adolescents living the 24/7 lifestyle: effects of caffeine and technology on sleep duration and daytime functioning. Pediatrics. 2009;123(6):e1005–10.PubMedCrossRef Calamaro CJ, Mason TB, Ratcliffe SJ. Adolescents living the 24/7 lifestyle: effects of caffeine and technology on sleep duration and daytime functioning. Pediatrics. 2009;123(6):e1005–10.PubMedCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Chahal H, Fung C, Kuhle S, Veugelers PJ. Availability and night-time use of electronic entertainment and communication devices are associated with short sleep duration and obesity among Canadian children. Pediatr Obes. 2013;8(1):42–51.PubMedCrossRef Chahal H, Fung C, Kuhle S, Veugelers PJ. Availability and night-time use of electronic entertainment and communication devices are associated with short sleep duration and obesity among Canadian children. Pediatr Obes. 2013;8(1):42–51.PubMedCrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Cespedes EM, Gillman MW, Kleinman K, Rifas-Shiman SL, Redline S, Taveras EM. Television viewing, bedroom television, and sleep duration from infancy to mid-childhood. Pediatrics. 2014;133(5):e1163–71.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Cespedes EM, Gillman MW, Kleinman K, Rifas-Shiman SL, Redline S, Taveras EM. Television viewing, bedroom television, and sleep duration from infancy to mid-childhood. Pediatrics. 2014;133(5):e1163–71.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Chandler JL, Brazendale K, Beets MW, Mealing BA. Classification of physical activity intensities using a wrist-worn accelerometer in 8-12-year-old children. Pediatr Obes. 2016;11(2):120–7.PubMedCrossRef Chandler JL, Brazendale K, Beets MW, Mealing BA. Classification of physical activity intensities using a wrist-worn accelerometer in 8-12-year-old children. Pediatr Obes. 2016;11(2):120–7.PubMedCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Goh SN, Teh LH, Tay WR, Anantharaman S, van Dam RM, Tan CS, Chua HL, Wong PG, Muller-Riemenschneider F. Sociodemographic, home environment and parental influences on total and device-specific screen viewing in children aged 2 years and below: an observational study. BMJ Open. 2016;6(1):e009113.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Goh SN, Teh LH, Tay WR, Anantharaman S, van Dam RM, Tan CS, Chua HL, Wong PG, Muller-Riemenschneider F. Sociodemographic, home environment and parental influences on total and device-specific screen viewing in children aged 2 years and below: an observational study. BMJ Open. 2016;6(1):e009113.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Jayawardena AYC, Martin R, et al. Immunisation uptake in Singapore. Singapore Fam Physician. 2011;37:31–5. Jayawardena AYC, Martin R, et al. Immunisation uptake in Singapore. Singapore Fam Physician. 2011;37:31–5.
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson DR, Field DE, Collins PA, Lorch EP, Nathan JG. Estimates of young children's time with television: a methodological comparison of parent reports with time-lapse video home observation. Child Dev. 1985;56(5):1345–57.PubMedCrossRef Anderson DR, Field DE, Collins PA, Lorch EP, Nathan JG. Estimates of young children's time with television: a methodological comparison of parent reports with time-lapse video home observation. Child Dev. 1985;56(5):1345–57.PubMedCrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Borzekowski DLG, Robinson TN. Viewing the viewers: ten video cases of children's television viewing behaviors. J Broadcast Electron Media. 1999;43(4):506–28.CrossRef Borzekowski DLG, Robinson TN. Viewing the viewers: ten video cases of children's television viewing behaviors. J Broadcast Electron Media. 1999;43(4):506–28.CrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Markovich AN, Gendron MA, Corkum PV. Validating the children’s sleep habits questionnaire against polysomnography and actigraphy in school-aged children. Front Psych. 2014;5:188. Markovich AN, Gendron MA, Corkum PV. Validating the children’s sleep habits questionnaire against polysomnography and actigraphy in school-aged children. Front Psych. 2014;5:188.
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Zozula R, Bodow M, Yatcilla D, Cody R, Rosen RC. Development of a brief, self-administered instrument for assessing sleep knowledge in medical education: “the ASKME survey”. Sleep. 2001;24(2):227–33.PubMed Zozula R, Bodow M, Yatcilla D, Cody R, Rosen RC. Development of a brief, self-administered instrument for assessing sleep knowledge in medical education: “the ASKME survey”. Sleep. 2001;24(2):227–33.PubMed
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Dias CC, Figueiredo B, Pinto TM. Children's Sleep Habits Questionnaire – Infant Version. J Pediatr. 2018;94(2):146–54. Dias CC, Figueiredo B, Pinto TM. Children's Sleep Habits Questionnaire – Infant Version. J Pediatr. 2018;94(2):146–54. 
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen BH, Lea RB. Essentials of statistics for the social and behavioral sciences. Hoboken: Wiley; 2004. Cohen BH, Lea RB. Essentials of statistics for the social and behavioral sciences. Hoboken: Wiley; 2004.
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Board HD: Public housing – a Singapore icon. 2015. Board HD: Public housing – a Singapore icon. 2015.
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Chou Y-H. Survey of sleep in infants and young children in northern Taiwan*. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2007;5(1):40–9.CrossRef Chou Y-H. Survey of sleep in infants and young children in northern Taiwan*. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2007;5(1):40–9.CrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Kohyama J, Mindell JA, Sadeh A. Sleep characteristics of young children in Japan: internet study and comparison with other Asian countries. Pediatr Int. 2011;53(5):649–55.PubMedCrossRef Kohyama J, Mindell JA, Sadeh A. Sleep characteristics of young children in Japan: internet study and comparison with other Asian countries. Pediatr Int. 2011;53(5):649–55.PubMedCrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Ahn Y, Williamson AA, Seo HJ, Sadeh A, Mindell JA. Sleep patterns among south Korean infants and toddlers: global comparison. J Korean Med Sci. 2016;31(2):261–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ahn Y, Williamson AA, Seo HJ, Sadeh A, Mindell JA. Sleep patterns among south Korean infants and toddlers: global comparison. J Korean Med Sci. 2016;31(2):261–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Cliff DP, McNeill J, Vella SA, Howard SJ, Santos R, Batterham M, Melhuish E, Okely AD, de Rosnay M. Adherence to 24-hour movement guidelines for the early years and associations with social-cognitive development among Australian preschool children. BMC Public Health. 2017;17(Suppl 5):857.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Cliff DP, McNeill J, Vella SA, Howard SJ, Santos R, Batterham M, Melhuish E, Okely AD, de Rosnay M. Adherence to 24-hour movement guidelines for the early years and associations with social-cognitive development among Australian preschool children. BMC Public Health. 2017;17(Suppl 5):857.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Chaput JP, Colley RC, Aubert S, Carson V, Janssen I, Roberts KC, Tremblay MS. Proportion of preschool-aged children meeting the Canadian 24-hour movement guidelines and associations with adiposity: results from the Canadian health measures survey. BMC Public Health. 2017;17(Suppl 5):829.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Chaput JP, Colley RC, Aubert S, Carson V, Janssen I, Roberts KC, Tremblay MS. Proportion of preschool-aged children meeting the Canadian 24-hour movement guidelines and associations with adiposity: results from the Canadian health measures survey. BMC Public Health. 2017;17(Suppl 5):829.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Paruthi S, Brooks LJ, D'Ambrosio C, Hall WA, Kotagal S, Lloyd RM, Malow BA, Maski K, Nichols C, Quan SF, et al. Recommended amount of sleep for pediatric populations: a consensus statement of the American academy of sleep medicine. J Clin Sleep Med. 2016;12(6):785–6.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Paruthi S, Brooks LJ, D'Ambrosio C, Hall WA, Kotagal S, Lloyd RM, Malow BA, Maski K, Nichols C, Quan SF, et al. Recommended amount of sleep for pediatric populations: a consensus statement of the American academy of sleep medicine. J Clin Sleep Med. 2016;12(6):785–6.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Hirshkowitz M, Whiton K, Albert SM, Alessi C, Bruni O, DonCarlos L, Hazen N, Herman J, Katz ES, Kheirandish-Gozal L, et al. National Sleep Foundation’s sleep time duration recommendations: methodology and results summary. Sleep Health. 2015;1(1):40–3.PubMedCrossRef Hirshkowitz M, Whiton K, Albert SM, Alessi C, Bruni O, DonCarlos L, Hazen N, Herman J, Katz ES, Kheirandish-Gozal L, et al. National Sleep Foundation’s sleep time duration recommendations: methodology and results summary. Sleep Health. 2015;1(1):40–3.PubMedCrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Thompson DA, Christakis DA. The association between television viewing and irregular sleep schedules among children less than 3 years of age. Pediatrics. 2005;116(4):851–6.PubMedCrossRef Thompson DA, Christakis DA. The association between television viewing and irregular sleep schedules among children less than 3 years of age. Pediatrics. 2005;116(4):851–6.PubMedCrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Gradisar M, Wolfson AR, Harvey AG, Hale L, Rosenberg R, Czeisler CA. The sleep and technology use of Americans: findings from the National Sleep Foundation’s 2011 sleep in America poll. J Clin Sleep Med. 2013;9(12):1291–9.PubMedPubMedCentral Gradisar M, Wolfson AR, Harvey AG, Hale L, Rosenberg R, Czeisler CA. The sleep and technology use of Americans: findings from the National Sleep Foundation’s 2011 sleep in America poll. J Clin Sleep Med. 2013;9(12):1291–9.PubMedPubMedCentral
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Punamaki RL, Wallenius M, Nygard CH, Saarni L, Rimpela A. Use of information and communication technology (ICT) and perceived health in adolescence: the role of sleeping habits and waking-time tiredness. J Adolesc. 2007;30(4):569–85.PubMedCrossRef Punamaki RL, Wallenius M, Nygard CH, Saarni L, Rimpela A. Use of information and communication technology (ICT) and perceived health in adolescence: the role of sleeping habits and waking-time tiredness. J Adolesc. 2007;30(4):569–85.PubMedCrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Falbe J, Davison KK, Franckle RL, Ganter C, Gortmaker SL, Smith L, Land T, Taveras EM. Sleep duration, restfulness, and screens in the sleep environment. Pediatrics. 2015;135(2):e367–75.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Falbe J, Davison KK, Franckle RL, Ganter C, Gortmaker SL, Smith L, Land T, Taveras EM. Sleep duration, restfulness, and screens in the sleep environment. Pediatrics. 2015;135(2):e367–75.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Mak YW, Wu CS, Hui DW, Lam SP, Tse HY, Yu WY, Wong HT. Association between screen viewing duration and sleep duration, sleep quality, and excessive daytime sleepiness among adolescents in Hong Kong. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2014;11(11):11201–19.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mak YW, Wu CS, Hui DW, Lam SP, Tse HY, Yu WY, Wong HT. Association between screen viewing duration and sleep duration, sleep quality, and excessive daytime sleepiness among adolescents in Hong Kong. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2014;11(11):11201–19.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Hysing M, Pallesen S, Stormark KM, Jakobsen R, Lundervold AJ, Sivertsen B. Sleep and use of electronic devices in adolescence: results from a large population-based study. BMJ Open. 2015;5(1):e006748.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hysing M, Pallesen S, Stormark KM, Jakobsen R, Lundervold AJ, Sivertsen B. Sleep and use of electronic devices in adolescence: results from a large population-based study. BMJ Open. 2015;5(1):e006748.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Dong S, Song Y, Jiang Y, Sun W, Wang Y, Jiang F. Multi-center study on the effects of television viewing on sleep quality among children under 4 years of age in China. Zhonghua Er Ke Za Zhi. 2015;53(12):907–12.PubMed Dong S, Song Y, Jiang Y, Sun W, Wang Y, Jiang F. Multi-center study on the effects of television viewing on sleep quality among children under 4 years of age in China. Zhonghua Er Ke Za Zhi. 2015;53(12):907–12.PubMed
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Carter B, Rees P, Hale L, Bhattacharjee D, Paradkar MS. Association between portable screen-based Media device access or use and sleep outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170(12):1202–8.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Carter B, Rees P, Hale L, Bhattacharjee D, Paradkar MS. Association between portable screen-based Media device access or use and sleep outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170(12):1202–8.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Kabali HK, Irigoyen MM, Nunez-Davis R, Budacki JG, Mohanty SH, Leister KP, Bonner RL Jr. Exposure and use of mobile media devices by young children. Pediatrics. 2015;136(6):1044–50.PubMedCrossRef Kabali HK, Irigoyen MM, Nunez-Davis R, Budacki JG, Mohanty SH, Leister KP, Bonner RL Jr. Exposure and use of mobile media devices by young children. Pediatrics. 2015;136(6):1044–50.PubMedCrossRef
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Wartella E, Rideout V, Lauricella AR, Connell S. Parenting in the age of digital technology. Report for the Center on Media and Human Development School of Communication Northwestern University. 2013 Jun. Wartella E, Rideout V, Lauricella AR, Connell S. Parenting in the age of digital technology. Report for the Center on Media and Human Development School of Communication Northwestern University. 2013 Jun.
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Hanafin S. Sleep patterns and problems in infants and young children in Ireland. Child Care Health Dev. 2018;44(3):470–5.PubMedCrossRef Hanafin S. Sleep patterns and problems in infants and young children in Ireland. Child Care Health Dev. 2018;44(3):470–5.PubMedCrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Linebarger DL, Vaala SE. Screen media and language development in infants and toddlers: an ecological perspective. Dev Rev. 2010;30(2):176–202.CrossRef Linebarger DL, Vaala SE. Screen media and language development in infants and toddlers: an ecological perspective. Dev Rev. 2010;30(2):176–202.CrossRef
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson CA, Bushman BJ. Effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive affect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: a meta-analytic review of the scientific literature. Psychol Sci. 2001;12(5):353–9.PubMedCrossRef Anderson CA, Bushman BJ. Effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive affect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: a meta-analytic review of the scientific literature. Psychol Sci. 2001;12(5):353–9.PubMedCrossRef
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Gentile DA, Oberg C, Sherwood NE, Story M, Walsh DA, Hogan M, American Academy of P. Well-child visits in the video age: pediatricians and the American academy of pediatrics’ guidelines for children’s media use. Pediatrics. 2004;114(5):1235–41.PubMedCrossRef Gentile DA, Oberg C, Sherwood NE, Story M, Walsh DA, Hogan M, American Academy of P. Well-child visits in the video age: pediatricians and the American academy of pediatrics’ guidelines for children’s media use. Pediatrics. 2004;114(5):1235–41.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Screen viewing behavior and sleep duration among children aged 2 and below
verfasst von
Bozhi Chen
Rob M. van Dam
Chuen Seng Tan
Hwee Ling Chua
Pey Gein Wong
Jonathan Y. Bernard
Falk Müller-Riemenschneider
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2019
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Public Health / Ausgabe 1/2019
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2458
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-018-6385-6

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2019

BMC Public Health 1/2019 Zur Ausgabe