Skip to main content
Erschienen in: The Cerebellum 4/2008

Open Access 01.12.2008

State Estimation in the Cerebellum

verfasst von: R. Chris Miall, Dominic King

Erschienen in: The Cerebellum | Ausgabe 4/2008

download
DOWNLOAD
print
DRUCKEN
insite
SUCHEN

Abstract

An exciting hypothesis about the cerebellum is that its role is one of state estimation—a process that combines efferent copies of motor commands with afferent sensory signals to produce a representation of the current status of the peripheral motor system. Sensory inputs alone cannot provide a perfect state signal because of inevitable delays in their afferent pathways. We have recently reported the effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over the ipsilateral cerebellum as healthy subjects made rapid reaching movements towards visually defined targets (Miall et al. in PLoS Biology 5:2733–2744, 2007). Errors in the initial direction and in the final finger position of this reach-to-target movement were consistent with the reaching movements being planned and initiated from an estimated hand position that was about 138 ms out of date. This interval is consistent with estimates of the delays in sensory motor pathways that would inform the central nervous system of the peripheral status. We now report new data using the same paradigm, testing the effects of varying the TMS stimulus train from one, two, or three pulses. We show that the errors in movement are relatively insensitive to the TMS pulse-train duration. The estimated time interval by which the hand position is mislocalized varied by only 12 ms as the TMS train duration increased by 100 ms. Thus, this interval is likely to reflect physiological processes within the cerebellum rather than the TMS-stimulus duration. This new evidence supports our earlier claim that the cerebellum is responsible for predictively updating a central state estimate over an interval of about 120–140 ms. Dysfunction of the cerebellum, whether through disease or experimental procedures, leads to motor errors consistent with a loss of knowledge of the true state of the motor system.

Introduction

Recent work in computational or theoretical motor control has been based around the concepts of internal models [1], optimal control [2], and probability [3]. In essence, this broad literature seeks to understand how the central nervous system controls a highly complex nonlinear and often unstable biomechanical system comprising the joints and muscles of the body. It must use optimal—or at least sufficient—strategies in the face of uncertainty about the exact properties of the system due to noise and delay in sensory systems, in central neural pathways, and in motor execution. And, it is assumed that the central nervous system (CNS) maintains information in the form of internal models—neural representations of the properties of the system it is controlling. The cerebellum is thought to be important for learning and storage of these internals models [46], and much experimental research has addressed this hypothesis. A particularly powerful idea is that the cerebellum forms a “forward model”—an internal model that generates a prediction of the expected outcome of motor commands [7]. This predictive signal could be used to plan actions, to help control on-going movements, to coordinate two or more simultaneous actions, and to update a central representation of the current state of the body. It is this last hypothetical role that we address in this paper.
The CNS needs an internal “state estimate” because it does not know the current state of the peripheral motor system because of unavoidable conduction delays in efferent and afferent pathways. Hence, sensory afferents are out of date by the time they reach the CNS, while any recent motor commands may not have yet affected the musculature [8]. Sensory-motor delays vary across modality but may be in the order of 100–300 ms [912]. Thus, the brain generates an estimate of the true state of the peripheral motor system, integrating incoming sensory information with predictions of the consequences of outgoing motor commands [5, 1315]. Hence, state estimation is closely coupled with forward modeling [7]. The cerebellum receives ascending proprioceptive inputs and efferent copies of descending motor commands, and it outputs to cortical and brain stem motor nuclei [4]. It also has the necessary adaptive mechanisms to support this hypothesized role, as forward model predictions must be refined and maintained by experience-based motor learning [4, 7]. So, to test the role of the cerebellum in these processes, we have recorded the response properties of Purkinje cells in the lateral cerebellum of monkeys performing a visually guided tracking task and have shown that they encode the direction of movement of a cursor on the screen even when the monkey’s hand moves in the opposite direction [16]. Thus, they appear to code the visual consequences of the actions, as predicted by the forward model hypothesis. Others have reported similar results suggesting the lateral cerebellum encodes the future kinematics of arm movements and not dynamics [17, 18]. The responses of the human cerebellum measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging also confirm its forward model role in coordinating actions, apparently using predictive information from one effector to help control the motion of another [19, 20]. Of course, the cerebellum is interconnected with other brain systems, and the forward model and state estimation processes may be shared with cortical structures including the superior parietal cortex [11, 2123] or elsewhere, depending on task [24].
In a recent paper [25], we reported the effects of brief disruption of the cerebellum during a reach-to-target action. We used transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over the ipsilateral cerebellum during subject’s reaction time to respond to an auditory cue and to point, without vision, to a previously observed target location. We predicted that TMS-induced disruption of the cerebellum would lead to inaccuracies in movement because the state estimation signals would be lost. A rapid reaching action made without accurate knowledge of the current state of the hand (its position and velocity) would overshoot [4], analogous to the hypermetria of cerebellar patients [26]. Moreover, if the reaching action started while the hand was already in motion, the initial direction of the movement towards the target would be inaccurate. For example, during a rightwards movement, plans to move forward to the target should reflect the continuously updated estimate of the hand’s current position. Without an accurate state estimate, the reach-to-target would be misdirected, reflecting the mislocated starting position of the hand. Both these predictions were borne out—there were small but significant increases in terminal errors when 100 ms of TMS was applied over the cerebellum but not elsewhere, and cerebellar TMS induced movement direction errors when the reaching movement started during a rightwards or leftwards action but not if the hand was initially stationary [25]. This last result reflects the expected insensitivity of movement direction if the starting posture is known and fixed, as the current state estimate is then no longer time dependent.
However, this work could not resolve an unknown relationship between the fixed duration of the TMS stimulation that we used and our estimate of the functional effect of about 138 ms. It was not clear if the 138 ms merely reflected the TMS duration (100 ms) plus an additional neuronal recovery time (38 ms).
In the present work, we directly test this relationship by repeating the original experiment but varying the TMS stimulus train from one to three pulses at 50 ms intervals. In other words, we varied stimulus duration from 0, 50 to 100 ms and tested its effects on terminal errors, on directional errors, and on the duration of the functional effect. We hypothesize that the state-estimation interval is unrelated to the TMS duration and instead reflects the interval over which the cerebellum predictively estimates the current state of the motor apparatus. Our null hypothesis is that the interval is directly related to the duration of the TMS-induced disruption of the cerebellum.

Methods

Thirteen subjects viewed a virtual image of a static target in 3D space ahead of them and started each trial by lifting their right index finger from a start key and moving slowly and steadily towards their right (Fig. 1a). Liquid crystal display goggles blocked the view of their hand and of the target as soon at the start key was released. An auditory go cue, 500–1500 ms after trial onset, instructed them to make a rapid upward and leftward pointing movement to the virtual target. We refer to this task as “dynamic.” Their index finger had typically moved laterally 10–40 cm from its original position when the go cue was delivered. On a random 50% of the 90 trials in each block, TMS was delivered within their reaction time after the auditory go cue, as a one-, two-, or three-pulse train of stimuli separated by 50 ms. Vision was allowed after the reach-to-target motion was complete, avoiding any slow drifting of accuracy across trials. In separate blocks, subjects also performed a “static” task. Here, the start button was moved laterally by 20 cm, and the subject was instructed to lift the index finger off the button but to hold stationary until the go cue; they then made a reach to the target from this static, constant, position. For other details of the task, refer to our previous paper [25]. Participants gave informal consent, and the experiments were approved by local ethics committee.

Results

Screening
We predict effects on azimuth angles in the dynamic task [25], and so we first screened participants for an effect of TMS on this measure with a very liberal inclusion criterion of p < 0.5. We rejected data from three participants who showed no differences between TMS and non-TMS trials, probably because of inter-subject differences in stimulation threshold or in coil-to-cerebellar distance (the coil was positioned relative to the inion). The remaining ten data sets were then analyzed in detail. Group mean trajectories for TMS and non-TMS trials in the dynamic task are shown in Fig. 1b. We concentrate in these results only on the dynamic task results. There were no significant differences in terminal errors between the static and dynamic task (analysis of variance, F(1,6) = 0.35, NS) or for elevation angles (F(1,6) = 0.24, NS); nor were the condition by TMS interactions significant. Expected differences in azimuth angle between the two conditions [25] are described below.
Terminal Error
The increase in end-point error for TMS trials (Fig. 2a) confirmed the original results, with a significant increase in error in all three TMS conditions compared to non-TMS trials (t(9) > 6.3, p < 0.0001, one-sample t tests). There was no evidence of an increase in error with the increasing number of TMS pulses, and although the errors were somewhat smaller than those observed with the original large sample, a comparison between the new and old three-pulse conditions was not statistically significant (t(40) = 1.6, p = 0.117).
Directional Error: Azimuth Angle
For the dynamic task, measurement of the difference in average direction between the TMS and non-TMS trials showed a significant TMS-induced change in azimuth angles for all three TMS conditions (Fig. 2b; t(9) > 4.8, p < 0.001) and, again, no significant difference between the new and old three-pulse conditions (t(40) = 0.78, p = 0.437). As expected, there were highly significant differences between the dynamic and static conditions (F(1,6) = 20, p = 0.004), as azimuth angles were not significantly different from zero in the static condition and did not change significantly with increasing TMS number. The interaction between task condition and TMS number was not significant (F(2,12) = 2.76, p = 0.10).
Elevation Angle
There were also significant changes in elevation angles (Fig. 2c; t(9) > 2.7, p < 0.02) and no difference between the new and old groups (t(40) = 0.82, p = 0.417). There was a trend towards an increase in the angular deviation as the number of TMS pulses increased, although this was not statistically significant in azimuth (F(2,18) = 0.398, p = 0.68). For elevation, this was significant (F(2,18) = 4.39, p = 0.028), with a linear increase in elevation error with increasing TMS pulses (F(1,9) = 13.1, p = 0.006).
State Estimation Interval
This is determined from the angular deviation in azimuth (Fig. 2b). The mean deviation was about 4° clockwise, consistent with a leftwards mislocalization of the hand position (i.e., backwards in time). We can therefore backtrack along the average movement trajectory to find the position at which the start-to-target angle is shifted by the same ∼4° amount and use the velocity of the hand to convert this distance to a time interval. These results are shown in Fig. 3, again in comparison with the previous data [25]. These intervals are smaller than previously seen, although the difference is not statistically significant (t(40) = 0.79, p = 0.435). The trend for the intervals to increase with increasing number of TMS pulses was not significant (F(2,18) = 0.2, p = 0.82).

Discussion

These results confirm that TMS over the lateral cerebellum during dynamic actions resulted in a directional deviation of the reaching movement and in increased positional error. These results confirm previous findings that the effects depend on the task, and TMS in the static condition does not generate significant errors in azimuth. Moreover, they strongly argue that the magnitude of the behavioral effect observed is not simply a reflection of the duration of the TMS train, as there was only a limited sensitivity to one, two, or three TMS pulses. Of particular importance is the finding that the estimated time by which the hand is mislocalized (Fig. 3) varied only 12 ms as the TMS stimulus train increased by 100 ms.
It is not certain what duration of effect a single TMS pulse would have on processing within the cerebellar system. Judging by its influence on primary motor cortex, the effect could be brief: there is a narrow window of 5–10 ms within which motor cortical excitability is reduced after each cerebellar shock [27, 28]. However, this indirect measure of cerebello-thalamic-cortical modulation does not indicate how long the cerebellar cortex itself takes to recover, and it is likely to be much longer. Judging by the effects of TMS over visual cortex on image detection [29] or over motor areas on reaction times [30], neocortical recovery takes 30–60 ms after a supra-threshold pulse. Adding ∼30 ms to the one-, two-, or three-pulse trains, we might expect our TMS protocol to disrupt the cerebellar hemisphere for about 30, 80 and 130 ms, respectively. The latter is close to the observed interval (Fig. 3). But, it is also close to the delay of sensory afferents informing the CNS about the previous hand positions [9, 10, 15]. And, since the differences between one, two, and three pulses were not significant (except in elevation angle), we suggest that the original 138 ms state estimation interval, based on a large sample size, is instead a good measure of the time over which the cerebellum anticipates the state of the ongoing hand movements.
There was, however, a trend for an increased effect with longer TMS trains, and the elevation angles were significantly related to the number of TMS pulses. This may indicate temporal summation of the effect, such that the three-pulse stimulus is more effective in disrupting the cerebellum than a single pulse [31]. Given that even three-pulse TMS does not completely block processing in the relevant arm areas of the anterior and/or posterior cerebellum, our estimate of ∼120–140 ms may be a slight underestimate but is not likely to be badly wrong.
Another issue is the localization of these TMS effects in the cerebellum. Prior research using TMS to condition the excitability of motor cortex [27, 28] is consistent with the brief excitation of the Purkinje cells, leading to cerebellar inhibition. It is also clear that the target site affects the hand area of motor cortex, consistent with activation of lateral cerebellar cortex, probably in lobules V and VI. It is not clear how widespread within neighboring areas of the cerebellum the effect is.
It also remains for us to test whether the effects we report depend only on the cerebellum or whether the state estimation process is shared with other areas [32] and whether the cerebellar state estimates are used to update other cortical regions. These will be the aims of further experiments.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust.

Open Access

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.
Open AccessThis is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License (https://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by-nc/​2.​0), which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Neurologie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes, den Premium-Inhalten der neurologischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Neurologie-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Hwang EJ, Shadmehr R (2005) Internal models of limb dynamics and the encoding of limb state. J Neural Eng 2(3):S266–S278PubMedCrossRef Hwang EJ, Shadmehr R (2005) Internal models of limb dynamics and the encoding of limb state. J Neural Eng 2(3):S266–S278PubMedCrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Todorov E, Jordan MI (2002) Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat Neurosci 5(11):1226–1235PubMedCrossRef Todorov E, Jordan MI (2002) Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat Neurosci 5(11):1226–1235PubMedCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Kording KP, Wolpert DM (2004) Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning. Nature 427(6971):244–247PubMedCrossRef Kording KP, Wolpert DM (2004) Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning. Nature 427(6971):244–247PubMedCrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Miall RC, Weir DJ, Wolpert DM, Stein JF (1993) Is the cerebellum a Smith Predictor? J Motor Behav 25:203–216 Miall RC, Weir DJ, Wolpert DM, Stein JF (1993) Is the cerebellum a Smith Predictor? J Motor Behav 25:203–216
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Wolpert DM, Miall RC, Kawato M (1998) Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends Cogn Sci 2:338–347CrossRef Wolpert DM, Miall RC, Kawato M (1998) Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends Cogn Sci 2:338–347CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Paulin MG (1989) A Kalman filter theory of the cerebellum. In: Arbib MA, Amari S (eds) Dynamic interactions in neural networks: models and data. Springer, New York Paulin MG (1989) A Kalman filter theory of the cerebellum. In: Arbib MA, Amari S (eds) Dynamic interactions in neural networks: models and data. Springer, New York
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Miall RC, Wolpert DM (1996) Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Netw 9:1265–1279PubMedCrossRef Miall RC, Wolpert DM (1996) Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Netw 9:1265–1279PubMedCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Miall RC, Weir DJ, Stein JF (1985) Visuomotor tracking with delayed visual feedback. Neurosci 16:511–520CrossRef Miall RC, Weir DJ, Stein JF (1985) Visuomotor tracking with delayed visual feedback. Neurosci 16:511–520CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Saunders JA, Knill DC (2003) Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control fast reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 152(3):341–352PubMedCrossRef Saunders JA, Knill DC (2003) Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control fast reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 152(3):341–352PubMedCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Desmurget M, Grafton S (2000) Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends Cogn Sci 4(11):423–431PubMedCrossRef Desmurget M, Grafton S (2000) Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends Cogn Sci 4(11):423–431PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Saunders JA, Knill DC (2005) Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control both the direction and distance of pointing movements. Exp Brain Res 162(4):458–473PubMedCrossRef Saunders JA, Knill DC (2005) Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control both the direction and distance of pointing movements. Exp Brain Res 162(4):458–473PubMedCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z (2000) Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nat Neurosci 3:1212–1217 (Suppl)PubMedCrossRef Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z (2000) Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nat Neurosci 3:1212–1217 (Suppl)PubMedCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI (1995) An internal model for sensorimotor control. Science 269:1880–1882PubMedCrossRef Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI (1995) An internal model for sensorimotor control. Science 269:1880–1882PubMedCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Ariff G, Donchin O, Nanayakkara T, Shadmehr R (2002) A real-time state predictor in motor control: study of saccadic eye movements during unseen reaching movements. J Neurosci 22(17):7721–7729PubMed Ariff G, Donchin O, Nanayakkara T, Shadmehr R (2002) A real-time state predictor in motor control: study of saccadic eye movements during unseen reaching movements. J Neurosci 22(17):7721–7729PubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu X, Robertson E, Miall RC (2003) Neuronal activity related to the visual representation of arm movements in the lateral cerebellar cortex. J Neurophysiol 89(3):1223–1237PubMedCrossRef Liu X, Robertson E, Miall RC (2003) Neuronal activity related to the visual representation of arm movements in the lateral cerebellar cortex. J Neurophysiol 89(3):1223–1237PubMedCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Roitman AV, Pasalar S, Johnson MT, Ebner TJ (2005) Position, direction of movement, and speed tuning of cerebellar Purkinje cells during circular manual tracking in monkey. J Neurosci 25(40):9244–9257PubMedCrossRef Roitman AV, Pasalar S, Johnson MT, Ebner TJ (2005) Position, direction of movement, and speed tuning of cerebellar Purkinje cells during circular manual tracking in monkey. J Neurosci 25(40):9244–9257PubMedCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Pasalar S, Roitman AV, Durfee WK, Ebner TJ (2006) Force field effects on cerebellar Purkinje cell discharge with implications for internal models. Nat Neurosci 9(11):1404–1411PubMedCrossRef Pasalar S, Roitman AV, Durfee WK, Ebner TJ (2006) Force field effects on cerebellar Purkinje cell discharge with implications for internal models. Nat Neurosci 9(11):1404–1411PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Miall RC, Reckess GZ, Imamizu H (2001) The cerebellum coordinates eye and hand tracking movements. Nat Neurosci 4(6):638–644PubMedCrossRef Miall RC, Reckess GZ, Imamizu H (2001) The cerebellum coordinates eye and hand tracking movements. Nat Neurosci 4(6):638–644PubMedCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Kawato M, Kuroda T, Imamizu H, Nakano E, Miyauchi S, Yoshioka T (2003) Internal forward models in the cerebellum: fMRI study on grip force and load force coupling. Prog Brain Res 142:171–188PubMedCrossRef Kawato M, Kuroda T, Imamizu H, Nakano E, Miyauchi S, Yoshioka T (2003) Internal forward models in the cerebellum: fMRI study on grip force and load force coupling. Prog Brain Res 142:171–188PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Wolpert DM, Goodbody SJ, Husain M (1998) Maintaining internal representations: the role of the human superior parietal lobe. Nat Neurosci 1(6):529–533PubMedCrossRef Wolpert DM, Goodbody SJ, Husain M (1998) Maintaining internal representations: the role of the human superior parietal lobe. Nat Neurosci 1(6):529–533PubMedCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Desmurget M, Epstein CM, Turner RS, Prablanc C, Alexander GE, Grafton ST (1999) Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target. Nat Neurosci 2(6):563–567PubMedCrossRef Desmurget M, Epstein CM, Turner RS, Prablanc C, Alexander GE, Grafton ST (1999) Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target. Nat Neurosci 2(6):563–567PubMedCrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Buneo CA, Andersen RA (2006) The posterior parietal cortex: sensorimotor interface for the planning and online control of visually guided movements. Neuropsychologia 44:2594–2606PubMedCrossRef Buneo CA, Andersen RA (2006) The posterior parietal cortex: sensorimotor interface for the planning and online control of visually guided movements. Neuropsychologia 44:2594–2606PubMedCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Leube DT, Knoblich G, Erb M, Grodd W, Bartels M, Kircher TT (2003) The neural correlates of perceiving one’s own movements. Neuroimage 20(4):2084–2090PubMedCrossRef Leube DT, Knoblich G, Erb M, Grodd W, Bartels M, Kircher TT (2003) The neural correlates of perceiving one’s own movements. Neuroimage 20(4):2084–2090PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Miall RC, Christensen LO, Cain O, Stanley J (2007) Disruption of state estimation in the human lateral cerebellum. PLoS Biol 5(11):2733–2744CrossRef Miall RC, Christensen LO, Cain O, Stanley J (2007) Disruption of state estimation in the human lateral cerebellum. PLoS Biol 5(11):2733–2744CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Holmes G (1917) The symptoms of acute cerebellar injuries due to gunshot injuries. Brain 40:461–535CrossRef Holmes G (1917) The symptoms of acute cerebellar injuries due to gunshot injuries. Brain 40:461–535CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Werhahn KJ, Taylor J, Ridding M, Meyer BU, Rothwell JC (1996) Effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation over the cerebellum on the excitability of human motor cortex. Electroenceph Clin Neurophysiol 101(1):58–66PubMedCrossRef Werhahn KJ, Taylor J, Ridding M, Meyer BU, Rothwell JC (1996) Effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation over the cerebellum on the excitability of human motor cortex. Electroenceph Clin Neurophysiol 101(1):58–66PubMedCrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Ugawa Y, Uesaka Y, Terao Y, Hanajima R, Kanazawa I (1995) Magnetic stimulation over the cerebellum in humans. Ann Neurol 37(6):703–713PubMedCrossRef Ugawa Y, Uesaka Y, Terao Y, Hanajima R, Kanazawa I (1995) Magnetic stimulation over the cerebellum in humans. Ann Neurol 37(6):703–713PubMedCrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Amassian VE, Maccabee PJ, Cracco RQ, Cracco JB, Rudell AP, Eberle L (1993) Measurement of information processing delays in human visual cortex with repetitive magnetic coil stimulation. Brain Res 605:317–321PubMedCrossRef Amassian VE, Maccabee PJ, Cracco RQ, Cracco JB, Rudell AP, Eberle L (1993) Measurement of information processing delays in human visual cortex with repetitive magnetic coil stimulation. Brain Res 605:317–321PubMedCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Day BL, Rothwell JC, Thompson PD, Maertens DN, Nakashima K, Shannon K et al (1989) Delay in the execution of voluntary movement by electrical or magnetic brain stimulation in intact man. Evidence for the storage of motor programs in the brain. Brain 112(Pt 3):649–663PubMedCrossRef Day BL, Rothwell JC, Thompson PD, Maertens DN, Nakashima K, Shannon K et al (1989) Delay in the execution of voluntary movement by electrical or magnetic brain stimulation in intact man. Evidence for the storage of motor programs in the brain. Brain 112(Pt 3):649–663PubMedCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Kujirai T, Caramia MD, Rothwell JC, Day BL, Thompson PD, Ferbert A et al (1993) Corticocortical inhibition in human motor cortex. J Physiol 471:501–519PubMed Kujirai T, Caramia MD, Rothwell JC, Day BL, Thompson PD, Ferbert A et al (1993) Corticocortical inhibition in human motor cortex. J Physiol 471:501–519PubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Mulliken GH, Musallam S, Andersen RA (2008) Forward estimation of movement state in posterior parietal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105(24):8170–8177PubMedCrossRef Mulliken GH, Musallam S, Andersen RA (2008) Forward estimation of movement state in posterior parietal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105(24):8170–8177PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
State Estimation in the Cerebellum
verfasst von
R. Chris Miall
Dominic King
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2008
Verlag
Springer-Verlag
Erschienen in
The Cerebellum / Ausgabe 4/2008
Print ISSN: 1473-4222
Elektronische ISSN: 1473-4230
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-008-0072-6

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 4/2008

The Cerebellum 4/2008 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Sind Frauen die fähigeren Ärzte?

30.04.2024 Gendermedizin Nachrichten

Patienten, die von Ärztinnen behandelt werden, dürfen offenbar auf bessere Therapieergebnisse hoffen als Patienten von Ärzten. Besonders gilt das offenbar für weibliche Kranke, wie eine Studie zeigt.

Akuter Schwindel: Wann lohnt sich eine MRT?

28.04.2024 Schwindel Nachrichten

Akuter Schwindel stellt oft eine diagnostische Herausforderung dar. Wie nützlich dabei eine MRT ist, hat eine Studie aus Finnland untersucht. Immerhin einer von sechs Patienten wurde mit akutem ischämischem Schlaganfall diagnostiziert.

Niedriger diastolischer Blutdruck erhöht Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Komplikationen

25.04.2024 Hypotonie Nachrichten

Wenn unter einer medikamentösen Hochdrucktherapie der diastolische Blutdruck in den Keller geht, steigt das Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Ereignisse: Darauf deutet eine Sekundäranalyse der SPRINT-Studie hin.

Frühe Alzheimertherapie lohnt sich

25.04.2024 AAN-Jahrestagung 2024 Nachrichten

Ist die Tau-Last noch gering, scheint der Vorteil von Lecanemab besonders groß zu sein. Und beginnen Erkrankte verzögert mit der Behandlung, erreichen sie nicht mehr die kognitive Leistung wie bei einem früheren Start. Darauf deuten neue Analysen der Phase-3-Studie Clarity AD.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.