Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Public Health 1/2023

Open Access 01.12.2023 | Research

The impact of boarding schools on the development of cognitive and non-cognitive abilities in adolescents

verfasst von: Fang Chang, Yanan Huo, Songyan Zhang, Hang Zeng, Bin Tang

Erschienen in: BMC Public Health | Ausgabe 1/2023

Abstract

Background

Since China adopted a policy to eliminate rural learning centers, boarding has become an important feature of the current rural student community. However, there is a lack of consensus on the impact of boarding schools on students' cognitive and non-cognitive development. This study investigates the effect of boarding schools on the development of cognitive and non-cognitive abilities of junior high school students in rural northwest China.

Methods

Using a sample of 5,660 seventh-grade students from 160 rural junior high schools across 19 counties, we identify a causal relationship between boarding and student abilities with the instrumental variables (IV) approach.

Results

The results suggest that boarding positively influences memory and attention, while it has no significant effect on other cognitive abilities such as reasoning, transcription speed, and accuracy. Furthermore, we find no significant association between boarding and the development of non-cognitive skills.

Conclusions

Given the widespread prevalence of boarding schools in rural regions, our study highlights the growing importance of improving school management to promote the development of students’ cognitive abilities and integrating the development of non-cognitive or social-emotional abilities into students’ daily routines.
Hinweise

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

Human ability is composed of cognitive and non-cognitive components, both of which are crucial to an individual's life [1, 2]. The influence of cognitive and non-cognitive abilities has been observed in various aspects of life, including academic performance, educational choices, wages, labor market outcomes, employment decisions, health behaviors, and social integration [35].
Cognitive and non-cognitive abilities are core components of human capital [1, 2, 6]. Cognitive abilities are the endowments for extracting, storing and utilizing information from the objective world, This encompasses skills such as logical reasoning, abstract thinking and memory [7], while non-cognitive abilities have emerged as a concept distinct from cognitive abilities, aiming to distinguish factors beyond cognitive itself. These encompass qualities such as motivation, authority, work norms, self-control, perseverance, and more [8]. Numerous researches have shown that cognitive and non-cognitive abilities play an important role in academic performance, educational decisions, wages, labor market performance, employment choices, health behaviors and social integration [1, 35, 8, 9]. Prior research has revealed a considerable disparity between the cognitive abilities of rural and urban students, with urban students scoring significantly higher on word and mathematics tests by 0.75 and 0.54 standard deviations, respectively [6, 10]. Rural students also tend to exhibit lower levels of non-cognitive skills, including depression, self-esteem, and values, with left-behind children experiencing even greater disadvantages [11].
Beginning in 2001, China adopted a policy to eliminate rural learning centers, leading to the consolidation of educational resources and the growth of rural boarding schools. By the end of 2016, 26.08 million rural students were enrolled as compulsory boarders, comprising 27.5% of the total student population. Of these, 16.66 million were boarding students in rural junior high schools, amounting to a boarding rate of 58.6% [12]. Therefore, a comprehensive evaluation of the cognitive and non-cognitive development of boarding students in rural areas has become essential.
Studies have shown that the communal learning environment in boarding schools can increase learning time, optimize teaching resources, and provide more opportunities for boarders to communicate with their teachers and peers [13, 14]. However, boarding students may also be exposed to at-risk peers, which can have negative effects on their development [15, 16]. Boarding can also cause stress for students as they are separated from their familiar surroundings and parents, which can be particularly significant during critical growth stages [17]. Consequently, there is a lack of consensus on the impact of boarding schools on students' cognitive and non-cognitive development.
Extant research on boarding schools has primarily focused on elite schools in developed countries, which have generally been associated with positive academic performance [18]. However, public boarding schools have been set up in many developed countries for marginalized groups, such as the SEED public boarding schools in the US and the Internet Excellence program in France. Quasi-experimental studies have shown that boarding has had a significant positive impact on the academic performance of disadvantaged students in reading and mathematics [19]. Similarly, rural boarding schools in France have positively impacted academic performance, particularly for outstanding boarders, with significant improvements in French and mathematics scores two years after enrollment [14]. Nonetheless, studies in Turkey have reported a negative correlation between boarding and academic performance in Grades 5 to 9 [15]. Boarding also has a significantly negative impact on students' mental health, with boarders displaying more problem behaviors, such as anxiety, depression, hostility, substance abuse, alcohol dependency, and school bullying [20, 21]. Notably, the impact of boarding varies at different stages of development. For instance, Mander et al. (2015) found no significant differences in social, emotional, and psychological well-being between boarders and non-boarders in elementary schools [22]. However, boarders in secondary schools exhibited a higher incidence of emotional difficulties, depression, anxiety, and stress compared to non-boarders. Given the mixed evidence, it is crucial to carefully consider the potential positive and negative impacts of boarding, especially for disadvantaged students attending public boarding schools.
As boarding school enrollment continues to rise in China, researchers have investigated the effects of boarding on students' cognitive and non-cognitive abilities and reported conflicting findings. Qiao and Di (2014) found that boarding significantly improved rural students' performance in mathematics [23], while Mo et al. (2012) reported a significant negative effect of boarding on primary school students' math scores [24]. Similarly, Wang et al. (2016), Li et al. (2018), and Zhu et al. (2019) found that boarding had no significant impact on students' standardized math scores or even reduced their standardized language scores [2527]. Most studies indicate that boarding has a negative impact on students' non-cognitive skills. Rural boarders are more likely to experience bullying, loneliness, and depression in schools and have lower self-esteem, resilience, and emotional intelligence than non-boarders [2730]. Taken together, these results suggest that the effects of boarding on students' academic and non-academic outcomes are complex and may vary depending on a range of factors, including the type of boarding school, the students' developmental stage, and their socio-economic background.
This paper aims to contribute to the existing literature on the impact of boarding on rural students' cognitive and non-cognitive abilities in three ways. Firstly, the literature has primarily measured cognitive abilities using subject-specific scores, which may not fully capture the breadth of cognitive abilities. There are numerous studies on cognitive abilities in different disciplines. psychologists commonly differentiate between fluid intelligence, which emphasizes more general capacities such as logical reasoning and abstract thinking, and crystallized intelligence, which is related to the accumulation of concrete knowledge and experience [31, 32]. Academic performance, such as math and reading tests is often used to measure crystallized intelligence [33]. Conversely, fluid intelligence is frequently assessed through quotient tests (IQ tests), exemplified bu tools like the WISC-IV and Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices [34]. To improve accuracy and precision in measuring cognitive abilities, this paper utilizes the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC), Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices, and standardized mathematics scores. Secondly, the literature has relied on self-administered questionnaires to measure non-cognitive abilities, which may lack comprehensiveness and comparability. In contrast, this paper uses the Big Five Personality Inventory to measure non-cognitive abilities accurately [35]. Finally, prior studies have examined the effects of boarding on cognitive or non-cognitive abilities separately, which prevents a comprehensive assessment of the impact of boarding on students' human capital.
This study uses an instrumental variable approach to address endogeneity issues and analyzes data from 160 junior high schools in rural northwest China to illustrate the effects of boarding on students' cognitive and non-cognitive abilities. The results indicate a significant positive effect of boarding on the cognitive abilities of rural junior high school students, particularly in memory and attention, areas associated with fluid intelligence. However, boarding has no significant impact on the non-cognitive abilities of rural students. Furthermore, we provide evidence of heterogeneity in the impact of boarding on cognitive and non-cognitive abilities by gender. We also find a significant positive effect on the cognitive abilities of left-behind children and students from families with better socioeconomic status.

Method

Participants

We conducted our study on first-year rural high school (seventh grade) students in three prefectures from two provinces in northwest China. These provinces were below the national median in terms of GDP, according to the National Bureau of Statistics of China (2015). Hence, the sample of rural students in these provinces can be considered representative of students in low-income areas in rural China.
We constructed our sample in two steps. First, we selected 23 counties from three prefectures, two counties with more developed economic status were excluded, and the remaining were included. Second, we obtained a list of all 496 junior high schools from the counties in Step 1. After excluding non-rural schools and schools with less than 20 students in the seventh grade (to address potential sample attrition or school merger issues), we obtained a final sample containing 5,660 seventh-grade students from 160 schools (see Table 1).
Table 1
Sample distribution
 
Counties
Schools
Students
Boarding students
Non-boarding students
Students tested by Raven
Students tested by Wechsler
Students tested by the Big five
Total Sample
19
160
5660
2601
3059
2503
472
5166
Province A
7
90
3413
841
2572
1530
208
3136
Province B
12
70
2247
1760
487
973
264
2030

Procedure

The sample was collected in two phases. The first phase was carried out in 2015, which involved administering tests to collect information on basic details of the sample students, mathematics teachers, and schools using questionnaires. Mathematics scores of students were also collected through tests (see Table 2). In the second phase, conducted in 2016, additional tests were administered, which included more Raven's tests, Wechsler tests, the Big Five Personality test, and the Perseverance Scale (see Table 2).
Table 2
Descriptive statistics of the main variables
 
Definition
N
Mean
Std. Dev
Min
Max
Panel A: Outcome variables
 WISC-IV scores
WISC-IV test scores
472
88.258
10.948
59
124
 Fluid Intelligence
The average of working memory, perceptual reasoning, and processing speed scores
472
8.472
1.684
3.667
13.333
 Crystal intelligence
The verbal comprehension score
472
7.928
2.577
2
16
 Similarities
Scores of scale
472
7.928
2.577
2
16
 Digit span
Scores of scale
472
8.117
2.507
2
17
 Coding
Scores of scale
472
9.71
2.481
2
19
 Matrix Reasoning
Scores of scale
472
7.591
2.238
1
17
 Raven's IQ
Raven's test scores
2503
93.641
17.511
5.35
133.3
 Math scores of 2016
Standardized math scores
5188
.02
0.996
-2.071
2.517
 Extraversion
Scores of scale
5166
3.349
0.562
1.125
5
 Agreeableness
Scores of scale
5166
3.739
0.536
1.444
5
 Conscientiousness
Scores of scale
5166
3.279
0.570
1.111
5
 Neuroticism
Scores of scale
5166
2.913
0.625
1
5
 Openness
Scores of scale
5166
3.411
0.569
1.3
4.9
Panel B: Student Characteristics
 Boarding
1 = Student is boarding
at school
5660
0.460
0.498
0
1
 Student age
A student’s age in years
5660
12.917
0.964
10
17
 Student gender
1 = Male; 0 = Female
5660
0.499
0.500
0
1
 Left-behind child status
1 = Father or mother migrated to a city for work for more than six months in the past year
5660
0.525
0.499
0
1
 Father's education level
1 = Junior high school and above
0 = Junior high school below
5660
0.359
0.480
0
1
 Mather's education level
1 = Junior high school and above
0 = Junior high school below
5660
0.213
0.410
0
1
 Family asset value
Indicators of wealth in the family
5660
-0.001
1.074
-1.496
3.019
 Math scores of 2015
Standardized math scores
5660
-0.112
0.974
-3.167
2.551
Panel C:School Characteristics
 School size
Number of students
5660
217.91
96.641
46
447
 Teacher-student ratio
Number of teachers per student
5660
0.137
0.065
0.039
.474
 Time required for the school to be the furthest administrative village
hours
5660
1.22
0.777
0.05
4
 The rate of school boarders
The number of boarders divided by the total number of students in the school
5660
0.469
0.409
0
1
Panel D:Teacher Characteristics
 Teaching years
years
5660
9.072
6.954
0
38
 Teacher’s gender
1 = Male; 0 = Female
5660
0.636
0.481
0
1
 Teacher's education level
1 = junior college above
0 = junior college and below
5660
0.517
0.500
0
1
 Teacher’s age
A teacher’s age in years
5660
32.499
5.869
22.332
56
 Teacher’s title
0 = have no title;1 = third-grade title;2 = second-grade title;3 = first-grade tltle;4 = Senior professional title
5660
1.093
1.005
0
4
The data collection involved three steps: (1) recruiting and training researchers, (2) conducting questionnaire tests in schools, and (3) administering cognitive and non-cognitive ability tests. For (1), the project team recruited college students as researchers and provided uniform training and simulation exercises to ensure recruited researchers mastered standardized operations of the study, thus reducing measurement errors caused by inconsistent implementation by researchers. For (2), researchers organized students to take standardized math tests and questionnaires, which were developed by the project team in collaboration with the best secondary school teachers and calibrated to match the academic level appropriate for seventh-grade rural students. All sample schools used standardized math tests with identical questions assigned by the project team and proctored by researchers on-site. Researchers also conducted one-on-one questionnaire interviews with principals and mathematics teachers. In (3), cognitive ability tests included Raven's test and Wechsler's test. Raven's test was administered in a group and took approximately 45 min. The Wechsler test needed to be conducted one-on-one and required highly trained personnel, participants therefore received training in professional institutions. Additionally, the project team organized several practical exercises in non-sample schools to ensure the accuracy and consistency of the Wechsler test. Given the significant testing and time costs of the Wechsler test, three students from each sample class were randomly selected to take the Wechsler test individually. Students are selected based on their mathematics scores in the first research sample class, which were rank ordered into three groups: high, medium, and low; one student from each group was randomly selected for the Wechsler's test. The rest of the class took the Raven's test. The non-cognitive skills component primarily consisted of the Big Five personality test and the Perseverance Scale test, both of which were included in the student questionnaire.

Cognitive ability

The objective of this research is to investigate students' cognitive abilities, measured using three tests: the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV), Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices (Raven's IQ test), and a mathematics test. Cattell's (1987) suggested that cognitive abilities are divisible into two categories: crystallized intelligence and fluid intelligence [36]. The former pertains to skills attained through experience and knowledge, such as vocabulary, calculation, and verbal comprehension, whereas the latter refers to neural development, including perception, memory, and reasoning ability.
The WISC-IV is a tool for assessing intelligence in children aged 6 to 16 and comprises four indices: verbal comprehension, perceptual reasoning, working memory, and processing speed, along with the total IQ score [37]. The Chinese version of the WISC-IV short-form scale was employed in this research, which contains four subtests representing the four indices [38].1 The four subtests utilized for estimating the WISC IQ score were similarities, digit span, coding, and matrix reasoning. Similarities is designed to capture crystallized intelligence, while digit span, coding, and matrix reasoning are intended to measure fluid intelligence [39]. The aggregated WISC IQ score was used in the regression analysis.
The Raven's IQ test is a nonverbal test of intelligence that consists of pictorial questions related to spatial reasoning and pattern matching, which are designed to assess observational and thinking ability [40]. The test is culture-, language-, and age-neutral and consists of 60 questions that can be converted into IQ scores based on normative patterns. It is defined to capture fluid intelligence and was used for robustness testing in this study [31].2
The mathematics test, administered to all students in the sample, was developed by experienced secondary school mathematics teachers based on the standard high school syllabus. The test captures crystallized intelligence and was used for robustness testing in this study [33]. Several pre-studies of the questions were carried out by the research team to assess their suitability.

Non-cognitive ability

Non-cognitive abilities represent a fundamental component of human capital and can be examined through various skills and traits, including self-control, self-esteem, self-confidence, due diligence, perseverance, self-awareness, and communication skills [45]. We employed the Big Five personality traits and the Short Grit Scales as measures of non-cognitive abilities.
DeYoung's Big Five personality traits consist of neuroticism, agreeableness, conscientiousness, extraversion, and openness, which capture diverse aspects of personality. Neuroticism assesses emotional instability and sensitivity, such as anxiety, hostility, depression, self-consciousness, impulsivity, and vulnerability. Extraversion captures interpersonal skills, positive affect, and energy levels. Openness refers to the imagination and intellectual curiosity as reflected in personal fantasy, aesthetics, feelings, actions, ideas, and values. Agreeableness evaluates how a person interacts with others through levels of trust, frankness, altruism, submissiveness, humility, and gentleness. Finally, conscientiousness assesses competence, order responsibility, effortful achievement, self-discipline, and thoughtfulness [46]. The Big Five personality traits have been widely studied and are recognized as being stable across different languages, disciplines, and raters [47, 48].
The Short Grit Scale, developed by Duckworth et al. (2007), measures perseverance and passion for long-term goals [49]. This scale consists of eight questions that evaluate student attitudes and behaviors towards long-term goals, such as the tendency to prioritize new ideas over existing plans [50]. The Short Grit Scale has demonstrated strong internal consistency, test–retest stability, and high predictive validity [51]. Grit is considered a facet of Big Five conscientiousness and has gained recent attention in the literature on human achievement. In this study, we utilized it for robustness testing.

Model design

Consider a statistical model that links a student's cognitive and non-cognitive abilities, boarding status, and other determinants of ability as represented by:
$${Y}_{jis}=\alpha +{\beta }_{1}Boardin{g}_{jis}+{\beta }_{2}{W}_{jis}+{\mu }_{j}+{\varepsilon }_{jis}$$
(1)
where \({Y}_{jis}\) denotes the cognitive and non-cognitive abilities of student i in school s in county j; \(Boardin{g}_{jis}\) is an indicator of the student's boarding status (1 if boarding, 0 otherwise), and \({W}_{jis}\) is a set of exogenous covariates that includes student (e.g., age and gender), family (e.g., parental education), and school (e.g., teacher qualifications and school facilities) characteristics; \({\mu }_{j}\) is county fixed effect; and the error term \({\varepsilon }_{jis}\) captures the influence of all unobserved factors.
Equation (1) may be subject to endogeneity issues for two main reasons. First, reverse causality may arise where students with lower cognitive abilities or academic performance could be more likely to choose boarding [14]. This concern is particularly true in cases such as the French excellent boarding school program, which is designed to provide elite education for disadvantaged groups. In contrast, boarding schools in rural China aim to integrate educational resources and are more likely to be chosen because of the distance between the student's home and school [26, 27, 52]. Therefore, reverse causality may not be a problem in this study. Second, omitted variables may also pose a problem, given that factors that affect students' cognitive and non-cognitive abilities may exist at multiple levels, and crucial indicators such as genetic factors and parental emotional involvement may be difficult to measure [27, 5355].
To address these problems, we use the standard instrumental variables (IV) approach to identify an exogenous source of variation in one's boarding status. The proportion of boarders of all students in a particular school is used as an instrumental variable for boarding. This strategy is based on the assumption that the proportion of boarders is a strong predictor of one's boarding status, because a higher proportion of boarders within a school indicates a higher likelihood for students to become boarders in that school. We employ a two-stage least squares (2SLS) framework to estimate Eq. (1) and the following first-stage equation.
The first-stage equation:
$$Boardin{g}_{jis}={\mu }_{0}+{\mu }_{1}sc{h}_{boardingjis}+{\mu }_{2}{W}_{jis}+{\mu }_{j}+{\varepsilon }_{jis}$$
(2)
The second stage equation:
$${Y}_{jis}=\alpha +{\beta }_{1}Boardin{g}_{jis}+{\beta }_{2}{W}_{jis}+{\mu }_{j}+{\varepsilon }_{jis}$$
(1)
where \(Boardin{g}_{jis}\) is the proportion of boarders. The definitions of other variables are the same as in Eq. (1).

Result

Distribution of cognitive abilities and non-cognitive abilities

Figure 1 shows the distribution of WISC-IV scores in our sample. The density distribution of WISC-IV scores is right-skewed for both boarding and non-boarding students compared to the norm, indicating a relatively high proportion of students with cognitive delays in our sample. Boarding students exhibit a less right-skewed distribution of WISC-IV scores compared to non-boarding students, suggesting that boarding students have higher WISC-IV scores on average. Fig. 2 shows the density distribution of Raven's IQ scores for the sample students. The estimated IQ scores on Raven's test for both boarding and non-boarding samples are not significantly different from the norm. Moreover, there is no significant difference between the Raven's IQ scores of boarding and non-boarding students. Finally, Fig. 3 illustrates the density distribution of standardized math scores for the sample students, suggesting that there is no significant difference between the boarding and non-boarding students.
Table 3 presents the differences in cognitive and non-cognitive abilities between boarding and non-boarding students. The results indicate that boarders' WISC-IV scores were 2.45 points higher than non-boarders, significant at the 5% level, and boarders' fluid intelligence scores were 0.363 points higher than non-boarder, similarly significant at the 5% level. Boarders also scored higher on the matrix reasoning scale by 0.82 points at the 1% significance level. Additionally, boarders' standardized math scores were statistically significantly higher than non-boarders. In terms of non-cognitive skills, boarders scored higher in extraversion by 0.042, but lower in agreeableness by 0.042 compared to non-boarders.
Table 3
Descriptive analysis of cognitive and non-cognitive abilities: comparisons between different types of students
 
Overall
Nonboarders
Boarders
Nonboarders—Boarders
 
N
Mean
N
Mean
N
Mean
Difference in mean
P-value
Effect size (Cohen’s d)
 
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
(9)
Cognitive ability
 WISC-IV scores
472
88.258
243
87.070
229
89.520
-2.450**
0.015
-0.225
 Fluid Intelligence
472
8.472
243
8.296
229
8.659
-0.363**
0.019
-0.217
 Crystal intelligence
472
7.928
243
7.745
229
8.122
-0.377
0.112
-0.147
 Similarities
472
7.928
243
7.745
229
8.122
-0.377
0.112
-0.147
 Digit span
472
8.117
243
7.984
229
8.258
-0.274
0.236
-0.109
 Coding
472
9.71
243
9.712
229
9.707
0.005
0.984
0.002
 Matrix Reasoning
472
7.591
243
7.193
229
8.013
-0.820***
 < 0.001
-0.372
 Raven's IQ
2503
93.641
1363
93.691
1140
93.582
0.109
0.877
0.006
 Math scores of 2016
5188
.02
2816
-0.004
2372
0.049
-0.053*
0.055
-0.053
Non-cognitive ability
 Extraversion
5166
3.349
2802
3.329
2364
3.372
-0.042***
0.007
-0.076
 Agreeableness
5166
3.739
2802
3.758
2364
3.716
0.042***
0.005
0.078
 Conscientiousness
5166
3.279
2802
3.280
2364
3.277
0.003
0.861
0.005
 Neuroticism
5166
2.913
2802
2.904
2364
2.924
-0.020
0.261
-0.031
 Openness
5166
3.411
2802
3.409
2364
3.413
-0.004
0.815
-0.007
 Grit scores
5166
3.349
2809
3.385
2369
3.380
0.005
0.748
0.009
Source: Author’s survey
Standard deviations in parentheses * p < 0.1. ** p < 0.05. *** p < 0.01
Table 4 verifies the representativeness of the WISC-IV-tested students in the sample. We examine the differences in student characteristics between those who took the test and those who did not. The results indicate no significant differences in individual characteristics, family characteristics, and baseline math scores between the two groups.
Table 4
Descriptive analysis of students who took the WISC-IV test: comparisons between different Characteristics of students
 
Overall
Students who did not take the Wechsler test
Students taking the Wechsler test
Differences
 
N
Mean
N
Mean
N
Mean
Difference in mean
P-value
Effect size (Cohen’s d)
 
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
(9)
Student age
5660
12.917
5188
12.914
472
12.948
-0.034
0.457
-0.036
Student gender
5660
.499
5188
0.499
472
0.504
-0.005
0.829
-0.010
Left-behind child status
5660
.525
5188
0.526
472
0.508
0.018
0.465
0.035
Father's education level
5660
.359
5188
0.359
472
0.360
-0.001
0.965
-0.002
Mather's education level
5660
.213
5188
0.212
472
0.225
-0.013
0.510
-0.032
Family asset value
5660
-.001
5188
0.003
472
-0.044
0.047
0.366
0.043
Math scores of 2015
5660
-.112
5188
-0.110
472
-0.139
0.029
0.529
0.030
Source: Author’s survey
Standard deviations in parentheses * p < 0.1. ** p < 0.05. *** p < 0.01

Impact of boarding on cognitive abilities

Table 5 presents an analysis of the impact of boarding on cognitive abilities among rural students, specifically focusing on WISC IQ scores, fluid intelligence, and crystal intelligence. Using ordinary least squares (OLS) estimates in columns 1, 3, and 5, the results show that boarding does not have a significant effect on students' cognitive abilities. To further examine the causal relationship between boarding and cognitive abilities, two-stage least squares (2SLS) estimates are presented in columns 2, 4, and 6, and the findings indicate that boarding has no significant impact on WISC IQ scores, fluid intelligence, and crystal intelligence. The first stage regression has a high F-statistic of 41.284, indicating the exclusion of weak instrumental variables. To better understand how boarding affects students' cognitive abilities, we also estimated the impacts of boarding on the four subdimensions of WISC IQ scores, which are similarities, digit span, coding, and matrix reasoning. The results presented in Table 6. The 2SLS estimates for boarding on students’ scores in digit span has a parameter estimate of 2.024, significant at the 5% level. Since scores in digit span is a test of attention and memory, the result highlights the positive impact of boarding on students' performance in this particular cognitive dimension.
Table 5
Impact of boarding on students' cognitive abilities
variables
WISC IQ score
WISC IQ score
Fluid intelligence
Fluid intelligence
Crystal intelligence
Crystal intelligence
 
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
 
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
Boarding
0.103
4.781
-0.048
0.635
0.181
0.934
 
(1.040)
(3.592)
(0.179)
(0.573)
(0.266)
(0.925)
Student-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
School-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Teacher-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Constant
139.817***
139.866***
16.501***
16.527***
15.396***
15.365***
 
(8.633)
(8.702)
(1.408)
(1.387)
(2.309)
(2.240)
Observations
472
472
472
472
472
472
R-squared
0.418
0.393
0.371
0.348
0.286
0.286
County fixed effects
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Phase I F-statistic values
 
41.284
 
41.284
 
41.284
Source: Author’s survey
Standard deviations in parentheses * p < 0.1. ** p < 0.05. *** p < 0.01
Table 6
Impact of boarding on subdimensions of cognitive abilities
variables
Similarities
Digit span
Coding
Matrix reasoning
 
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
 
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
Boarding
0.181
0.934
0.278
2.024**
-0.685**
-0.472
0.263
0.352
 
(0.266)
(0.925)
(0.268)
(0.982)
(0.281)
(0.892)
(0.256)
(0.831)
Student-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
School-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Teacher-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Constant
15.396***
15.365***
18.264***
18.195***
19.467***
19.517***
11.771***
11.869***
 
(2.309)
(2.240)
(2.216)
(2.380)
(2.098)
(2.162)
(1.976)
(2.013)
Observations
472
472
472
472
472
472
472
472
R-squared
0.286
0.274
0.201
0.134
0.272
0.271
0.223
0.223
County fixed effects
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Phase I F-statistic values
 
41.284
 
41.284
 
41.284
 
41.284
Source: Author’s survey
Standard deviations in parentheses * p < 0.1. ** p < 0.05. *** p < 0.01

Impact of boarding on students' non-cognitive abilities

Table 7 presents the effects of boarding on the personality traits of rural students, encompassing extraversion, agreeableness, dutifulness, neuroticism, and openness. The OLS results in columns (1), (3), (5), (7), and (9) suggest that while there is a positive relationship between boarding and the extraversion of rural students, the IV results indicate that boarding does not have a statistically significant effect on any of the five personality traits examined. Therefore, we conclude that boarding does not have any significant effects on the non-cognitive abilities of rural students.
Table 7
Impact of boarding on students' non-cognitive abilities
variables
Extraversion
Agreeableness
Conscientiousness
Neuroticism
Openness
 
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
 
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
(9)
(10)
Boarding
0.036*
-0.064
-0.034
0.023
0.009
0.109
-0.001
0.034
0.005
0.097
 
(0.020)
(0.082)
(0.022)
(0.077)
(0.025)
(0.083)
(0.023)
(0.090)
(0.023)
(0.083)
Student-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
School-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Teacher-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Constant
3.541***
3.555***
3.687***
3.707***
3.193***
3.190***
2.753***
2.747***
3.368***
3.367***
 
(0.153)
(0.158)
(0.169)
(0.149)
(0.178)
(0.161)
(0.206)
(0.175)
(0.171)
(0.160)
Observations
5,166
5,166
5,166
5,166
5,166
5,166
5,166
5,166
5,166
5,166
R-squared
0.020
0.015
0.039
0.037
0.016
0.012
0.028
0.028
0.021
0.018
County fixed effects
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Phase I F-statistic values
 
342.982
 
342.982
 
342.982
 
342.982
342.982
342.982
Source: Author’s survey
Standard deviations in parentheses * p < 0.1. ** p < 0.05. *** p < 0.01

Robustness test

To enhance the robustness of the research findings, we conducted additional regression analyses. First, we added the bootstrap method to the original instrumental variables method to re-estimate the impact of boarding on students' cognitive and non-cognitive abilities. The bootstrap method involves treating the observed sample as the entire population, and repeatedly resampling with replacement from the original sample to estimate the sampling distribution. This approach can provide an estimate of the distribution without introducing bias. In this paper, we conducted 1000 bootstrap samples and then used the instrumental variables method for estimation, which can provide more robust standard errors. Tables 8 and 9 show the results, which indicate that boarding still has a significant positive effect at the 10% level on students' scores in digit span and no significant effect on students' non-cognitive abilities, which is consistent with the results above.
Table 8
Robustness test I: bootstrap sampling—cognitive abilities
variables
WISC IQ score
Fluid intelligence
Crystal intelligence
Similarities
Digit span
Coding
Matrix reasoning
 
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
Boarding
4.781
0.635
0.898
0.934
2.024*
-0.472
0.352
 
(3.725)
(0.621)
(0.877)
(1.022)
(1.034)
(0 .990)
(0.890)
Student-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
School-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Teacher-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Constant
139.866***
16.527***
15.373***
15.365***
18.195***
19.517***
11.869***
 
(9.140)
(1.550)
(2.439)
(2.520)
(2.340)
(2.289)
(2.113)
Observations
472
472
472
472
472
472
472
R-squared
0.393
0.348
0.274
0.274
0.133
0.271
0.223
County fixed effects
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Phase I F-statistic values
41.732
41.732
41.732
41.732
41.732
41.732
41.732
Source: Author’s survey
Standard deviations in parentheses * p < 0.1. ** p < 0.05. *** p < 0.01
Table 9
Robustness test I: bootstrap sampling-non-cognitive abilities
variable
Extraversion
Agreeableness
Conscientiousness
Neuroticism
Openness
 
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
Boarding
-0.064
0.023
0.109
0.034
0.097
 
(0.087)
(0.076)
(0.088)
(0.092)
(0.080)
Student-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
School-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Teacher-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Constant
3.555***
3.707***
3.190***
2.747***
3.367***
 
(0.156)
(0.148)
(0.161)
(0.176)
(0.162)
Observations
5,166
5,166
5,166
5,166
5,166
R-squared
0.015
0.037
0.012
0.028
0.018
County fixed effects
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Phase I F-statistic values
342.982
342.982
342.982
342.982
342.982
Source: Author’s survey
Standard deviations in parentheses * p < 0.1. ** p < 0.05. *** p < 0.01
Second, we performed robustness tests using Raven's IQ, standardized math, and grit scores as additional measures of cognitive and non-cognitive abilities. Raven's IQ and standardized math scores are measures of fluid and crystal intelligence, respectively. The results in Table 10 suggest that boarding does not have a significant effect on students' Raven's IQ and standardized math scores, which are consistent with previous findings. Grit is closely related to conscientiousness in the Big Five personality traits, and although the two are not identical, they share strong similarities such as diligence and perseverance [56]. It has also been shown that grit is a more refined measure of conscientiousness [57, 58]. However, columns (5) and (6) of Table 10 show that boarding does not have a significant effect on students' grit scores, which are consistent with the previous results.
Table 10
Robustness test II: impact of boarding on students' cognitive and non-cognitive abilities
variables
Raven’s IQ scores
Standardization
Math scores of 2016
Grit scores
 
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
Boarding
-0.548
1.669
-0.052
0.086
0.003
-0.014
 
(0.855)
(2.933)
(0.039)
(0.110)
(0.021)
(0.083)
Student-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
School-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Teacher-level control variables
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Constant
167.539***
170.819***
2.049***
2.226***
3.146***
3.209***
 
(7.639)
(5.789)
(0.330)
(0.214)
(0.153)
(0.161)
Observations
2,503
2,503
5,188
5,188
5,188
5,188
R-squared
0.337
0.335
0.426
0.423
0.426
0.423
County fixed effects
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
YES
Phase I F-statistic values
 
172.725
 
348.218
 
349.966
Source: Author’s survey
Standard deviations in parentheses * p < 0.1. ** p < 0.05. *** p < 0.01

Heterogeneity analysis

In addition to analyzing the effects of boarding, we investigate the heterogeneity of these effects along three dimensions: gender, whether the student is a left-behind child, and family asset status. Family asset status is defined by ranking students' family asset indices from smallest to largest, and students in the top 25% of the sample are classified as having bad family conditions, with a dummy variable indicating whether a student's family conditions are bad. Table 11 presents the results of the heterogeneity analysis for cognitive abilities. The estimates in Panels A and B show that boarding has a significant positive effect on boys' WISC IQ scores, particularly in fluid intelligence, as evidenced by improvements in digit span and matrix reasoning. These findings suggest that boarding enhances boys' memory, attention, and reasoning abilities. Panels C and D indicate that boarding also has a significant positive impact on left-behind children's WISC IQ scores, again largely reflected in fluid intelligence. Boarding increases the digit span score (memory and attention) of left-behind children by 2.952 points (p < 0.05). However, the coding score for non-left-behind children is negatively affected, indicating that boarding reduces their transcription speed, accuracy, general learning ability, and anti-distraction ability. Panels E and F demonstrate that boarding has a significant positive effect at the 5% level on the digit span scores (memory and attention) of students with good family conditions, while there is no significant effect on students' cognitive abilities from low-income families.
Table 11
Results of the heterogeneity analysis of the impact of boarding on students' cognitive abilities
variables
WISC IQ score
Fluid intelligence
Crystal intelligence
Similarities
Digit span
Coding
Matrix reasoning
 
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
 
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
(9)
(10)
(11)
(12)
(13)
(14)
Panel A: Sample of male students
 Boarding
0.448
9.128**
0.132
1.494**
-0.205
0.998
-0.205
0.998
0.346
2.756**
-0.604
0.295
0.654*
1.432*
 
(1.418)
(3.991)
(0.250)
(0.609)
(0.362)
(1.046)
(0.362)
(1.046)
(0.422)
(1.110)
(0.399)
(0.918)
(0.378)
(0.867)
 Observations
238
238
238
238
238
238
238
238
238
238
238
238
238
238
 Phase I F-statistic values
 
36.872
 
36.872
 
36.872
 
36.872
 
36.872
 
36.872
 
36.872
Panel B: Sample of female students
 Boarding
-0.584
-1.371
-0.336
-0.275
0.512
0.167
0.512
0.167
-0.102
1.284
-0.674
-0.456
-0.231
-1.652
 
(1.764)
(7.339)
(0.299)
(1.193)
(0.404)
(1.796)
(0.404)
(1.796)
(0.427)
(1.934)
(0.431)
(1.831)
(0.402)
(1.771)
 Observations
234
234
234
234
234
234
234
234
234
234
234
234
234
234
 Phase I F-statistic values
 
9.784
 
9.784
 
9.784
 
9.784
 
9.784
 
9.784
 
9.784
Panel C: Sample of left-behind children
 Boarding
-0.140
9.751*
0.116
1.652**
-0.458
1.006
-0.458
1.006
0.671
2.952**
-0.008
1.855
-0.315
0.147
 
(1.653)
(5.256)
(0.266)
(0.828)
(0.438)
(1.344)
(0.438)
(1.344)
(0.425)
(1.385)
(0.434)
(1.255)
(0.354)
(1.159)
 Observations
240
240
240
240
240
240
240
240
240
240
240
240
240
240
 Phase I F-statistic values
 
21.891
 
21.891
 
21.891
 
21.891
 
21.891
 
21.891
 
21.891
Panel D: Sample of non-left-behind children
 Boarding
1.909
-1.566
0.045
-0.770
0.925***
1.363
0.925***
1.363
0.079
1.470
-0.623
-3.446**
0.680*
-0.336
 
(1.305)
(5.544)
(0.243)
(0.913)
(0.338)
(1.436)
(0.338)
(1.436)
(0.389)
(1.535)
(0.383)
(1.551)
(0.353)
(1.311)
 Observations
232
232
232
232
232
232
232
232
232
232
232
232
232
232
 Phase I F-statistic values
 
15.299
 
15.299
 
15.299
 
15.299
 
15.299
 
15.299
 
15.299
Panel E: Sample with poor family conditions
 Boarding
-1.441
6.109
-0.280
0.809
-0.012
1.311
-0.012
1.311
-0.255
1.519
-1.246**
0.064
0.661
0.842
 
(1.942)
(5.490)
(0.311)
(0.863)
(0.465)
(1.329)
(0.465)
(1.329)
(0.527)
(1.543)
(0.587)
(1.327)
(0.596)
(1.288)
 Observations
144
144
144
144
144
144
144
144
144
144
144
144
144
144
 Phase I F-statistic values
 
15.818
 
15.818
 
15.818
 
15.818
 
15.818
 
15.818
 
15.818
Panel F: Sample with good family conditions
 Boarding
0.940
7.132
0.085
1.257
0.283
0.630
0.283
0.630
0.530
2.884**
-0.385
0.197
0.111
0.690
 
(1.365)
(5.126)
(0.233)
(0.831)
(0.324)
(1.304)
(0.324)
(1.304)
(0.366)
(1.402)
(0.346)
(1.282)
(0.318)
(1.174)
 Observations
328
328
328
328
328
328
328
328
328
328
328
328
328
328
 Phase I F-statistic values
 
19.995
 
19.995
 
19.995
 
19.995
 
19.995
 
19.995
 
19.995
Source: Author’s survey
Standard deviations in parentheses * p < 0.1. ** p < 0.05. *** p < 0.01
The heterogeneity analysis of non-cognitive abilities is presented in Table 12. Panels A and B show gender differences in the effect of boarding on students' non-cognitive abilities. Specifically, there is a significant positive effect of boarding on girls' conscientiousness at the 10% statistical level, indicating that boarding enhances girls' abilities in areas such as responsibility, workability, and self-control. However, no significant differences were found in the effect of boarding on non-cognitive abilities in other aspects. These findings suggest that boarding schools have varying effects on different dimensions of students' cognitive and non-cognitive abilities, highlighting the importance of considering heterogeneity in understanding the overall impact of boarding school education.
Table 12
Results of the heterogeneity analysis of the effect of boarding on students' non-cognitive abilities
Variables
Extraversion
Agreeableness
Conscientiousness
Neuroticism
Openness
 
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
OLS
IV
 
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
(9)
(10)
Panel A: Sample of male students
 Boarding
0.002
-0.113
-0.060*
-0.099
0.025
0.006
0.023
0.053
0.031
0.101
 
(0.027)
(0.111)
(0.031)
(0.112)
(0.036)
(0.117)
(0.032)
(0.125)
(0.032)
(0.115)
 Observations
2,578
2,578
2,578
2,578
2,578
2,578
2,578
2,578
2,578
2,578
 Phase I F-statistic values
 
169.575
 
169.575
 
169.575
 
169.575
 
169.575
Panel B: Sample of female students
 Boarding
0.074**
0.006
-0.014
0.130
-0.016
0.222*
-0.027
0.002
-0.029
0.112
 
(0.033)
(0.119)
(0.028)
(0.106)
(0.031)
(0.118)
(0.033)
(0.130)
(0.030)
(0.118)
 Observations
2,588
2,588
2,588
2,588
2,588
2,588
2,588
2,588
2,588
2,588
 Phase I F-statistic values
 
176.668
 
176.668
 
176.668
 
176.668
 
176.668
Panel C: Sample of left-behind children
 Boarding
0.032
-0.099
-0.025
0.003
0.033
0.121
0.000
0.079
0.023
0.035
 
(0.025)
(0.126)
(0.027)
(0.115)
(0.033)
(0.126)
(0.035)
(0.136)
(0.030)
(0.123)
 Observations
2,715
2,715
2,715
2,715
2,715
2,715
2,715
2,715
2,715
2,715
 Phase I F-statistic values
 
142.219
 
142.219
 
142.219
 
142.219
 
142.219
Panel D: Sample of non-left-behind children
 Boarding
0.037
-0.084
-0.036
0.050
-0.022
0.052
-0.020
0.008
-0.009
0.179
 
(0.032)
(0.117)
(0.032)
(0.115)
(0.035)
(0.121)
(0.033)
(0.133)
(0.034)
(0.123)
 Observations
2,451
2,451
2,451
2,451
2,451
2,451
2,451
2,451
2,451
2,451
 Phase I F-statistic values
 
172.425
 
172.425
 
172.425
 
172.425
 
172.425
Panel E: Sample with poor family conditions
 Boarding
0.025
-0.157
-0.039
-0.056
0.019
0.173
0.006
0.129
-0.007
0.135
 
(0.034)
(0.155)
(0.041)
(0.146)
(0.042)
(0.159)
(0.038)
(0.176)
(0.038)
(0.164)
 Observations
1,399
1,399
1,399
1,399
1,399
1,399
1,399
1,399
1,399
1,399
 Phase I F-statistic values
 
84.057
 
84.057
 
84.057
 
84.057
 
84.057
Panel F: Sample with good family conditions
 Boarding
0.044*
-0.016
-0.030
0.065
0.003
0.081
-0.005
-0.002
0.005
0.063
 
(0.025)
(0.103)
(0.025)
(0.097)
(0.028)
(0.104)
(0.028)
(0.113)
(0.025)
(0.102)
 Observations
3,767
3,767
3,767
3,767
3,767
3,767
3,767
3,767
3,767
3,767
 Phase I F-statistic values
 
225.648
 
225.648
 
225.648
 
225.648
 
225.648
Source: Author’s survey
Standard deviations in parentheses * p < 0.1. ** p < 0.05. *** p < 0.01

Conclusion and discussion

This study examines the impact of boarding on the cognitive and non-cognitive abilities of 5,660 junior high school students from 160 schools in rural northwest China using an instrumental variable (IV) approach. Our findings suggest that boarding has a significant positive effect on the digit span scores of junior high school students, which is a key component of the Working Memory Index in WISC, and suggests that boarding improves students’ memory and attention. However, we did not find any significant effects on other aspects of cognitive ability, such as logical thinking, reasoning, and transcription speed and accuracy. Additionally, we found no effects on non-cognitive abilities. To ensure the robustness of our findings, we conducted bootstrap and alternative variable tests, which supported our main results.
We used comprehensive indicators of cognitive abilities, namely crystal and fluid intelligence, to investigate the impact of boarding on rural students' cognitive abilities. Our analysis shows that boarding has a significant impact only on the ability of students to perform digit-span tasks, which is an important component of the Working Memory Index in the WISC. This finding suggests that boarding can positively affect students' memory and attention skills. One possible explanation for this result is that intensive and continuous learning or training can enhance an individual's cognitive abilities [5961]. Moreover, boarders have a more collaborative learning and living environment in comparison to commuters, and they spend more time on general studies and homework [29]. Thus, boarding can significantly improve students' cognitive abilities. Furthermore, previous research has shown that the breadth of children's working memory increases linearly between the ages of 4 and 15 [62]. As boarders spend more time at school than commuters, they may have more opportunities to exercise their cognitive abilities, particularly in the area of attention and memory. Therefore, the positive effect of boarding on their cognitive abilities is likely to be reflected in their performance on digit span tasks. Most previous studies on the relationship between boarding and students' cognitive ability have often used academic performance (such as grades in math, language, and reading) as a proxy variable for cognitive ability. The conclusions drawn from these studies have been mixed. For instance, some studies suggest a positive effect of boarding on students' math performance in elementary school [19, 23], while others demonstrate a significant negative influence on reading scores [26, 28]. On the other hand, boarding has been linked to a positive effect on math and language scores among junior high school students [29, 63]. Nonetheless, academic performance merely measures a certain dimension of students' cognitive ability, often reflecting crystallized intelligence and not offering a comprehensive evaluation of cognitive ability. From this perspective, this study employs the Wechsler test to more precisely gauge the impact of boarding on students' cognitive ability. In the existing literature, China Education Tracking Survey (CEPS) data is the only source that gauged students' cognitive ability through a unified scale (but not the Wechsler and Raven tests utilized in this study). Their findings indicated that boarding does not significantly impact students' cognitive abilities [63], which broadly aligns with the results of our study.
Moreover, regarding boarding and noncognitive ability, the current literature has primarily focused on mental health indicators as measures of noncognitive ability (e.g., depression, bullying, etc.). For instance, studies have pointed to potential psychological risks associated with boarding for students [27, 28, 6466]. However, the Big Five personality traits measurement is an internationally recognized and widely employed tool for non-cognitive abilities. Notably, there is a scarcity of literature investigates the impact of boarding on students' non-cognitive skills within the framework of these five dimensions.
We conducted further analysis on the heterogeneity of the effects of boarding on the cognitive and non-cognitive abilities of rural junior high school students by gender, stay-at-home status, and family conditions. Our results indicate that the effect of boarding on the cognitive abilities of boys is greater than that of girls, particularly in fluid intelligence, as measured by digit span and matrix reasoning scales that show improved memory, attention, and reasoning abilities. This could be due to differences in time management skills between boys and girls, with boys benefitting from the external discipline and communal learning atmosphere in boarding schools [67]. For left-behind students, boarding has a greater impact on their cognitive abilities, particularly in the areas of memory and attention, as they lack parental engagement and receive more support from teachers [13]. Additionally, boarding can have a positive impact on the cognitive abilities of students from better-off families due to improved nutritional intake in school, which is associated with better cognitive development [68]. Existing research often uses academic performance as a proxy for cognitive abilities, which may not accurately capture the full range of cognitive skills. Furthermore, the positive effects of boarding on non-cognitive abilities were not observed in our study, suggesting the need for further investigation into the impact of boarding on non-cognitive development.
Our analysis of the heterogeneous effects of boarding on students' cognitive abilities also extends to their non-cognitive abilities. We find that boarding has a more significant impact on girls' conscientiousness of the Big Five personality traits, which is consistent with previous research on gender differences in conscientiousness [69]. This may be attributed to female personality traits and the role of gender as a moderating variable affecting individual commitment [70]. Social role theory also suggests that individuals of different genders develop different senses of group identity, causing them to behave differently in different social situations [71]. Therefore, greater commitment by girls to group characteristics may explain why they are more likely to be influenced by the communal learning atmosphere and external discipline that boarding schools provide. Interestingly, we did not find a significant effect of boarding on non-cognitive abilities among left-behind children or those from different family backgrounds. This finding supports our main estimation results and suggests that boarding may not have a substantial impact on the non-cognitive abilities of rural middle school students. Furthermore, we ruled out the possibility of positive and negative effects of boarding in different subsamples cancelling each other out, which strengthens this conclusion.
In conclusion, our study provides evidence that boarding schools is not detrimental to the development of new human capital, including cognitive and non-cognitive abilities, among rural junior high school students. Moreover, boarding positively affects some dimensions of students' cognitive abilities, particularly in the areas of memory and attention, and has a greater effect on certain subgroups such as boys, left-behind children, and students from better-off families. The growing prevalence of boarding schools in rural areas underscores the need to explore ways to further enhance students' cognitive skills and foster the development of non-cognitive or socio-emotional abilities in their daily boarding life. This issue deserves ongoing attention and efforts from educators and policymakers.
We do acknowledge one limitation of this study. While our analysis and findings indicate that boarding has not significantly impacted the cognitive and non-cognitive abilities of rural junior high school students, it remains possible that potential adverse effects are still present. It’s worth noting that the cognitive and non-cognitive skills evaluated in this paper using measurement scales may not fully encompass the nuances of various students’ behaviors tied to boarding. For instance, aspects like the dynamics between boarders and roommates, as well as the prolonged separation of boarders from parents, could potentially exert negative influences on their non-cognitive abilities, including interpersonal skills and emotional well-being [20, 21, 63, 72]. This points to both the limitation of our current study and the necessity for further research.

Acknowledgements

We thank the researchers from Center for Experimental Economics in Education, Shaanxi Normal University for help on developing the study protocol and training the enumerators. We are also grateful to all respondents who participated in this study and the enumerators for data collection efforts.

Declarations

The Stanford University Institutional Review Board (IRB Protocol ID #19748) approved this study. Permission was received from local Boards of Education in each region and the principals of all schools. Written informed consent was obtained from at least one parent and/or legal guardian for all child participants.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Fußnoten
1
WISC-IV is a clinical instrument created by David Wechsler in the United States. It is utilized to individually evaluate the intelligence of children between the aged 6 and 16 years and is internationally recognized as one of the most authoritative and valid personal intelligence tests. It was published in 1949 and updated to the fourth edition (WISC-IV) in 2003. In 2007, Prof. Houcan Zhang of the School of Psychology at Beijing Normal University, introduced the fourth edition to China. Leveraging Chinese census data, 1100 people were proportionally sampled from 12 provinces in mainland China, with 100 people in each age group, to establish the Chinese version of the norm (with a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15). Resulting in a calculated mean of 88.258 and a standard deviation of 10.948 for the students in our study’s samples. The average reliability coefficients of each synthetic score of the scale ranged from 0.87 to 0.97, meeting the standards set by the original American version. For this paper, the results of the Wechsler test were adjusted according to the Chinese norm [38].
 
2
Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices is designed by the British psychologist Raven in 1938 designed for people over age of 5.5 with normal intellectual development. The norms used in this paper are from the 1989 version (a sample of 5,108 students aged between 5 and 70 underwent testing to determine China’s Raven’s norm for the Raven test, with a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15) [41], as no updated norms have been established specifically for the Chinese population. Related studies in China have also used this test [4244]. The split-half and retest reliabilities of the Raven test were 0.95 (p < 0.1) for the single-sample test and 0.82 (p < 0.1) for the retest. Concurrent validity was 0.71 (P < 0.1), and predictive validity was 0.45 (P < 0.1).
 
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Heckman JJ, Stixrud J, Urzua S. The effects of cognitive and non-cognitive abilities on labor market outcomes and social behavior. J Law Econ. 2006;24:411–82. Heckman JJ, Stixrud J, Urzua S. The effects of cognitive and non-cognitive abilities on labor market outcomes and social behavior. J Law Econ. 2006;24:411–82.
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Farkas G. Cognitive skills and non-cognitive traits and behaviors in stratification processes. Ann Rev Sociol. 2003;29:541–62.CrossRef Farkas G. Cognitive skills and non-cognitive traits and behaviors in stratification processes. Ann Rev Sociol. 2003;29:541–62.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Durlak JA, Weissberg RP, Dymnicki AB, Taylor RD, Schellinger KB. The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: A meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Dev. 2011;82:405–32.PubMedCrossRef Durlak JA, Weissberg RP, Dymnicki AB, Taylor RD, Schellinger KB. The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: A meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Dev. 2011;82:405–32.PubMedCrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Huang G, Xie Y, Xu H. Cognitive ability: Social correlates and consequences in contemporary China. Chinese Sociolog Rev. 2015;47:287–313.CrossRef Huang G, Xie Y, Xu H. Cognitive ability: Social correlates and consequences in contemporary China. Chinese Sociolog Rev. 2015;47:287–313.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Autor DH. Skills, education, and the rise of earnings inequality among the “other 99 percent.” Scie. 2014;344:843–51. Autor DH. Skills, education, and the rise of earnings inequality among the “other 99 percent.” Scie. 2014;344:843–51.
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Bowles S, Gintis H. Schooling in Capitalist America Revisited. Sociol Educ. 2002;75:1–18.CrossRef Bowles S, Gintis H. Schooling in Capitalist America Revisited. Sociol Educ. 2002;75:1–18.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Lleras Borghans L, Duckworth AL, Heckman JJ, ter Weel B. The economics and psychology of personality traits. J Human Res. 2008;43:972–1059. Lleras Borghans L, Duckworth AL, Heckman JJ, ter Weel B. The economics and psychology of personality traits. J Human Res. 2008;43:972–1059.
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang D, Li X, Xue J. Education inequality between rural and urban areas of the People’s Republic of China, migrants’ children education, and some implications. Asian Dev Rev. 2015;32:196–224.CrossRef Zhang D, Li X, Xue J. Education inequality between rural and urban areas of the People’s Republic of China, migrants’ children education, and some implications. Asian Dev Rev. 2015;32:196–224.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Xu H, Xie Y. The causal effects of rural-to-urban migration on children’s well-being in China. Eur Sociol Rev. 2015;31:502–19.CrossRef Xu H, Xie Y. The causal effects of rural-to-urban migration on children’s well-being in China. Eur Sociol Rev. 2015;31:502–19.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin AJ, Papworth B, Ginns P. Liem GA Boarding school, academic motivation and engagement, and psychological well-being: a large-scale investigation. Am Educ Res J. 2014;51:1007–49.CrossRef Martin AJ, Papworth B, Ginns P. Liem GA Boarding school, academic motivation and engagement, and psychological well-being: a large-scale investigation. Am Educ Res J. 2014;51:1007–49.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Behaghel L, de Chaisemartin C, Gurgand M. Ready for boarding? The effects of a boarding school for disadvantaged students. Am Econ J Appl Econ. 2017;9:140–64.CrossRef Behaghel L, de Chaisemartin C, Gurgand M. Ready for boarding? The effects of a boarding school for disadvantaged students. Am Econ J Appl Econ. 2017;9:140–64.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Ak L, Sayil M. Three different types of elementary school students’ school achievements, are perceived social support, school attitudes, and behavior-adjustment problems. Kuram ve Uygulamada Egitim Bilimleri. 2006;6:293. Ak L, Sayil M. Three different types of elementary school students’ school achievements, are perceived social support, school attitudes, and behavior-adjustment problems. Kuram ve Uygulamada Egitim Bilimleri. 2006;6:293.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang X, Li Y, Zhang Q, Lu F, Wang Y. Smoking and its risk factors in Chinese elementary and middle school students: A nationally representative sample study. Addict Behav. 2014;39:837–41.PubMedCrossRef Zhang X, Li Y, Zhang Q, Lu F, Wang Y. Smoking and its risk factors in Chinese elementary and middle school students: A nationally representative sample study. Addict Behav. 2014;39:837–41.PubMedCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Granot D, Mayseless O. Attachment security and adjustment to school in middle childhood. Int J Behav Dev. 2001;25:530–41.CrossRef Granot D, Mayseless O. Attachment security and adjustment to school in middle childhood. Int J Behav Dev. 2001;25:530–41.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Steel A, Erhardt R, Phelps R, Upham P. Estimates of enhanced outcomes in employment, income, health, and volunteerism for the association of boarding schools member school graduates. J Adv Acad. 2015;26:227–45. Steel A, Erhardt R, Phelps R, Upham P. Estimates of enhanced outcomes in employment, income, health, and volunteerism for the association of boarding schools member school graduates. J Adv Acad. 2015;26:227–45.
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Curto VE, Fryer RG. The potential of urban boarding schools for the poor: evidence from SEED. J Law Econ. 2014;32:65–93. Curto VE, Fryer RG. The potential of urban boarding schools for the poor: evidence from SEED. J Law Econ. 2014;32:65–93.
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Evans-Campbell T, Walters KL, Pearson CR, Campbell CD. Indian boarding school experience, substance use, and mental health among urban Two-Spirit American Indian/Alaska Natives. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2012;38:421–7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Evans-Campbell T, Walters KL, Pearson CR, Campbell CD. Indian boarding school experience, substance use, and mental health among urban Two-Spirit American Indian/Alaska Natives. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2012;38:421–7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Pfeiffer JP, Pinquart M. Bullying in German boarding schools: a pilot study. Sch Psychol Int. 2014;35:580–91.CrossRef Pfeiffer JP, Pinquart M. Bullying in German boarding schools: a pilot study. Sch Psychol Int. 2014;35:580–91.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Mander DJ, Lester L, Cross D. The social and emotional well-being and mental health implications for adolescents transitioning to secondary boarding school. Int J Child Adolesc Health. 2015;8:131. Mander DJ, Lester L, Cross D. The social and emotional well-being and mental health implications for adolescents transitioning to secondary boarding school. Int J Child Adolesc Health. 2015;8:131.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Qiao T, Di L. The causal inference of boarding’s effect in rural primary and secondary school education. J Soc Develop. 2014;1:138–52+245 (in Chinese). Qiao T, Di L. The causal inference of boarding’s effect in rural primary and secondary school education. J Soc Develop. 2014;1:138–52+245 (in Chinese).
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Mo D, Yi H, Zhang L, Shi Y, Rozelle S, Medina A. Transfer paths and academic performance: the primary school merger program in China. Int J Educ Dev. 2012;32:423–31.CrossRef Mo D, Yi H, Zhang L, Shi Y, Rozelle S, Medina A. Transfer paths and academic performance: the primary school merger program in China. Int J Educ Dev. 2012;32:423–31.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang A, Medina A, Luo R, Shi Y, Yue A. To board or not to board: Evidence from nutrition, health and education outcomes of students in rural China. Chin World Econ. 2016;24:52–66.CrossRef Wang A, Medina A, Luo R, Shi Y, Yue A. To board or not to board: Evidence from nutrition, health and education outcomes of students in rural China. Chin World Econ. 2016;24:52–66.CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Li X, Zhu Z, Song Y, Wu Y. The impact of boarding on the reading ability of rural children in poor areas in China: Evidence from 137 boarding schools in Hebei and Sichuan provinces. China Rural Surv. 2018;2:129–44. Li X, Zhu Z, Song Y, Wu Y. The impact of boarding on the reading ability of rural children in poor areas in China: Evidence from 137 boarding schools in Hebei and Sichuan provinces. China Rural Surv. 2018;2:129–44.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhu Z, Li Y, Song Y. Boarding education and children’s development: evidence from 137 rural boarding schools. Educ Res. 2019;8:79–91 (in Chinese). Zhu Z, Li Y, Song Y. Boarding education and children’s development: evidence from 137 rural boarding schools. Educ Res. 2019;8:79–91 (in Chinese).
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Hou H, Wu Y, Song Y. Low-age boarding and rural pupils’ human capital accumulation: evidence from the “Primary School Merger Program.” Chin Rural Econ. 2018;7:113–29. Hou H, Wu Y, Song Y. Low-age boarding and rural pupils’ human capital accumulation: evidence from the “Primary School Merger Program.” Chin Rural Econ. 2018;7:113–29.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Yao S, Gao Y. Can large scale construction of boarding schools promote the development of students in rural area better. Educ Econ. 2018;34:53–60 (in Chinese). Yao S, Gao Y. Can large scale construction of boarding schools promote the development of students in rural area better. Educ Econ. 2018;34:53–60 (in Chinese).
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Tang B, Wang Y, Gao Y, Wu S, Li H, Chen Y, et al. The effect of boarding on the mental health of primary school students in Western Rural China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17:8200.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Tang B, Wang Y, Gao Y, Wu S, Li H, Chen Y, et al. The effect of boarding on the mental health of primary school students in Western Rural China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17:8200.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Nisbett RE, Aronson J, Blair C, Dickens W, Flynn J, Halpern DF, et al. Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments. Am Psychol. 2012;67:130.PubMedCrossRef Nisbett RE, Aronson J, Blair C, Dickens W, Flynn J, Halpern DF, et al. Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments. Am Psychol. 2012;67:130.PubMedCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Kautz T, Heckman JJ, Diris R, Weel B, Borghans L. December. Fostering and Measuring Skills: Improving Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Skills to Promote Lifetime Success. National Bureau of Economic Research.2014. https://doi.org/10.3386/w20749. Kautz T, Heckman JJ, Diris R, Weel B, Borghans L. December. Fostering and Measuring Skills: Improving Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Skills to Promote Lifetime Success. National Bureau of Economic Research.2014. https://​doi.​org/​10.​3386/​w20749.
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Roberts RD, Goff GN, Anjoul F, Kyllonen PC, Pallier G, Stankov L. The armed services vocational aptitude battery (ASVAB): Little more than acculturated learning (Gc)!? Learn Individ Differ. 2000;12:81–103.CrossRef Roberts RD, Goff GN, Anjoul F, Kyllonen PC, Pallier G, Stankov L. The armed services vocational aptitude battery (ASVAB): Little more than acculturated learning (Gc)!? Learn Individ Differ. 2000;12:81–103.CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Raven J, Raven JC, Court J. Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment; 1988. Raven J, Raven JC, Court J. Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment; 1988.
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Costa PT, McCrae RR. Four ways five factors are basic. Personality Individ Differ. 1992;13:653–65.CrossRef Costa PT, McCrae RR. Four ways five factors are basic. Personality Individ Differ. 1992;13:653–65.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Cattell RB. Intelligence: Its structure, growth and action. Oxford, England: North-Holland; 1987. Cattell RB. Intelligence: Its structure, growth and action. Oxford, England: North-Holland; 1987.
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children—4th edition (WISC-IV®). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2003. Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children—4th edition (WISC-IV®). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2003.
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang H. The revision of WISC-IV Chinese version. Psychol Sci. 2009;32:1177–9 (in Chinese). Zhang H. The revision of WISC-IV Chinese version. Psychol Sci. 2009;32:1177–9 (in Chinese).
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Keith TZ, Fine JG, Taub GE, Reynolds MR, Kranzler JH. Higher order, multisample, confirmatory factor analysis of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth Edition: What does it measure. Sch Psychol Rev. 2006;35:108–27.CrossRef Keith TZ, Fine JG, Taub GE, Reynolds MR, Kranzler JH. Higher order, multisample, confirmatory factor analysis of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth Edition: What does it measure. Sch Psychol Rev. 2006;35:108–27.CrossRef
40.
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang H, Wang X. Standardization research on Raven’s standard progressive matrices in China. Acta Psychologica Sinica. 1989;2:113–21 (in Chinese). Zhang H, Wang X. Standardization research on Raven’s standard progressive matrices in China. Acta Psychologica Sinica. 1989;2:113–21 (in Chinese).
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Fan S, Liu H. Analysis of potential types of responses in raven advanced reasoning test. Psycholog Explor. 2016;36:257–63 (in Chinese). Fan S, Liu H. Analysis of potential types of responses in raven advanced reasoning test. Psycholog Explor. 2016;36:257–63 (in Chinese).
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Li C, Luo X, Wang T, Xu F. Gender differences in Raven’s reasoning test. Adv Psychol Sci. 2011;19:1076–82 (in Chinese). Li C, Luo X, Wang T, Xu F. Gender differences in Raven’s reasoning test. Adv Psychol Sci. 2011;19:1076–82 (in Chinese).
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Li H, Zheng Y. Analysis of combined raven testing and intervention effects on children with learning difficulties. Western Med. 2009;21:2129–30 (in Chinese). Li H, Zheng Y. Analysis of combined raven testing and intervention effects on children with learning difficulties. Western Med. 2009;21:2129–30 (in Chinese).
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Patrick CL. Student evaluations of teaching: effects of the Big Five personality traits, grades and the validity hypothesis. Assess Eval High Educ. 2011;36:239–49.CrossRef Patrick CL. Student evaluations of teaching: effects of the Big Five personality traits, grades and the validity hypothesis. Assess Eval High Educ. 2011;36:239–49.CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat John OP, Naumann LP, Soto CJ. Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and conceptual issues. The Guilford Press; 2008. John OP, Naumann LP, Soto CJ. Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and conceptual issues. The Guilford Press; 2008.
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang M, Dai X, Yao SQ. Development of Chinese Big Five Personality Inventory (CBF-PI)—Theoretical framework and relability analysis. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2010;18:545–8. Wang M, Dai X, Yao SQ. Development of Chinese Big Five Personality Inventory (CBF-PI)—Theoretical framework and relability analysis. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2010;18:545–8.
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Duckworth AL, Peterson C, Matthews MD, Kelly DR. Grit: Perseverance and passion for long-term goals. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2007;92:1087–101.PubMedCrossRef Duckworth AL, Peterson C, Matthews MD, Kelly DR. Grit: Perseverance and passion for long-term goals. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2007;92:1087–101.PubMedCrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Harms PD. Grit: the power of passion and perseverance. Pers Psychol. 2016;69:1021–4.CrossRef Harms PD. Grit: the power of passion and perseverance. Pers Psychol. 2016;69:1021–4.CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Duckworth AL, Quinn PD. Development and validation of the short grit scale (Grit–S). J Pers Assess. 2009;91:166–74.PubMedCrossRef Duckworth AL, Quinn PD. Development and validation of the short grit scale (Grit–S). J Pers Assess. 2009;91:166–74.PubMedCrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen Q, Chen Y, Zhao Q. Impacts of boarding on primary school students’ mental health outcomes – Instrumental-Variable evidence from rural northwestern China. Econ Hum Biol. 2020;39:100920.PubMedCrossRef Chen Q, Chen Y, Zhao Q. Impacts of boarding on primary school students’ mental health outcomes – Instrumental-Variable evidence from rural northwestern China. Econ Hum Biol. 2020;39:100920.PubMedCrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Jensen AR. Raising the IQ: the Ramey and Haskins study. Intelligence. 1981;5:29–40.CrossRef Jensen AR. Raising the IQ: the Ramey and Haskins study. Intelligence. 1981;5:29–40.CrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Rimfeld K, Kovas Y, Dale PS, Plomin R. True grit and genetics: predicting academic achievement from personality. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2016;2016(111):780.CrossRef Rimfeld K, Kovas Y, Dale PS, Plomin R. True grit and genetics: predicting academic achievement from personality. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2016;2016(111):780.CrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat MacCann C, Duckworth AL, Roberts RD. Empirical identification of the major facets of conscientiousness. Learn Individ Differ. 2009;19:451–8.CrossRef MacCann C, Duckworth AL, Roberts RD. Empirical identification of the major facets of conscientiousness. Learn Individ Differ. 2009;19:451–8.CrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Eisenberg N, Duckworth AL, Spinrad TL, Valiente C. Conscientiousness: Origins in childhood? Dev Psychol. 2014;50:1331–49.PubMedCrossRef Eisenberg N, Duckworth AL, Spinrad TL, Valiente C. Conscientiousness: Origins in childhood? Dev Psychol. 2014;50:1331–49.PubMedCrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Ceci SJ, Liker JK. A day at the races: A study of IQ, expertise, and cognitive complexity. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1986;115:255–66.CrossRef Ceci SJ, Liker JK. A day at the races: A study of IQ, expertise, and cognitive complexity. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1986;115:255–66.CrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Jacobson MJ. Problem solving, cognition, and complex systems: differences between experts and novices. Complexity. 2001;6:41–9.CrossRef Jacobson MJ. Problem solving, cognition, and complex systems: differences between experts and novices. Complexity. 2001;6:41–9.CrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ, Ambridge B, Wearing H. The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Dev Psychol. 2004;40:177–90.PubMedCrossRef Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ, Ambridge B, Wearing H. The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Dev Psychol. 2004;40:177–90.PubMedCrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhou J, Xu N. The impact of boarding on student's academic achievement, cognitive ability and non-cognitive ability in junior high school. Educ Sci Res. 2021;5:53–9 (in Chinese). Zhou J, Xu N. The impact of boarding on student's academic achievement, cognitive ability and non-cognitive ability in junior high school. Educ Sci Res. 2021;5:53–9 (in Chinese).
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu W, Hou W. A study on the mental health of junior first-year high school students and its influencing factors ——An analysis based on China’s educational follow-up survey(CEPS). J Shanghai Educ Res. 2018;04:39–43+48 (in Chinese). Liu W, Hou W. A study on the mental health of junior first-year high school students and its influencing factors ——An analysis based on China’s educational follow-up survey(CEPS). J Shanghai Educ Res. 2018;04:39–43+48 (in Chinese).
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Kleinfeld J, Bloom J. Boarding schools: effects on the mental health of Eskimo adolescents. Am J Psychiatry. 1977;134:411–7.PubMedCrossRef Kleinfeld J, Bloom J. Boarding schools: effects on the mental health of Eskimo adolescents. Am J Psychiatry. 1977;134:411–7.PubMedCrossRef
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Shen Z, Chen L, Cui J, et al. Influencing factors of mental health of boarding and non-boarding junior high school students. Educ Meas Eval. 2012;8:38–42 (in Chinese). Shen Z, Chen L, Cui J, et al. Influencing factors of mental health of boarding and non-boarding junior high school students. Educ Meas Eval. 2012;8:38–42 (in Chinese).
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Li B, Yang J. Relations of time management disposition self-efficiency attribution of junior school student to academic achievement. Psychol Expl. 2004;4:67–71 (in Chinese). Li B, Yang J. Relations of time management disposition self-efficiency attribution of junior school student to academic achievement. Psychol Expl. 2004;4:67–71 (in Chinese).
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Fang H, Ma G. Nutrition problems and countermeasures for students in rural boarding schools in China. Chin J School Health. 2010;31:1039–40 (in Chinese). Fang H, Ma G. Nutrition problems and countermeasures for students in rural boarding schools in China. Chin J School Health. 2010;31:1039–40 (in Chinese).
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Kling KC, Noftle EE, Robins RW. Why do standardized tests underpredict women’s academic performance? The role of conscientiousness. Soc Psycholog Personal Scie. 2013;4:600–6.CrossRef Kling KC, Noftle EE, Robins RW. Why do standardized tests underpredict women’s academic performance? The role of conscientiousness. Soc Psycholog Personal Scie. 2013;4:600–6.CrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Vianello M, Robusto E, Anselmi P. Implicit conscientiousness predicts academic performance. Personal Individ Differ. 2010;48:452–7.CrossRef Vianello M, Robusto E, Anselmi P. Implicit conscientiousness predicts academic performance. Personal Individ Differ. 2010;48:452–7.CrossRef
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Eagly AH, Wood W. Explaining sex differences in social behavior: a meta-analytic perspective. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 1991;17:306–15.CrossRef Eagly AH, Wood W. Explaining sex differences in social behavior: a meta-analytic perspective. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 1991;17:306–15.CrossRef
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang S, Mao Y. The impact of boarding on social-emotional competence of left-behind children: an empirical study in 11 provinces and autonomous region in western China. J Educ Studies. 2015;11:111–20 (in Chinese). Wang S, Mao Y. The impact of boarding on social-emotional competence of left-behind children: an empirical study in 11 provinces and autonomous region in western China. J Educ Studies. 2015;11:111–20 (in Chinese).
Metadaten
Titel
The impact of boarding schools on the development of cognitive and non-cognitive abilities in adolescents
verfasst von
Fang Chang
Yanan Huo
Songyan Zhang
Hang Zeng
Bin Tang
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2023
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Public Health / Ausgabe 1/2023
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2458
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-023-16748-8

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2023

BMC Public Health 1/2023 Zur Ausgabe