Skip to main content
Erschienen in: International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health 8/2019

Open Access 08.07.2019 | Original Article

Working in preschool increases the risk of hearing-related symptoms: a cohort study among Swedish women

Erschienen in: International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health | Ausgabe 8/2019

Abstract

Purpose

To assess whether working in preschools increases the risk of hearing-related symptoms and whether age, occupational noise, and stressful working conditions affect the risk.

Methods

Questionnaire data on hearing-related symptoms were analysed in women aged 24–65 (4718 preschool teachers, and 4122 randomly selected general population controls). Prevalence and risk ratio (RR) of self-reported hearing loss, tinnitus, difficulty perceiving speech, hyperacusis and sound-induced auditory fatigue were assessed by comparing the cohorts in relation to age and self-reported occupational noise and stressful working conditions (effort–reward imbalance and emotional demands). RR was calculated using log-binomial regression models adjusted for age, education, income, smoking, hearing protection, and leisure noise. Incidence rates and incidence rate ratios (IRR) were calculated for retrospectively reported onset of all symptoms except sound-induced auditory fatigue.

Results

Compared to the controls, preschool teachers had overall more than twofold RR of sound-induced auditory fatigue (RR 2.4, 95% confidence interval 2.2–2.5) and hyperacusis (RR 2.3, 2.1–2.5) and almost twofold for difficulty perceiving speech (RR 1.9, 1.7–2.0). Preschool teachers had a threefold IRR of hyperacusis (IRR 3.1, 2.8–3.4) and twofold for difficulty perceiving speech (IRR 2.4, 2.2–2.6). Significantly although slightly less increased RR and IRR were observed for hearing loss and tinnitus. RR and IRR were generally still increased for preschool teachers when stratified by age and occupational exposure to noise and stress.

Conclusions

This large cohort study showed that working as preschool teacher increases the risk of self-reported hearing-related symptoms, indicating a need of preventative measures.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s00420-019-01453-0) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

The majority of research on occupational noise has been performed in traditionally male-dominated and high level-exposure occupations such as industry, mining and construction (Concha-Barrientos et al. 2004; Kurmis and Apps 2007; Lie et al. 2016). Most of these studies have assessed hearing loss measured using pure tone audiometry as the main outcome. Thus, there is a well-recognised causal relationship between occupational noise exposure from machines and tools and the outcome hearing loss (Nelson et al. 2005). In contrast, there is a lack of studies assessing the risks of hearing-related symptoms in traditionally female-dominated occupations, such as preschool teachers. In preschools, the main noise sources are children’s voices, screams and playing activity (Persson Waye et al. 2010; Sjödin et al. 2012; Gerhardsson and Nilsson 2013). The sound environment is highly irregular and intermittent, with equivalent sound levels exceeding 85 dBA in one-minute loggins—up to 100 times per hour (Sjödin et al. 2012). The average sound level indoors in Swedish preschools has been measured close to the lower action level 80 dBA Leq (Persson Waye et al. 2010; Sjödin et al. 2012). The action level is regulated by the Swedish Work Authorities to reduce the risk of noise-induced auditory disorder among employees.
In addition to noise, preschool teachers face several psychosocial stressors at work, such as meeting children’s needs, time pressure and being interrupted (Kelly and Berthelsen 1995). Increased hazard ratios for stress-related disorders have been reported for preschool teachers compared to personnel in non-human service occupations (Wieclaw et al. 2006). Interestingly, a Swedish population-based cross-sectional study has reported an association between occupational stressors and hearing-related symptoms (Hasson et al. 2011). The hypothesis, based mainly on experimental research, is that a stress response may modulate hearing sensitivity on a neuro-endocrine level in two ways. They are (a) through activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis via glucocorticoid receptors in the cochlea (Canlon et al. 2007), and (b) through sympathetic stimulation affecting cochlear blood flow via adrenergic α-receptors within the cochlea (Bielefeld and Henderson 2007). Prolonged exposure to stress without sufficient recovery is thought to cause an abnormal functioning of the stress response (McEwen 2006), which has been hypothesised to cause auditory disorders (Canlon et al. 2013). However, the causal effect between stress exposure and hearing disorder has not been thoroughly studied in humans.
Non-occupational factors may also be associated to hearing-related symptoms. Previous studies have found associations between smoking and hearing loss (Cruickshanks et al. 1998; Ferrite and Santana 2005), and self-reported hearing problems have been found more common in low socioeconomic groups (Hasson et al. 2010).
A smaller cross-sectional study has indicated that preschool personnel have a high prevalence of hearing-related symptoms: 31% prevalence of tinnitus and 45% prevalence of hyperacusis among 101 personnel surveyed (Sjödin et al. 2012). This is considerably higher than the prevalence found in the general Swedish population, which is about 10–15% for tinnitus and about 8–9% for hyperacusis, depending on symptom definition (Axelsson and Ringdahl 1989; Andersson et al. 2002; Hasson et al. 2010; Paulin et al. 2016). To the best of our knowledge, no previous studies have compared hearing-related symptoms among preschool personnel and randomly selected population controls. Thus, risk estimates of hearing-related symptoms are lacking for this occupational group.
The aim of this study was, therefore, to assess whether working in preschools increases the relative risk of hearing-related symptoms and whether age, exposure to occupational noise or stressful working conditions affect the risk.

Methods

Study design and population

This cohort study includes baseline and retrospective data collected by postal questionnaires sent out in 2013 and 2014 to 11232 preschool teachers and 14524 women from the general population in Sweden. The preschool cohort included all individuals with a preschool teachers’ degree issued between 1980 and 2012 from universities in the Västra Götaland County of Sweden. The control cohort, which was randomly selected from the Swedish Population and Tax Agency Register, included women born between 1943 and 1989 and currently residing in Västra Götaland County. The response rate to the questionnaire survey was 51% in the preschool cohort and 38% in the population control cohort. As seen in Fig. 1, the final study sample included women 24–65 years of age (born between 1948 and 1989) and consisted of 4718 preschool teachers who had worked in preschools and 4122 controls who, based on an assessment of free text responses on occupational history, had not reported working in preschools. A sub-analysis was also performed including only women currently working, for whom questionnaire data on exposure to occupational noise and stressful working-conditions was available; the sub-analysis included 4205 preschool teachers and 3250 controls.
About 6 months after the first mailing and two reminders were sent, 10% of non-respondents within each cohort were randomly selected for a shorter non-response survey. In total, 105 non-responders within the preschool cohort and 164 within the control cohort returned the non-response questionnaire.

Outcome and predictor variables

The prevalence of current self-reported symptoms and incidence rates based on retrospectively reported symptom onset were assessed using a questionnaire assessing five hearing-related symptoms as outcomes: self-reported hearing loss, tinnitus, difficulty perceiving speech, hyperacusis and sound-induced auditory fatigue. Self-reported hearing loss was defined by a “yes” response to the question: “Do you have a hearing loss?”. Difficulty perceiving speech was defined by “yes” responses to both work and leisure time for the question: “Do you [at work/in leisure time] have difficulty perceiving speech in an environment where several people are speaking at the same time?”. Tinnitus, hyperacusis and sound-induced auditory fatigue were defined by “sometime each week or more often” responses to the questions: “Do you have tinnitus (a ringing, whizzing or other sound without an external source)?”, “Are you sensitive to sounds (feel discomfort or pain by everyday sounds)?” and “Do you during or after work experience ‘sound fatigue’?”, respectively. Occurrence of symptoms was reported as age and/or year of onset in free text to the question “When did you first notice [the symptom]?”Age of onset was recalculated to calendar year of onset, and onset was excluded if the two measures of occurrence differed > 3 years. The survey did not include onset for sound-induced auditory fatigue. Identical symptom definitions have been validated among women exposed to moderately or high sound levels in obstetrics care (Fredriksson et al. 2016).  In the non-response survey, only self-reporting of hearing loss and tinnitus were included. Tinnitus was assessed using a binary response scale corresponding to the definition of tinnitus detailed above. Self-reported hearing loss was assessed identically as in the larger survey.
Current exposure to occupational noise was assessed with two items: one item reflected whether noise at work was so loud to the extent that conversation was difficult to hear and the other item, whether noise at work was so loud that the person had to raise their voice to communicate. Noise exposure was defined by a report of “about 25% of time” at work or more on one or both of the items. Similar items have been validated in other studies (Neitzel et al. 2009; Schlaefer et al. 2009). Current exposure to stressful working conditions was measured using the effort–reward imbalance (ERI) short version questionnaire, which includes ten items (Siegrist et al. 2004), and the short version of emotional demands from the Copenhagen Psychosocial Questionnaire (COPSOQ), which includes two items assessing experiences of emotionally difficult situations and emotional effects, respectively (Kristensen et al. 2005). For the ERI, one missing value within each dimension was allowed, and replaced by the individual mean from the remaining items in that dimension. A continuous ratio between the effort dimension and the reward dimension was calculated. According to standard praxis, respondents were defined as being exposed to stressful working conditions if they had ERI ratio > 1 (indicating inadequate rewards in relation to the efforts) and/or reported “often” or “always” on both COPSOQ-items. A combined current exposure of noise and stressful working conditions was defined as meeting both the noise and stress definitions, and currently unexposed to noise and stressful working conditions was defined as not meeting any of these definitions of exposure.
We also assessed possible confounding by variables including age, highest attained education level and household income (combined as a proxy for socioeconomic status), smoking (ever smoked daily during at least 1 month), use of hearing protection at work and leisure noise exposure. Leisure noise was assessed as a continuous variable using an index calculated as a sum score from items measuring noisy leisure activities, use of hearing protection in leisure time and listening to music with headphones. A similar index was used in a previous study (Fredriksson et al. 2015). Other potential risk factors for hearing-related disorder, such as a family history of hearing loss, were also collected.
All questionnaire items, response alternatives and variable definitions are presented in Online Resource 1.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using SAS version 9.4 for Windows (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). Differences in proportions of demographic variables between cohorts were analysed using Chi square test and Mann–Whitney for medians. Point prevalence was calculated as the proportion of cases reporting a currently occurring symptom at the time of survey divided by the total number within each cohort included in the analysis. Log-binomial regression was used to analyse risk ratios (RR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI), with the population control cohort as the reference. RRs were assessed in relation to age categorised into five discrete strata. In the analyses of RR in relation to occupational noise and stressful working conditions, only women currently working were included. Regression models were adjusted for potential confounders including age, education and income, smoking, use of hearing protection at work and leisure noise index. In addition, a test of trend (Mantel–Haenszel Chi square test) was used to assess whether symptom prevalence increased by age category within each cohort, and by exposure strata within each cohort. The order of the exposure categories was assumed as: unexposed followed by stress only, noise only, and lastly, both noise and stress). Incidence rates (IR) were calculated as retrospectively reported symptom onset between age 24 and 65 divided by the sum of person-years at risk (number of years after age 24) between the ages of 24–65, and presented per 1000 person-years. For IR, the 95% CI was calculated as approximating a Poisson distribution (Rosner 2015). Incidence rate ratios (IRR) were calculated with the test-based 95% CI (Miettinen 1976). The population control cohort was used as the reference. The recall bias of retrospectively reported symptom onset was assessed visually in Kaplan–Meier survival curves stratified by event recall time (reporting the onset as occurring 5, 10, or 15 years prior to the survey). Finally, a non-response analysis of RR for self-reported hearing loss and tinnitus was assessed by comparing non-respondents to respondents separately within each cohort, using log-binomial regression and adjusting for age, with respondents as the reference. In the non-response analysis, sampling weights were used and standard errors were corrected for the difference in sample size between respondents and non-respondents (SAS usage note 23003, available at http://​support.​sas.​com/​kb/​23/​003.​html, retrieved 16-07-07). The significance level was set at 5% (p = 0.05) for all tests.

Results

A major difference between the two cohorts was found in the proportion reporting occupational noise exposure. As shown in Table 1, 75% of the preschool teachers reported having to raise their voice due to noise at work, compared to 29% among the controls. Despite this, fewer preschool teachers reported use of hearing protection at work (Fig. 2). Preschool teachers also reported stressful working conditions more frequently than controls. For example, effort–reward imbalance was found in 80% of the preschool cohort, compared to 59% in the control cohort. Due to the difference in selection criteria for the two cohorts, there was also a large difference in education level (Table 1). Median years of working in preschool amongst the preschool teachers was 12 years (IQR 6–20 years), based on data from n = 4566 preschool teachers. Despite our efforts of excluding controls who had worked in preschool, a post hoc analysis showed that a very small proportion (0.2%, n = 94) of the control cohort had worked as child caretaker, most likely in preschool, with a median of 5.5 working years (IQR 2–13 years).
Table 1
Demographic data on female preschool teachers and randomly selected women as population controls
 
Preschool cohort (total n = 4718)a
Population controls (total n = 4122)a
p valueb
Median (IQR)
n/total n
%
(95% CI)
Median (IQR)
n/total n
%
(95% CI)
Age in years
45 (38–53)
   
48 (39–57)
   
< 0.0001
Employment status (currently working)
 
4265/4714
90
(90–91)
 
3310/4114
80
(79–82)
< 0.0001
Education and income combined (mutually exclusive categories)c
        
< 0.0001
 University education and ≥ 30,000 SEK
 
3804/4653
82
(81–83)
 
1774/4027
44
(43–46)
 
 No university education and ≥ 30,000 SEK, or, university education and < 30,000 SEK
 
849/4653
18
(17–19)
 
1528/4027
38
(36–39)
 
 Lower than university education and < 30,000 SEK
 
0/4653
0
 
725/4027
18
(17–19)
 
Smoking (ever smoked daily)
 
1213/4703
26
(25–27)
 
1591/4089
39
(37–40)
< 0.0001
Family history of hearing loss (< age 55)
 
881/4702
19
(18–20)
 
755/4100
18
(17–20)
0.698
Ear infections (recurrent or prolonged)
 
728/4690
16
(14–17)
 
580/4092
14
(13–15)
0.077
Tympanostomy tube (ever)
 
191/3351
6
(5–6)
 
149/2743
5
(5–6)
0.651
Noisy leisure activities (≥ month or more)
 
1179/4705
25
(24–26)
 
1199/4089
29
(28–31)
< 0.0001
Hearing protection leisure time (always or often)d
 
208/1164
18
(16–20)
 
233/1182
20
(17–22)
0.253
Loud music in headphones (≥ month, ≥ 75% vol.)
 
358/2695
13
(12–15)
 
460/2458
19
(17–20)
< 0.0001
Loud noise, can´t hear conversation (≥ 25% time)
 
3368/4515
75
(73–76)
 
1176/3688
32
(30–33)
< 0.0001
Loud noise, have to raise own voice (≥ 25% time)
 
3376/4517
75
(73–76)
 
1078/3689
29
(28–31)
< 0.0001
Hearing protection at work (always or often)
 
123/4521
3
(2–3)
 
170/3698
5
(4–5)
< 0.0001
Changed job/workplace due to noise (ever)
 
312/4685
7
(6–7)
 
72/4034
2
(1–2)
< 0.0001
Stressful working conditions
Effort–reward imbalance (ERI) (ratio > 1)
 
3725/4684
80
(78–81)
 
2383/4012
59
(58–61)
< 0.0001
Emotional demands, COPSOQ (often or always)
 
1699/4663
36
(35–38)
 
919/3985
23
(22–24)
< 0.0001
Exposure strata among currently workinge
        
< 0.0001
 Unexposed to noise and stress
 
384/4205
9
(8–10)
 
919/3250
29
(27–30)
 
 Stress only (ERI or COPSOQ)
 
831/4205
20
(19–21)
 
1483/3250
46
(44–48)
 
 Noise only (exposed ≥ 25% of time at work)
 
294/4205
7
(6–8)
 
163/3250
5
(4–6)
 
 Both noise and stress exposure
 
2696/4205
64
(63–66)
 
685/3250
21
(20–22)
 
IQR inter-quartile range
aTable shows data for subjects with non-missing data
bp values based on non-parametric test of difference in medians or Chi square test of difference in proportions between the two cohorts
cAll preschool teachers have a university degree and data was obtained from national registry. For controls, the proportion reporting university as the highest attained education level are shown. The rest had compulsory schooling or lower
dProportion of non-missing data among those reporting noisy leisure activities
eExclusive categories among currently working respondents excluding individuals with missing data on noise and/or stress exposure
The main result was the significantly increased risk of hearing-related symptoms among preschool teachers compared to controls. As seen in Table 2, the overall adjusted RR was more than twofold for sound-induced auditory fatigue and for hyperacusis, and almost twofold for difficulty perceiving speech. For the self-reported symptoms hearing loss and tinnitus, the risk estimates were also significantly increased although to a lesser degree. When stratifying by age, adjusted RRs were still increased for the preschool cohort and significantly so in all, but the youngest age strata for hearing loss and tinnitus (Table 2). Symptom prevalence was generally increased with increased age in both cohorts (p < 0.05), with the exception of hyperacusis and sound-induced auditory fatigue. Prevalence of hyperacusis did not increase by age among the controls (p =0.971), and sound-induced auditory fatigue did not increase by age among preschool teachers (p = 0.551), nor among controls (p = 0.644).
Table 2
2013–2014 year prevalence of hearing-related symptoms and risk ratios, among female preschool teachers compared to randomly selected women as population controls, in relation to age
 
Prevalence (%)
Risk ratio (RR)a (preschool/control)
Preschool cohort (total n = 4718)b
Population controls (total n = 4122)b
Crude
Adjustedc
n/total n
%
95% CI
n/total n
%
95% CI
RR
95% CI
RR
95% CI
Hearing loss
24–29
24/286
8
(5–12)
26/384
7
(4–9)
1.2
(0.7–2.1)
1.4
(0.8–2.5)
30–39
119/1114
11
(9–13)
57/725
8
(6–10)
1.4
(1.0–1.8)
1.7
(1.2–2.4)
40–49
269/1579
17
(15–19)
122/1048
12
(10–14)
1.5
(1.2–1.8)
1.7
(1.4–2.1)
50–59
356/1398
25
(23–28)
211/1147
18
(16–21)
1.4
(1.2–1.6)
1.6
(1.3–1.9)
60–65
101/293
34
(29–40)
172/749
23
(20–26)
1.5
(1.2–1.8)
1.8
(1.4–2.3)
All
869/4670
19
(17–20)
588/4053
15
(13–16)
1.3
(1.2–1.4)
1.6
(1.5–1.8)
Tinnitus
24–29
30/288
10
(7–14)
48/387
12
(9–16)
0.8
(0.6–1.3)
1.0
(0.6–1.8)
30–39
145/1122
13
(11–15)
60/726
8
(6–10)
1.6
(1.2–2.1)
1.6
(1.2–2.2)
40–49
273/1580
17
(15–19)
114/1052
11
(9–13)
1.6
(1.3–2.0)
1.9
(1.5–2.4)
50–59
332/1397
24
(22–26)
183/1155
16
(14–18)
1.5
(1.3–1.8)
1.6
(1.4–2.0)
60–65
78/297
26
(21–31)
150/753
20
(17–23)
1.3
(1.0–1.7)
1.8
(1.4–2.5)
All
858/4684
18
(17–19)
555/4073
14
(13–15)
1.3
(1.2–1.5)
1.7
(1.5–1.9)
Difficulty perceiving speech
24–29
94/285
33
(28–38)
72/375
19
(15–23)
1.7
(1.3–2.2)
1.8
(1.4–2.5)
30–39
429/1108
39
(36–42)
145/717
20
(17–23)
1.9
(1.6–2.3)
2.0
(1.7–2.4)
40–49
745/1576
47
(45–50)
253/1040
24
(22–27)
1.9
(1.7–2.2)
2.0
(1.8–2.3)
50–59
732/1380
53
(50–56)
354/1137
31
(28–34)
1.7
(1.5–1.9)
1.7
(1.5–1.9)
60–65
144/280
49
(43–54)
229/713
32
(29–36)
1.5
(1.3–1.8)
1.6
(1.3–1.9)
All
2136/4629
46
(45–48)
1053/3982
26
(25–28)
1.7
(1.6–1.9)
1.9
(1.7–2.0)
Hyperacusis
24–29
77/287
27
(22–32)
62/386
16
(12–20)
1.7
(1.2–2.3)
1.7
(1.2–2.4)
30–39
420/1121
37
(35–40)
144/730
20
(17–23)
1.9
(1.6–2.2)
2.0
(1.7–2.5)
40–49
649/1585
41
(39–43)
198/1053
19
(16–21)
2.2
(1.9–2.5)
2.3
(2.0–2.7)
50–59
582/1402
42
(39–44)
213/1162
18
(16–21)
2.3
(2.0–2.6)
2.5
(2.1–2.9)
60–65
102/299
34
(29–39)
134/752
18
(15–21)
1.9
(1.5–2.4)
2.3
(1.7–3.1)
All
1830/4694
39
(38–40)
751/4083
18
(17–20)
2.1
(2.0–2.3)
2.3
(2.1–2.5)
Sound-induced auditory fatigue
24–29
205/288
71
(66–76)
119/385
31
(26–36)
2.3
(2.0–2.7)
1.2
(1.1–1.3)
30–39
805/1126
71
(69–74)
232/724
32
(29–35)
2.2
(2.0–2.5)
2.4
(2.1–2.7)
40–49
1140/1585
72
(70–74)
340/1047
32
(30–35)
2.2
(2.0–2.4)
2.3
(2.1–2.5)
50–59
1002/1403
71
(69–74)
373/1155
32
(30–35)
2.2
(2.0–2.4)
1.5
(1.4–1.6)
60–65
201/295
68
(63–73)
239/735
33
(29–36)
2.1
(1.8–2.4)
2.2
(1.9–2.6)
All
3353/4697
71
(70–73)
1303/4046
32
(31–34)
2.2
(2.1–2.3)
2.4
(2.2–2.5)
aBold indicates significant risk ratio estimate from log-binomial regression (p < 0.05)
bTable shows data for subjects with non-missing data
cAdjusted for age (except for the age-stratified RR), education and income combined, smoking, hearing protection at work and leisure noise index
In addition, the exposure-stratified analysis, performed among women currently working, showed that preschool teachers still had increased adjusted RRs of hearing-related symptoms compared to controls when taking current exposure to occupational noise and stressful working conditions into account (Table 3). The increased risks observed for preschool teachers were statistically significant in most exposure strata, but also within the unexposed strata. The prevalence of hearing-related symptoms was most pronounced within the strata exposed to both noise and stressful working conditions for both cohorts, and the test of trend showed a significant increase in prevalence from the unexposed category through to exposure to both noise and stressful working conditions for all symptoms in both cohorts (p < 0.05).
Table 3
2013–2014 year prevalence of hearing-related symptoms and risk ratios, among currently working female preschool teachers compared to currently working randomly selected women as population controls, in relation to occupational exposure
 
Prevalence (%)
Risk ratio (RR)b (preschool/control)
Preschool cohort (total n = 4205)a
Population controls (total n = 3250)a
Crude
Adjustedc
n/total n
%
(95% CI)
n/total n
%
(95% CI)
RR
(95% CI)
RR
(95% CI)
Hearing loss
Unexposed to noise and stress
55/378
15
(11–18)
96/913
11
(9–13)
1.4
(1.0–1.9)
1.6
(1.1–2.2)
Stress only
106/825
13
(11–15)
188/1469
13
(11–15)
1.0
(0.8–1.3)
1.2
(0.97–1.6)
Noise only
60/293
20
(16–25)
30/160
19
(13–25)
1.1
(0.7–1.6)
1.3
(0.8–2.2)
Both noise and stress
566/2676
21
(20–23)
131/673
19
(16–22)
1.1
(0.9–1.3)
1.4
(1.1–1.7)
All
787/4172
19
(18–20)
445/3215
14
(13–15)
1.4
(1.2–1.5)
1.7
(1.5–1.9)
Tinnitus
Unexposed to noise and stress
51/382
13
(10–17)
76/915
8
(7–10)
1.6
(1.2–2.2)
2.1
(1.4–3.0)
Stress only
104/827
13
(10–15)
180/1476
12
(11–14)
1.0
(0.8–1.3)
1.2
(0.9–1.5)
Noise only
48/294
16
(12–21)
17/161
11
(6–15)
1.5
(0.9–2.6)
1.6
(0.8–2.9)
Both noise and stress
560/2679
21
(19–22)
130/681
19
(16–22)
1.1
(0.9–1.3)
1.4
(1.2–1.7)
All
763/4182
18
(17–19)
403/3233
12
(11–14)
1.5
(1.3–1.6)
1.8
(1.6–2.0)
Difficulty perceiving speech
Unexposed to noise and stress
101/380
27
(22–31)
155/917
17
(14–19)
1.6
(1.3–2.0)
1.6
(1.3–2.0)
Stress only
279/825
34
(31–37)
368/1480
25
(23–27)
1.4
(1.2–1.5)
1.5
(1.3–1.7)
Noise only
128/293
44
(38–49)
46/162
28
(21–35)
1.5
(1.2–2.0)
1.4
(1.0–2.0)
Both noise and stress
1453/2675
54
(53–56)
312/682
46
(42–49)
1.2
(1.1–1.3)
1.3
(1.1–1.4)
All
1961/4173
47
(46–49)
881/3241
27
(26–29)
1.7
(1.6–1.8)
1.9
(1.7–2.0)
Hyperacusis
Unexposed to noise and stress
67/383
17
(14–21)
93/915
10
(8–12)
1.7
(1.3–2.3)
1.7
(1.2–2.3)
Stress only
191/830
23
(20–26)
230/1477
16
(14–17)
1.5
(1.2–1.8)
1.5
(1.2–1.8)
Noise only
76/292
26
(21–31)
26/162
16
(10–22)
1.6
(1.1–2.4)
1.3
(0.8–2.1)
Both noise and stress
1305/2685
49
(47–50)
191/681
28
(25–31)
1.7
(1.5–2.0)
1.8
(1.6–2.1)
All
1639/4190
39
(38–41)
540/3235
17
(15–18)
2.3
(2.2–2.6)
2.4
(2.2–2.6)
Sound-induced auditory fatigue
Unexposed to noise and stress
98/384
26
(21–30)
88/915
10
(8–12)
2.7
(2.0–3.4)
2.7
(2.0–3.6)
Stress only
370/828
45
(41–48)
368/1477
25
(23–27)
1.8
(1.6–2.0)
1.6
(1.5–1.8)
Noise only
192/292
66
(60–71)
59/163
36
(29–44)
1.8
(1.5–2.3)
1.5
(1.2–2.0)
Both noise and stress
2353/2690
87
(86–89)
463/683
68
(64–71)
1.3
(1.2–1.4)
1.3
(1.2–1.4)
All
3013/4194
72
(70–73)
978/3238
30
(29–32)
2.4
(2.3–2.5)
1.9
(1.8–1.9)
aIncluding only women currently working with data on occupational exposures. Table shows data for subjects with non-missing data
bBold indicates significant risk ratio estimate from log-binomial regression (p < 0.05)
cAdjusted for age, education and income combined, smoking, hearing protection at work and leisure noise index
Furthermore, the preschool cohort reported the first occurrence of symptoms (symptom onset) earlier in life compared to the controls. Thus, incidence rates were higher in the preschool cohort than in the control cohort resulting in significantly increased IRRs for the preschool cohort compared to the control cohort for all the symptoms assessed (Table 4). Hyperacusis and difficulty perceiving speech displayed a tripled and doubled IRR, respectively, for the preschool cohort. The effect of recall due to retrospective report of symptom onset was similar in both cohorts, as shown by generally parallel Kaplan–Meier survival curves stratified by different length of recall time (Online Resource 2).
Table 4
Incidence rates and incidence rate ratio of hearing-related symptoms with onset between age 24–65, among female preschool teachers compared to randomly selected women as population controls, in relation to birth year a
 
Incidence rate (IR) (cases/1000 person-years)
Incidence rate ratio (IRR)b (preschool/control)
Preschool cohort (total n = 4718)c
Population controls (total n = 4122)c
Crude
n/total n
IR
(95% CI)
n/total n
IR
(95% CI)
IRR
(95% CI)
Hearing loss
Birth cohort 1989–1984
9/271
10.1
(3.5–16.7)
3/361
2.9
(− 0.4–6.3)
3.4
(1.0–11.7)
Birth cohort 1984–1974
67/1062
6.0
(4.5–7.4)
19/687
2.6
(1.4–3.8)
2.3
(1.4–3.7)
Birth cohort 1974–1964
210/1520
7.2
(6.2–8.2)
71/997
3.5
(2.7–4.4)
2.0
(1.6–2.6)
Birth cohort 1964–1954
284/1326
7.7
(6.8–8.6)
152/1088
4.8
(4.1–5.6)
1.6
(1.3–1.9)
Birth cohort 1954–1948
87/279
9.2
(7.3–11.2)
138/715
5.3
(4.5–6.2)
1.7
(1.3–2.3)
All
657/4458
7.5
(6.9–8.1)
383/3848
4.5
(4.0–4.9)
1.7
(1.5–1.9)
Tinnitus
Birth cohort 1989–1984
17/275
19.1
(10.0–28.2)
9/348
9.0
(3.1–14.9)
2.1
(1.0–4.7)
Birth cohort 1984–1974
109/1086
9.6
(7.8–11.4)
30/696
4.1
(2.6–5.6)
2.3
(1.6–3.5)
Birth cohort 1974–1964
251/1558
8.4
(7.4–9.5)
87/1025
4.2
(3.3–5.1)
2.0
(1.7–2.5)
Birth cohort 1964–1954
311/1376
8.0
(7.1–8.9)
159/1131
4.8
(4.1–5.6)
1.7
(1.4–2.0)
Birth cohort 1954–1948
73/292
7.1
(5.6–8.7)
134/737
5.0
(4.1–5.8)
1.4
(1.1–1.9)
All
761/4587
8.4
(7.8–9.0)
419/3937
4.7
(4.3–5.2)
1.8
(1.6–2.0)
Difficulty perceiving speech
Birth cohort 1989–1984
58/249
77.7
(57.7–97.8)
24/327
26.5
(15.9–37.0)
2.9
(1.9–4.6)
Birth cohort 1984–1974
347/1026
36.2
(32.4–40.0)
92/664
13.8
(11.0–16.7)
2.6
(2.1–3.3)
Birth cohort 1974–1964
676/1507
25.8
(23.9–27.8)
193/980
10.3
(8.8–11.7)
2.5
(2.2–2.9)
Birth cohort 1964–1954
675/1323
19.8
(18.3–21.2)
301/1084
10.0
(8.8–11.1)
2.0
(1.7–2.3)
Birth cohort 1954–1948
121/265
13.7
(11.3–16.1)
198/682
8.2
(7.0–9.3)
1.7
(1.3–2.1)
All
1877/4370
23.6
(22.5–24.7)
808/3737
10.0
(9.3–10.7)
2.4
(2.2–2.6)
Hyperacusis
Birth cohort 1989–1984
56/266
69.7
(51.5–88.0)
10/334
10.6
(4.0–17.1)
6.6
(3.7–11.8)
Birth cohort 1984–1974
350/1051
35.2
(31.5–38.9)
90/676
13.3
(10.5–16.0)
2.7
(2.1–3.3)
Birth cohort 1974–1964
586/1522
21.9
(20.1–23.6)
146/1001
7.4
(6.2–8.6)
2.9
(2.5–3.5)
Birth cohort 1964–1954
530/1350
14.8
(13.6–16.1)
179/1128
5.5
(4.7–6.3)
2.7
(2.3–3.2)
Birth cohort 1954–1948
92/289
9.2
(7.4–11.1)
114/732
4.3
(3.5–5.0)
2.2
(1.7–2.8)
All
1614/4478
19.4
(18.5–20.3)
539/3871
6.2
(5.7–6.8)
3.1
(2.8–3.4)
Sound-induced auditory fatigue d
 
N/A
  
N/A
   
aSubjects in birth cohort 1989–1984 had age 24–29 at year of survey (2013–2014), birth cohort 1984–1974 had age 30–39, birth cohort 1974–1964 had age 40–49, birth cohort 1964–1954 had age 50–59 and birth cohort 1954–1948 had age 60–65. Hence, strata are mutually exclusive categories
bBold indicates significant incidence rate ratio estimate from log-binomial regression (p < 0.05)
cTable shows data for subjects with non-missing data
dData on incidence (symptom onset year) for sound-induced auditory fatigue was not collected
Lastly, the non-response analysis showed that, compared to responders, non-responders in both cohorts had increased age-adjusted RR of self-reported hearing loss (preschool cohort RR: 1.4, 95% CI 1.3–1.6; control cohort RR: 1.4, 95% CI 1.2–1.5). For tinnitus, RR was significantly increased for non-responders in the preschool cohort (RR: 1.2, 95% CI 1.1–1.3), but not in the control cohort (RR 0.9, 95% CI 0.8–1.0).

Discussion

The main finding of this study was the increased risks of self-reported hearing-related symptoms among female preschool teachers, compared to women in the general population. To the best of our knowledge, the current study is the first to present relative risk estimates for this occupational group.
Although causal effects of occupational exposure to noise and stress were not explicitly assessed, we found support for the importance of these occupational factors for hearing-related symptoms. Firstly, a larger proportion of preschool teachers reported these occupational exposures compared to controls. These results are in line with previous studies showing that preschool teachers are exposed to high sound levels in preschool (Persson Waye et al. 2010; Sjödin et al. 2012; Gerhardsson and Nilsson 2013) and have an increased risk of stressful working conditions compared to non-human service occupations (Wieclaw et al. 2006). In addition, other potential risk factors, such as smoking and low socioeconomic status were less common among preschool teachers. Nonetheless, we cannot neglect the possibility that these factors confound our main findings. However, when adjusting for smoking, we still observed the same findings with only minor changes in the risk estimates. Secondly, a high symptom prevalence was found among those currently exposed to noise and stress at work compared to those who were defined as unexposed. Thirdly, we found that preschool teachers seldom use hearing protection at work even though a majority of them report being exposed to high sound levels at work. This could further explain the increased risk, as preschool teachers would per se be more exposed than controls who report similar extent of noise exposure. A similar behaviour has been reported among musicians (Laitinen 2005). Taken together, these results strongly indicate an importance of the preschool work environment in explaining the identified increase in risk of hearing-related symptoms among preschool teachers.
Researchers have argued that a functioning stress response is vital for the auditory system (Hasson et al. 2013). Although stressful working conditions is an important factor within human service professions such as preschool teaching (Wieclaw et al. 2006), whether it has a causal effect on hearing-related symptoms needs to be studied further. Interestingly, the increase in prevalence of hearing-related symptoms observed in the combined noise and stress exposure strata in the current study indicate a possible interaction effect between noise and stress. A previous study has showed an additive effect of exposure to both stressful working conditions, assessed as job-strain, and road-traffic noise on myocardial infarction (Selander et al. 2013). It should be noted that the Job-Demand-Control, used to assess job-strain in the cited study, to a larger extent focus on instrumental contents compared to the ERI model used in the current study, which emphasise social aspects of work.
In the current study, one of the most notable increases in risk was seen for the symptom hyperacusis, with more than a twofold RR and as much as a threefold IRR for the preschool teachers compared to the controls. Although different aetiologies involving both peripheral and central underlying factors may explain hyperacusis, noise exposure has been suggested to be one of the most common causes, and noise has been used in experimental studies to induce hyperacusis (Aazh et al. 2014; Pienkowski et al. 2014). Hyperacusis has previously been found prevalent among rock and jazz musicians who are exposed to high sound levels (Kähäri et al. 2003), also indicating an effect of noise exposure. In addition, studies have shown that teachers and childcare professionals are common occupations among hyperacusis patients together with musicians (Anari et al. 1999; Jüris et al. 2013). The prevalence of hyperacusis among preschool teachers in this study is similar to a previous study reporting that 45% out of 101 Swedish preschool personnel experienced hyperacusis “sometimes” or “quite often” (Sjödin et al. 2012). Hyperacusis has been defined as “discomfort for sounds that would be acceptable to most people” (Khalfa et al. 2002), where moderately intense sounds are judged as very loud (Tyler et al. 2014), or even painful (Baguley 2014). As hyperacusis can lead to avoidance and fear of everyday sounds (Bläsing et al. 2010; Blaesing and Kroener-Herwig 2012), the unpredictable and irregular sound environment in preschools can be expected to be particularly disabling for preschool personnel with hyperacusis. These consequences might influence the prevalence and could possibly further explain the pronounced increase in risk among the preschool teachers compared to the controls.
The risk of self-reported hearing loss was less pronounced compared to other symptoms in our study. A review recently concluded that there is insufficient evidence for occupational noise in preschool causing elevated pure tone hearing thresholds and that noise levels are probably too low to cause such hearing loss (Lie et al. 2016). Preschool personnel have been reported to have slightly worse pure tone hearing thresholds compared to reference data, although most had thresholds within the “normal range”, i.e., better than 20–25 dB HL (Sjödin et al. 2012). It is worth noting though that elevated pure tone thresholds even within the normal range may be indicative of disorder. It is possible that self-reporting of hearing loss cannot fully capture slightly elevated pure tone thresholds. This is in line with previous validation studies indicating a higher sensitivity for self-reported hearing loss in relation to more severely elevated pure tone thresholds (Nondahl et al. 1998; Sindhusake et al. 2001; Fredriksson et al. 2016). Based on earlier research among preschool teachers, we had expected that tinnitus would be more common in the preschool cohort (Sjödin et al. 2012). Instead, occurrence and relative risk of tinnitus were less pronounced compared to the other symptoms. The prevalence estimates among controls were, however, comparable to that of earlier population studies (Axelsson and Ringdahl 1989; Hasson et al. 2010).
Notably, sound-induced auditory fatigue was clearly the most common symptom in our study. Unlike the other symptoms, the prevalence of sound-induced auditory fatigue was not affected by age, but was particularly high in relation to noise exposure at the current workplace. Unfortunately, the IR of sound-induced auditory fatigue was not available to assess. There is limited previous research on sound-induced auditory fatigue, but we hypothesise that the information content of the noise and the difficulty to wear hearing protection in preschool, and likely also exposure to psychosocial stress, are important factors explaining this symptom. These factors are more prominent in the preschool work environment, which may explain the increased risk among the preschool teachers. Our current results are in line with a previous study in which we found increased odds ratios of sound-induced auditory fatigue among obstetrical personnel in relation to self-reported occupational noise exposure, noise annoyance, and to some degree work-related stress, but not in relation to age (Fredriksson et al. 2015).
The current study has both limitations and strengths. A major strength is the large sample size, which yielded power to detect effects, except in parts of the stratified analyses. The overall response rate to the survey was 38% in the control cohort and 51% in the preschool cohort, and generally lower in the younger age groups compared to the older age groups, particularly among controls. This could affect the generalisability of our results, potentially causing a response bias. However, the non-response analysis indicated relatively minor influence on our main findings and, if anything, a possible underestimation of the risk among responders compared to non-responders. Another limitation is the cross-sectional data. However, collecting retrospective reports of symptom onset gave us the opportunity to also assess incidence. A similar retrospective self-report method has been used successfully for assessing asthma onset (Torén et al. 2006). We found that the effect of recall of retrospectively reported onset was similar in both cohorts, and thus should not bias these risk estimates. Nevertheless, as subjects in the two cohorts may have different demands on hearing ability and awareness of changes in their hearing, it is possible that recall of symptoms onset, influenced by perception bias, will differ slightly in the two cohorts.
The study has limitations regarding interpretation of exposure effects and causal effects, as we focused on comparing two cohorts having a different occupational history and occupational exposure, rather than assessing exposure effects directly. Misclassification of exposure may have affected the results of the exposure-stratified analysis, as we saw increased risk in the unexposed strata and as we only had current exposure available. Further analysis of the direct effects of exposure as well as long-term causal effects in longitudinal studies are needed. Potential misclassification of outcomes due to self-report also limits the extent to which conclusion can be drawn regarding manifestation of specific auditory physiological disorders. To date, symptoms such as tinnitus and hyperacusis, however, are mainly diagnosed by self-report in the clinic.
Future research should also study the consequences of suffering from hearing-related symptoms such as hyperacusis in relation to the ability to continue working in preschool. These consequences may to some extent explain why preschool teachers in this study reported to have changed job due to noise at work to a greater extent than the control cohort has. This also indicates a need for preventative and remedying measures to be taken in the preschool work environment.

Conclusion

This study showed that working as a preschool teacher significantly increases the relative risk of self-reported hearing-related symptoms compared to women in the general population. The relative risk was generally increased both when stratifying by age and by current exposure to occupational noise and stressful working conditions. Overall, the risk was most pronounced for the symptoms sound-induced auditory fatigue, hyperacusis and difficulty perceiving speech, and increased to a somewhat lesser degree for self-reported hearing loss and tinnitus.
Longitudinal studies are needed to ascertain causal effects and to disentangle the mechanisms for the different symptoms. At present, the high symptom prevalence found in this study among preschool teachers and women exposed to noise and stress at work indicate that these occupational factors are of importance. Thus, preventative measures in the work environment should not be delayed.

Acknowledgements

Open access funding provided by University of Gothenburg. The authors would like to thank Agneta Agge for assistance in data collection and Laith Hussain-Alkateeb for input on statistical analysis.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors have no competing interest to report.

Ethical approval

The Ethics Committee of Gothenburg Sweden approved this study (060-13). All participants received written information and gave their consent by returning a completed questionnaire.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Aazh H, McFerran D, Salvi R, Prasher D, Jastreboff M, Jastreboff P (2014) Insights from the first international conference on hyperacusis: causes, evaluation, diagnosis and treatment. Noise Health 16(69):123CrossRef Aazh H, McFerran D, Salvi R, Prasher D, Jastreboff M, Jastreboff P (2014) Insights from the first international conference on hyperacusis: causes, evaluation, diagnosis and treatment. Noise Health 16(69):123CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Andersson G, Lindvall N, Hursti T, Carlbring P (2002) Hypersensitivity to sound (hyperacusis): a prevalence study conducted via the internet and post: Hipersensibilidad al sonido (hiperacusia): un estudio de prevalencia realizado por internet y por correo. Int J Audiol 41(8):545–554. https://doi.org/10.3109/14992020209056075 CrossRef Andersson G, Lindvall N, Hursti T, Carlbring P (2002) Hypersensitivity to sound (hyperacusis): a prevalence study conducted via the internet and post: Hipersensibilidad al sonido (hiperacusia): un estudio de prevalencia realizado por internet y por correo. Int J Audiol 41(8):545–554. https://​doi.​org/​10.​3109/​1499202020905607​5 CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Baguley DM (2014) Hyperacusis: an overview. Seminars in hearing. Thieme Medical Publishers, Stuttgart Baguley DM (2014) Hyperacusis: an overview. Seminars in hearing. Thieme Medical Publishers, Stuttgart
Zurück zum Zitat Concha-Barrientos M, Campbell-Lendrum D, Steenland K (2004) Occupational noise: assessing the burden of disease from work related hearing impairment at national and local levels. Environmental Burden of Disease Series, No. 9, Geneva, World Health Organization Concha-Barrientos M, Campbell-Lendrum D, Steenland K (2004) Occupational noise: assessing the burden of disease from work related hearing impairment at national and local levels. Environmental Burden of Disease Series, No. 9, Geneva, World Health Organization
Zurück zum Zitat Fredriksson S, Hammar O, Torén K, Tenenbaum A, Persson Waye K (2015) The effect of occupational noise exposure on tinnitus and sound-induced auditory fatigue among obstetrics personnel: a cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 5(3):e005793CrossRef Fredriksson S, Hammar O, Torén K, Tenenbaum A, Persson Waye K (2015) The effect of occupational noise exposure on tinnitus and sound-induced auditory fatigue among obstetrics personnel: a cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 5(3):e005793CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gerhardsson L, Nilsson E (2013) Noise disturbances in daycare centers before and after acoustical treatment. J Environ Health 75(7):36–40 Gerhardsson L, Nilsson E (2013) Noise disturbances in daycare centers before and after acoustical treatment. J Environ Health 75(7):36–40
Zurück zum Zitat Hasson D, Theorell T, Wallen M, Leineweber C, Canlon B (2011) Stress and prevalence of hearing problems in the Swedish working population. BMC Public Health 11(1):130CrossRef Hasson D, Theorell T, Wallen M, Leineweber C, Canlon B (2011) Stress and prevalence of hearing problems in the Swedish working population. BMC Public Health 11(1):130CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Khalfa S, Dubal S, Veuillet E, Perez-Diaz F, Jouvent R, Collet L (2002) Psychometric normalization of a hyperacusis questionnaire. ORL 64(6):436–442CrossRef Khalfa S, Dubal S, Veuillet E, Perez-Diaz F, Jouvent R, Collet L (2002) Psychometric normalization of a hyperacusis questionnaire. ORL 64(6):436–442CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kristensen TS, Hannerz H, Høgh A, Borg V (2005) The Copenhagen Psychosocial Questionnaire—a tool for the assessment and improvement of the psychosocial work environment. Scand J Work Environ Health 31(6):438–449CrossRef Kristensen TS, Hannerz H, Høgh A, Borg V (2005) The Copenhagen Psychosocial Questionnaire—a tool for the assessment and improvement of the psychosocial work environment. Scand J Work Environ Health 31(6):438–449CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Laitinen H (2005) Factors affecting the use of hearing protectors among classical music players. Noise Health 7(26):21–29CrossRef Laitinen H (2005) Factors affecting the use of hearing protectors among classical music players. Noise Health 7(26):21–29CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat McEwen BS (2006) Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators: central role of the brain. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 8(4):367 McEwen BS (2006) Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators: central role of the brain. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 8(4):367
Zurück zum Zitat Persson Waye K, Agge A, Hillström J, Lindström F (2010) Being in a pre-school sound environment—annoyance and subjective symptoms among personnel and children. Internoise, Lisbon Persson Waye K, Agge A, Hillström J, Lindström F (2010) Being in a pre-school sound environment—annoyance and subjective symptoms among personnel and children. Internoise, Lisbon
Zurück zum Zitat Pienkowski M, Tyler RS, Roncancio ER, Jun HJ, Brozoski T, Dauman N, Coelho CB, Andersson G, Keiner AJ, Cacace AT (2014) A review of hyperacusis and future directions: Part II. Measurement, mechanisms, and treatment. Am J Audiol 23(4):420–436CrossRef Pienkowski M, Tyler RS, Roncancio ER, Jun HJ, Brozoski T, Dauman N, Coelho CB, Andersson G, Keiner AJ, Cacace AT (2014) A review of hyperacusis and future directions: Part II. Measurement, mechanisms, and treatment. Am J Audiol 23(4):420–436CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rosner B (2015) Confidence limits for incidence rates. In: Fundamentals of biostatistics, 8th edn. Cengage Learning, Inc., Mason, OH, pp 781 Rosner B (2015) Confidence limits for incidence rates. In: Fundamentals of biostatistics, 8th edn. Cengage Learning, Inc., Mason, OH, pp 781
Zurück zum Zitat Selander J, Bluhm G, Nilsson M, Hallqvist M, Theorell T, Willix P, Pershagen G (2013) Joint effects of job strain and road-traffic and occupational noise on myocardial infarction. Scand J Work Environ Health 39(2):195–203CrossRef Selander J, Bluhm G, Nilsson M, Hallqvist M, Theorell T, Willix P, Pershagen G (2013) Joint effects of job strain and road-traffic and occupational noise on myocardial infarction. Scand J Work Environ Health 39(2):195–203CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sindhusake D, Mitchell P, Smith W, Golding M, Newall P, Hartley D, Rubin G (2001) Validation of self-reported hearing loss. The Blue Mountains hearing study. Int J Epidemiol 30(6):1371–1378CrossRef Sindhusake D, Mitchell P, Smith W, Golding M, Newall P, Hartley D, Rubin G (2001) Validation of self-reported hearing loss. The Blue Mountains hearing study. Int J Epidemiol 30(6):1371–1378CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sjödin F, Kjellberg A, Knutsson A, Landström U, Lindberg L (2012) Noise exposure and auditory effects on preschool personnel. Noise Health 14(57):72–82CrossRef Sjödin F, Kjellberg A, Knutsson A, Landström U, Lindberg L (2012) Noise exposure and auditory effects on preschool personnel. Noise Health 14(57):72–82CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tyler RS, Pienkowski M, Roncancio ER, Jun HJ, Brozoski T, Dauman N, Coelho CB, Andersson G, Keiner AJ, Cacace AT (2014) A review of hyperacusis and future directions: part I. Definitions and manifestations. Am J Audiol 23(4):402–419CrossRef Tyler RS, Pienkowski M, Roncancio ER, Jun HJ, Brozoski T, Dauman N, Coelho CB, Andersson G, Keiner AJ, Cacace AT (2014) A review of hyperacusis and future directions: part I. Definitions and manifestations. Am J Audiol 23(4):402–419CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Working in preschool increases the risk of hearing-related symptoms: a cohort study among Swedish women
Publikationsdatum
08.07.2019
Erschienen in
International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health / Ausgabe 8/2019
Print ISSN: 0340-0131
Elektronische ISSN: 1432-1246
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00420-019-01453-0

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 8/2019

International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health 8/2019 Zur Ausgabe