Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Ophthalmology 1/2020

Open Access 01.12.2020 | Research article

Association of Photopic and Mesopic Contrast Sensitivity in older drivers with risk of motor vehicle collision using naturalistic driving data

verfasst von: Cynthia Owsley, Thomas Swain, Rong Liu, Gerald McGwin Jr, Mi Young Kwon

Erschienen in: BMC Ophthalmology | Ausgabe 1/2020

Abstract

Background

Older drivers have a crash rate nearly equal to that of young drivers whose crash rate is the highest among all age groups. Contrast sensitivity impairment is common in older adults. The purpose of this study is to examine whether parameters from the photopic and mesopic contrast sensitivity functions (CSF) are associated with incident motor vehicle crash involvement by older drivers.

Methods

This study utilized data from older drivers (ages ≥60 years) who participated in the Strategic Highway Research Program Naturalistic Driving Study, a prospective, population-based study. At baseline participants underwent photopic and mesopic contrast sensitivity testing for targets from 1.5–18 cycles per degree. Model fitting generated area under the log CSF (AULCSF) and peak log sensitivity. Participant vehicles were instrumented with sensors that captured continuous driving data when the vehicle was operating (accelerometers, global positioning system, forward radar, 4-channel video). They participated for 1–2 years. Crashes were coded from the video and other data streams by trained analysts.

Results

The photopic analysis was based on 844 drivers, and the mesopic on 854 drivers. Photopic AULCSF and peak log contrast sensitivity were not associated with crash rate, whether defined as all crashes or at-fault crashes only (all p > 0.05). Mesopic AULCSF and peak log sensitivity were associated with an increased crash rate when considered for all crashes (rate ratio (RR): 1.36, 95% CI: 1.06–1.72; RR: 1.28, 95% CI: 1.01–1.63, respectively) and at-fault crashes only (RR: 1.50, 95% CI: 1.16–1.93; RR: 1.38, 95% CI: 1.07–1.78, respectively).

Conclusions

Results suggest that photopic contrast sensitivity testing may not help us understand future crash risk at the older-driver population level. Results highlight a previously unappreciated association between older adults’ mesopic contrast sensitivity deficits and crash involvement regardless of the time of day. Given the wide variability of light levels encountered in both day and night driving, mesopic vision tests, with their reliance on both cone and rod vision, may be a more comprehensive assessment of the visual system’s ability to process the roadway environment.
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
AULCSF
Area under log contrast sensitivity function
CSF
Contrast sensitivity function
CSmax
Contrast sensitivitymax
DAS
Data acquisition system
GPS
Global positioning system
IRB
Institutional Review Board
RR
Rate ratio
SF
Spatial frequency
SHRP2
Strategic Highway Research Program
US
United States

Background

Older drivers are the fastest growing group of drivers in the United States (US) [1], both in terms of the number of drivers and the number of annual miles driven. They have a crash rate nearly equal to that of young drivers whose crash rate is the highest among all age groups [2, 3]. Research over the past several decades has indicated that vision impairment contributes to older drivers’ increased crash risk [46]. The vast majority of studies on vision and older driver safety have utilized accident reports from police agencies as the outcome measure for collision involvement. In the Unites States these accident reports are routinely indexed by the state jurisdiction in which they occur, with many states making these reports available to scientists (after appropriate regulatory approval) for the purposes of traffic safety research. While these reports provide a wealth of information about the circumstances of a crash (e.g., driver age, place, weather conditions, vehicles involved), they reveal little to nothing about visual and other mechanisms underlying the occurrence of a crash. An alternative to using accident reports to study risk factors for crash involvement is to conduct naturalistic driving studies [7, 8]. Naturalistic driving data are generated by participants driving their own vehicles in the course of their everyday life over long observation periods (e.g., one to two years). Their vehicles are unobtrusively equipped with sensors and video cameras, which record vehicle kinematics, global positioning system (GPS) location, presence of near-by objects, driver behavior (e.g., gaze direction, secondary task activity), and the roadway environment. Naturalistic data provide an unprecedented level of objective detail on safety critical events such as crashes including pre-crash information about driver behavior and roadway contextual factors.
It is well established that spatial contrast sensitivity impairment is common among older adults [912]. As with most of the vision and driver safety research literature, studies on contrast sensitivity and crash risk have relied on accident reports comparing collision rates for those with contrast sensitivity impairment to those with no or minimal impairment. While some studies report that contrast sensitivity impairment is associated with a recent history of crash involvement [13, 14] others have reported no association between contrast sensitivity and incident (or future) crash involvement [1517]. Contrast sensitivity impairment from age-related cataract is associated with a history of higher collision rates [18] and on-road driving performance problems [19, 20]. Drivers with contrast sensitivity impairment secondary to Parkinson’s disease experience on-road difficulties [2124]. Impaired contrast sensitivity regardless of etiology has also been associated with on-road driving problems [25, 26].
Older driver studies on contrast sensitivity and collision involvement up until now have for the most part assessed contrast sensitivity using a letter chart, specifically the Pelli-Robson chart [27]. The Pelli-Robson chart measures how much contrast is required to identify letters subtending 2.8° of visual angle; letter size is not varied on the chart, but letter contrast is. The chart is a popular choice in epidemiological and clinical studies because it is brief, easy to administer, and has good reproducibility [2830]. It also has confirmed construct validity for everyday visual task performance in that reduced contrast sensitivity as measured by the chart is associated with adverse outcomes such as falls [31], mortality [32], performance mobility deficits [33], and slowed reading [34]. A limitation of the Pelli-Robson chart is that it does not measure a person’s contrast sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency (i.e., target size) [35].
Prior research using SHRP2 data from older drivers [36] examined the relationship between contrast sensitivity and crash risk. In SHRP2 contrast sensitivity was assessed by the Optec 6500 P which measures contrast sensitivity for five spatial frequency targets ranging from 1.5 to 18 cycles per degree (cpd) [37]. Contrast sensitivity results were presented for some but not all spatial frequencies tested, with no explanation provided for why some were omitted. It appears that only statistically significant results were presented.
When a set of thresholds for a range of spatial frequencies is measured as done in SHRP2, a model can be used to fit thresholds to form a spatial contrast sensitivity function (CSF); many CSF models have been put forth [38, 39]. Some of these models can provide good fits to the raw data with only 4 parameters [40]. The major advantage of measuring the CSF, rather than contrast sensitivity for a single target size, is that it constitutes a comprehensive summary of visibility for a broad variety of spatial stimuli [39].
The purpose of this study is to examine the association of photopic and mesopic CSFs with motor vehicle crash involvement by older drivers using naturalistic driving techniques. Photopic vision is mediated by cone photoreceptors only, whereas mesopic vision is mediated by both cone and rod photoreceptors.

Methods

Data source

The study utilized data from the Strategic Highway Research Program (SHRP2) Naturalistic Driving Study, the largest naturalistic driving study conducted to date [8]. Details of the SHRP2 study design and methods have been previously published [4143]. The study consisted of a large sample of drivers from six study sites in the United States (Bloomington, IN; State College, PA; Tampa Bay, FL; Buffalo, NY; Durham, NC; Seattle, WA), representing various geographies, climates, state laws, and road types. A combination of random-digit dialing and public advertising was used to recruit more than 18,000 individuals for screening [41]. The Virginia Tech Center for Survey Research phoned potential participants to discuss the study protocol and confirm eligibility. Individuals who were licensed drivers, drove at least 3 days per week, planned to keep the vehicle for the duration of the study, and had an eligible and mechanically sound vehicle were eligible to participate [41]. Participants completed a standard intake process during a single in-person visit at regional sites. Participants were enrolled from October 2010 through December 2013 and most were followed for 1 or 2 years. The final SHRP2 sample included 3541 drivers aged 16–98 years. The focus in the present analysis was on the ≥60-year-old subsample of SHRP2 which included 1019 participants, since deficits in spatial contrast sensitivity typically become more prevalent by the 60s [912]. The Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the National Academy of Sciences, Virginia Tech Transportation Institute (VTTI), and the regional study sites provided oversight for the SHRP2 study [44]. The analysis reported here was approved by the IRB of the University of Alabama at Birmingham. The study followed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki.
At the enrollment visit, participants completed a questionnaire on demographic characteristics (age, gender, race/ethnicity). Contrast sensitivity was measured for each eye separately using habitual correction under both daytime conditions (85 cd/m2, photopic condition) and night-time conditions (3 cd/m2, mesopic condition) using the Optec 6500 P (Stereo Optical Co., Inc., Chicago, IL), a contrast sensitivity screening device [37]. It is based on the Functional Acuity Contrast Test [45]. The test presents sine-wave grating stimuli of 1.5, 3, 6, 12, and 18 cpd as nine circular patches, with each patch being 1.5 log contrast decreased from the one before it. The grating pattern can be oriented to the left, up, or right, creating a three-alternative forced-choice response format. The participant is asked to indicate the orientation of the grating. Testing for a given spatial frequency is terminated at the first incorrect response, with the last correct response designated as the contrast sensitivity for that spatial frequency.

CSF model fitting

The contrast sensitivity data (i.e., 1/contrast threshold versus spatial frequency) were fitted with an asymmetric parabolic function shown to well describe both normal and low vision CSFs [46]. The function has four parameters as shown by the following equation:
$$ f(SF)=\left\{\begin{array}{c}\ {CS}_{max}-{\left( SF-{SF}_{max}\right)}^2\times {\left({\mathrm{width}}_L\right)}^2\ if\ SF<{SF}_{max}\\ {}{CS}_{max}-{\left( SF-{SF}_{max}\right)}^2\times {\left({\mathrm{width}}_R\right)}^2\ if\ SF\ge {SF}_{max}\end{array}\right., $$
where f (SF) is the contrast sensitivity at a spatial frequency SF, CSmax is the peak contrast sensitivity, SFmax is the peak spatial frequency, the spatial frequency at which CSmax occurs, and widthL and widthR are the left and right bandwidths of the asymmetric parabolic function, respectively (Fig. 1). The values for contrast sensitivity and spatial frequency were log-transformed before curve-fitting. The fits were achieved using a simplex search method [47] to search for the optimal fit producing the least squares error. The goodness of the fit was evaluated with R squared for each individual curve-fit.
As demonstrated by Chung and Legge [46], it is reasonable to fit low-vision CSF data with the asymmetric parabolic function with fixed WidthL and WidthR parameter values that are obtained from normal-vision CSFs. In other words, the asymmetric parabolic function with two free parameters and two fixed Width parameter values are sufficient enough to describe the CSF data under different visual conditions. In this study, we, thus, adopted the “template fit” approach used by Chung and Legge [46]. We adopted WidthL and WidthR parameter values estimated from the fitted curves of our 70 best photopic CSF data. By reducing the number of free parameters, we were able to avoid overfitting that might have occurred in data from a subset of subjects. We, however, confirmed that this template-fit approach yields a reasonable fit to the CSF data across subjects and viewing conditions by evaluating the goodness of the fit with R2 for each individual curve-fit. More than 80% of individual curve-fits showed a R2 value greater than 0.8 and more than 90% of individual fits showed a R2 value greater than 0.5. Participants with an R2 less than 0.5 were removed from the study. To relate the CSF to driving performance, we examined the peak contrast sensitivity, CSmax and area under the log CSF (AULCSF) which represents the overall contrast sensitivity of the subject. Driving uses both eyes, therefore binocular contrast sensitivity measures were created. Since SHRP2 did not test the CSF under binocular viewing conditions, the binocular CSF for each subject was constructed by using the better contrast sensitivity of the two eyes for a given spatial frequency.

Naturalistic driving data

While the participant underwent the enrollment visit, the personal vehicle of each participant was instrumented with a data acquisition system (DAS) that captured continuous driving data anytime the vehicle was operating. Participants were instructed to drive their vehicles as they normally would while enrolled in the study; enrollees participated for either one or 2 years. Results from other naturalistic driving studies demonstrate that participants adapt quickly to the instrumented vehicle, engaging in generally typical driving activities and reporting they are not unduly influenced by the equipment installed in their vehicles [48, 49]. The DAS included several types of sensors including accelerometers, global positioning system (GPS), forward radar, a color video camera view of the forward roadway, and three grayscale video cameras of the rear view, the driver’s face, and view over the driver’s right shoulder.
At regular intervals, data were transmitted to VTTI for processing. After removing trip files from non-consenting drivers (i.e., persons other than the participant who drove the vehicle), trained VTTI analysts reviewed video data when vehicle physical sensors detected [1] large changes in speed or position of the car with respect to the road, [2] activation of advanced safety systems (e.g. anti-lock braking), [3] the participant pushed the critical incident button to flag an event, or [4] the analysts detected an event [50]. A short window of video surrounding the potential event was reviewed to verify and classify the event as a crash or near-crash [51]. Crashes were defined as events where the SHRP2 participant’s vehicle made contact with any object (vehicles, pedestrians, cyclists, animals, tree, buildings), at any speed, including non-premeditated departures from the roadway where at least one tire left the paved or intended travel surface of the road. At-fault crash status of the driver was determined by VTTI analysts and was only coded if there was observable evidence in the video that the driver committed an error that led to the crash. VTTI analysts coding crash events were masked to each participant’s status on variables collected at the enrollment visit. Inter-rater agreement on classifying events was periodically assessed for VTTI analysts and compared to an expert rater, with the overall agreement being 88% for crash events [52]. The number of miles driven during the participant’s enrollment period was based on the odometer reading or, if unavailable, the GPS data.

Statistical analysis

Demographic characteristics and contrast sensitivity parameters (AULCSF and peak contrast sensitivity) were summarized for the sample. Impaired contrast sensitivity parameters were defined by those values in the lowest quartile of the distribution for AULCSF and peak contrast sensitivity. Poisson regression was utilized to model age adjusted rate ratios (RR) and 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) for AULCSF and peak contrast sensitivity for all crashes and all at-fault crashes regardless of the time of day of the crash. Models were fit using a log-link, log of miles driven as the offset, and the level of significance set to < 0.05.

Results

Of the 1019 participants in SHRP2 ≥ 60 years old, 89 were missing both photopic or mesopic contrast sensitivity data, resulting in 930 persons available for analysis. When fitting photopic CSF models, an additional 9 were missing photopic data, and 77 had R2 < 0.5 for the model fit, and thus were deleted, resulting in a total of 844 persons with photopic contrast sensitivity parameters. When fitting mesopic CSF models, an additional 7 were missing mesopic data and 69 had R2 < 0.5 for model fit and thus were deleted, resulting in 854 with mesopic contrast sensitivity parameters.
Approximately ¾ of the sample were in their 60s or 70s with the remaining ≥80 years old (Table 1). There were slightly more men than woman. The vast majority of participants (over 95%) were white of non-Hispanic origin. Table 2 shows the mean and standard deviation of contrast sensitivity parameters for the sample. Consistent with the literature [53], mesopic AULCSF and peak sensitivity were lower than those parameters for photopic contrast sensitivity.
Table 1
Demographic characteristics of the sample (N = 915)
Characteristic
n
% a
Age, years
 60–69
310
33.9
 70–79
396
43.3
 80–90
200
21.9
 90–99
9
1.0
Gender
 Men
491
53.7
 Women
424
46.3
Race
 White
871
95.2
 Black
15
1.6
 Otherb
22
2.4
 Unknown
7
0.8
Ethnicity
 Hispanic or Latino
12
1.3
 Not Hispanic or Latino
872
95.3
 Unknown
31
3.4
aPercents may not sum to 100 due to rounding
bOther includes Asian, American Indian or Alaska Native, and Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander
Table 2
Contrast sensitivity parameters for the sample
 
Mean
Standard Deviation
Photopic contrast sensitivity parameters (N = 844)
 AULCSF a
1.70
0.37
 Peak log sensitivity
1.89
0.21
Mesopic contrast sensitivity parameters (N = 854)
 AULCSF a
1.25
0.37
 Peak log sensitivity
1.75
0.23
aAULCSF is area under the log contrast sensitivity function
Participants drove a total of 7,417,879 miles with an average of 8107 ± 6967 miles driven over 233 ± 183 h of driving time per person. The most common vehicle driven was a passenger car (68.1%), followed by sport utility vehicle or pickup (17.6%), mini-van (5.8%), crossover (1.9%), and unknown (6.6%). Of the 915 participants, 688 had no crashes, 161 had 1 crash, 44 had 2 crashes, 9 had 3 crashes, 5 had 4 crashes, 2 had 5 crashes, 1 had 6 crashes, 3 had 7 crashes, 1 had 8 crashes, and 1 had 13 crashes. This resulted in 354 total crash events among 227 participants. Of the 354 crashes, 307 were deemed at-fault for the participant driving. The majority of crashes (272 of 354, 77%) and at-fault crashes (237 of 307, 77%) occurred during the daytime.
Table 3 lists the age-adjusted associations between photopic and mesopic contrast sensitivity parameters and crash rate. Photopic AULCSF and peak log contrast sensitivity were not associated with crash rate, whether defined as all crashes or at-fault crashes only (all p > 0.05). Mesopic AULCSF was associated with an increased crash rate when defined in terms of all crashes; drivers in the lowest AULCSF quartile were 36% more likely to incur crashes per mile driven than were those in the upper three quartiles (RR: 1.36, 95% CI: 1.06–1.72). The association between peak mesopic log sensitivity and crash involvement was slightly weaker than that for AULCSF but statistically significant, with drivers in the lowest log sensitivity quartile 28% more likely to be crash involved (RR: 1.28, 95% CI: 1.01–1.63). Both associations were stronger when considering only at-fault crashes; drivers in the lowest AULCSF and peak log sensitivity quartiles were 50 and 38% more likely, respectively, to be crash involved as compared to those in the upper three quartiles (RR: 1.50, 95% CI: 1.16–1.93; RR: 1.38, 95% CI: 1.07–1.78).
Table 3
Age-adjusted associations between impaired contrast sensitivity and rate of motor vehicle crash involvement for all crashes and for at-fault crashes
 
All Crashes (N events = 354)
At-fault Crashes (N events = 307)
Rate ratio (95% confidence interval)
P-value
Rate ratio (95% confidence interval)
P-value
Photopic contrast sensitivity parameters
 AULCSFa
0.80 (0.61–1.05)
0.102
0.77 (0.57–1.03)
0.077
 Peak log sensitivity
0.80 (0.61–1.04)
0.091
0.77 (0.58–1.03)
0.072
Mesopic contrast sensitivity parameters
 AULCSFa
1.36 (1.06–1.72)
0.015
1.50 (1.16–1.93)
0.002
 Peak log sensitivity
1.28 (1.01–1.63)
0.042
1.38 (1.07–1.78)
0.014
aAULCSF is area under the log contrast sensitivity function

Discussion

In a prospective population-based study on older drivers in the US, impaired photopic contrast sensitivity was not associated with an increased rate for incident motor vehicle crash involvement. Our results agree with two previous prospective, population-based studies on older drivers, also performed in the US [16, 17]. It is the case that other studies have found positive associations between photopic contrast sensitivity deficits and crash involvement but the outcome variable in these studies was a history of crashes, not future crashes [13, 14]. The limitation with using study designs where the outcome is crashes retrospective to enrollment is that one does not know whether, for example, the photopic contrast sensitivity impairment existed before the crash. These studies taken together provide little support for photopic contrast sensitivity testing at the population level for the purposes of reducing the burden of motor vehicle crashes.
It is useful to consider why photopic contrast sensitivity deficit is unrelated to future crash risk. First, studies have shown that contrast sensitivity impairment is related to self-reported driving difficulty [54] as well as self-regulation [55]. Driver’s with photopic contrast sensitivity deficits are more likely to avoid challenging driving situations [55, 56], reduce driving exposure (i.e., mileage) [55], and are at greater risk for driving cessation [57, 58]. Second, one of the most common causes of contrast sensitivity loss is age-related cataract. However, most older adults in the US undergo cataract surgery and intraocular lens implantation after cataracts start hampering their visual daily activities. Cataract surgery reduces motor vehicle crash risk by 50% [59]. Thus, while contrast sensitivity loss has a negative impact on driving habits, its potential negative impact on crash risk appears to be mitigated by changes in driving habits and the wide-spread availability of cataract surgery.
Although the association between photopic contrast sensitivity and crash involvement was null in our study, mesopic contrast sensitivity impairment was associated with incident crashes and at-fault crashes. For example, drivers with impaired mesopic sensitivity as defined by AULCSF were 50% more likely to incur an at-fault crash as compared to those who were unimpaired. That mesopic contrast sensitivity impairment elevates collision risk in older drivers is a novel finding for the literature. Previous studies suggest that mesopic vision deficits contribute to driver safety and performance problems in studies of samples of drivers that have a wide range of ages, but not specifically focused on older drivers. Lachenmayr et al. [60] reported that drivers with worse mesopic vision were more likely to be involved in night-time collisions, however 2/3 of those studied were not older adults, but were ≤ 60 years old. A study on bus and truck drivers found that those with “reduced twilight vision” were more frequently involved in collisions [61]. Older drivers with mesopic vision acuity impairment exhibited worse night-time driving performance on a closed driving course [62]. Black et al. found that correcting astigmatism in young drivers with toric contact lenses improved on-road driving performance which was also linked to better mesopic vision [63].
A strength of this study is the use of incident crashes as an outcome measure based on naturalistic driving recordings. The study was population-based and included a large sample of older drivers. This is the first study to utilize a contrast sensitivity function model and the parameters it generates to examine associations between older driver safety and contrast sensitivity. A strength of this approach is that it comprehensively considers the entire spatial envelope of visibility, not each spatial target’s piecemeal role in collision risk (the approach used in a previous report using SHRP2 data [36]). In that report [36], the association between collision involvement and contrast sensitivity was evaluated separately at each of 4 spatial frequencies under photopic and mesopic viewing conditions; that is, they did not fit a CSF model to the data before analysis. A limitation of our study is an insufficient sample size and number of crash events in order to study contrast sensitivity and crash involvement stratified by day- versus night-time crashes.

Conclusion

This study agrees with previous research [16, 17] that photopic contrast sensitivity testing is not useful for understanding future crash risk at the older-driver population level. Although many studies have linked photopic contrast sensitivity impairment to a history of crash involvement, the preponderance of evidence thus far suggests that photopic contrast sensitivity will be an ineffective screening tool to identify older drivers who are likely to be crash involved in the future. This study highlights a previously unappreciated association between older adults’ mesopic contrast sensitivity deficits and crash involvement regardless of the time of day. Mesopic vision is mediated by both cone and rod photoreceptors. Given the wide variability of light levels encountered in both day and night driving and the many environmental features impacting roadway illumination, mesopic vision tests, with their reliance on both cone and rod vision, may be a more comprehensive assessment of the visual system’s ability to process the roadway environment.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.
The IRBs of the National Academy of Sciences, VTTI, and the regional study sites approved the SHRP2 study and provided oversight for the study. Participants provided written informed consent after the nature and purpose of the study was described. The analysis reported here was approved by the IRB of the University of Alabama at Birmingham. The study followed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
4.
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Owsley C, Sekuler R, Siemsen D. Contrast sensitivity throughout adulthood. Vis Res. 1983;23:689–99.PubMedCrossRef Owsley C, Sekuler R, Siemsen D. Contrast sensitivity throughout adulthood. Vis Res. 1983;23:689–99.PubMedCrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Rubin GS, West SK, Munoz B, Bandeen-Roche K, Zeger S, Schein O, Fried LP, Project Team SEE. A comprehensive assessment of visual impairment in a population of older Americans. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1997;38:557–68.PubMed Rubin GS, West SK, Munoz B, Bandeen-Roche K, Zeger S, Schein O, Fried LP, Project Team SEE. A comprehensive assessment of visual impairment in a population of older Americans. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1997;38:557–68.PubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Haegerstrom-Portnoy G, Schneck ME, Brabyn JA. Seeing into old age: vision function beyond acuity. Optom Vis Sci. 1999;76:141–58.PubMedCrossRef Haegerstrom-Portnoy G, Schneck ME, Brabyn JA. Seeing into old age: vision function beyond acuity. Optom Vis Sci. 1999;76:141–58.PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Klein BEK, Klein R, Knudtson MS, Lee KE. Relationship of measures of frailty to visual function: the beaver dam eye study. Trans Am Ophthalmol Soc. 2003;101:191–200.PubMedPubMedCentral Klein BEK, Klein R, Knudtson MS, Lee KE. Relationship of measures of frailty to visual function: the beaver dam eye study. Trans Am Ophthalmol Soc. 2003;101:191–200.PubMedPubMedCentral
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Ball K, Owsley C, Sloane ME, Roenker DL, Bruni JR. Visual attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes in older drivers. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1993;34:3110–23.PubMed Ball K, Owsley C, Sloane ME, Roenker DL, Bruni JR. Visual attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes in older drivers. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1993;34:3110–23.PubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Green KA, McGwin G Jr, Owsley C. Associations between visual, hearing, and dual sensory impairments and history of motor vehicle collision involvement of older drivers. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2013;61:252–7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Green KA, McGwin G Jr, Owsley C. Associations between visual, hearing, and dual sensory impairments and history of motor vehicle collision involvement of older drivers. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2013;61:252–7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Owsley C, Ball K, McGwin G Jr, Sloane ME, Roenker DL, White MF, Overly ET. Visual processing impairment and risk of motor vehicle crash among older adults. JAMA. 1998;279:1083–8.PubMedCrossRef Owsley C, Ball K, McGwin G Jr, Sloane ME, Roenker DL, White MF, Overly ET. Visual processing impairment and risk of motor vehicle crash among older adults. JAMA. 1998;279:1083–8.PubMedCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Rubin GS, Ng ES, Bandeen-Roche K, Keyl PM, Freeman EE, West SK. A prospective, population-based study of the role of visual impairment in motor vehicle crashes among older drivers: the SEE study. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007;48:1483–91.PubMedCrossRef Rubin GS, Ng ES, Bandeen-Roche K, Keyl PM, Freeman EE, West SK. A prospective, population-based study of the role of visual impairment in motor vehicle crashes among older drivers: the SEE study. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007;48:1483–91.PubMedCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Cross JM, McGwin G Jr, Rubin GS, Ball KK, West SK, Roenker DL, Owsley C. Visual and medical risk factors for motor vehicle collision involvement among older drivers. Br J Ophthalmol. 2009;93:400–4.PubMedCrossRef Cross JM, McGwin G Jr, Rubin GS, Ball KK, West SK, Roenker DL, Owsley C. Visual and medical risk factors for motor vehicle collision involvement among older drivers. Br J Ophthalmol. 2009;93:400–4.PubMedCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Owsley C, Stalvey BT, Wells J, Sloane ME, McGwin G Jr. Visual risk factors for crash involvement in older drivers with cataract. Arch Ophthalmol. 2001;119:881–7.PubMedCrossRef Owsley C, Stalvey BT, Wells J, Sloane ME, McGwin G Jr. Visual risk factors for crash involvement in older drivers with cataract. Arch Ophthalmol. 2001;119:881–7.PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Wood JM, Carberry TP. Older drivers and cataracts: measures of driving performance before and after cataract surgery. Transp Res Rec. 1865;2004:7–13. Wood JM, Carberry TP. Older drivers and cataracts: measures of driving performance before and after cataract surgery. Transp Res Rec. 1865;2004:7–13.
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Worringham C, Wood JM, Kerr G, Silburn P. Predictors of driving assessment outcome in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2006;21:230–5.PubMedCrossRef Worringham C, Wood JM, Kerr G, Silburn P. Predictors of driving assessment outcome in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2006;21:230–5.PubMedCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Amick MM, Grace J, Ott BR. Visual and cognitive predictors of driving safety in Parkinson's disease patients. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2007;22:957–67.PubMedCrossRef Amick MM, Grace J, Ott BR. Visual and cognitive predictors of driving safety in Parkinson's disease patients. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2007;22:957–67.PubMedCrossRef
23.
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Alvarez L, Classen S. Driving with Parkinson’s disease: cut points for clinical predictors of on-road outcomes. Can J Occup Ther. 2018;85:232–41.PubMedCrossRef Alvarez L, Classen S. Driving with Parkinson’s disease: cut points for clinical predictors of on-road outcomes. Can J Occup Ther. 2018;85:232–41.PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Bowers AR, Anastasio RJ, Sheldon SS, O”Connor MG, Hollis AM, Howe PD, Horowitz TS. Can we improve clinical prediction of at-risk older drivers? Accid Anal Prev. 2013;59:537–47.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Bowers AR, Anastasio RJ, Sheldon SS, O”Connor MG, Hollis AM, Howe PD, Horowitz TS. Can we improve clinical prediction of at-risk older drivers? Accid Anal Prev. 2013;59:537–47.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Anstey KJ, Horswill MS, Wood JM, Hatherly C. The role of cognitive and visual abilities as predictors in the multifactorial model of driving safety. Accid Anal Prev. 2012;45:766–74.PubMedCrossRef Anstey KJ, Horswill MS, Wood JM, Hatherly C. The role of cognitive and visual abilities as predictors in the multifactorial model of driving safety. Accid Anal Prev. 2012;45:766–74.PubMedCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Pelli DG, Robson JG, Wilkins AJ. The design of a new letter chart for measuring contrast sensitivity. Clin Vis Sci. 1988;2:187–99. Pelli DG, Robson JG, Wilkins AJ. The design of a new letter chart for measuring contrast sensitivity. Clin Vis Sci. 1988;2:187–99.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Rubin GS. Reliability and sensitivity of clinical contrast sensitivity tests. Clin Vis Sci. 1988;2:169–77. Rubin GS. Reliability and sensitivity of clinical contrast sensitivity tests. Clin Vis Sci. 1988;2:169–77.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Elliott DB, Sanderson K, Conkey A. The reliability of the Pelli-Robson contrast sensitivity chart. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1990;10:21–4.PubMedCrossRef Elliott DB, Sanderson K, Conkey A. The reliability of the Pelli-Robson contrast sensitivity chart. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1990;10:21–4.PubMedCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Elliott DB, Bullimore MA, Bailey IL. Improving the reliability of the Pelli-Robson contrast sensitivity test. Clin Vis Sci. 1991;6:471–5. Elliott DB, Bullimore MA, Bailey IL. Improving the reliability of the Pelli-Robson contrast sensitivity test. Clin Vis Sci. 1991;6:471–5.
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Cummings SR, Nevitt MC, Browner WS, Stone K, Fox KM, Ensrud KE, Cauley J, Black D, Vogt TM. Risk factors for hip fracture in white women. NEJM. 1995;332:767–73.PubMedCrossRef Cummings SR, Nevitt MC, Browner WS, Stone K, Fox KM, Ensrud KE, Cauley J, Black D, Vogt TM. Risk factors for hip fracture in white women. NEJM. 1995;332:767–73.PubMedCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Pedula K, Coleman A, Hillier T, Ensrud K, Nevitt M, Hochberg M, Mangione C. Visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, and mortality in older women: study of osteoporotic fractures. JAGS. 2006;54:1871–7.CrossRef Pedula K, Coleman A, Hillier T, Ensrud K, Nevitt M, Hochberg M, Mangione C. Visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, and mortality in older women: study of osteoporotic fractures. JAGS. 2006;54:1871–7.CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Swenor BK, Simonsick EM, Ferruncci L, Newman AB, Rubin S, Wilson V, Health ABCS. Visual impairment and incident mobility limitations: the health, aging and body composition study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2015;63:46–54.PubMedCrossRef Swenor BK, Simonsick EM, Ferruncci L, Newman AB, Rubin S, Wilson V, Health ABCS. Visual impairment and incident mobility limitations: the health, aging and body composition study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2015;63:46–54.PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Pearce E, Sivaprasad S, Chong NV. Factors affecting reading speed in patients with diabetic macular edema treated with laser photocoagulation. PLoS One. 2014;29:e105696.CrossRef Pearce E, Sivaprasad S, Chong NV. Factors affecting reading speed in patients with diabetic macular edema treated with laser photocoagulation. PLoS One. 2014;29:e105696.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Antin JF, Guo F, Fang Y, Dingus TA, Hankey JM, Perez MA. The influence of functional health on seniors’ driving risk. J Transp Health. 2017;6:237–44.CrossRef Antin JF, Guo F, Fang Y, Dingus TA, Hankey JM, Perez MA. The influence of functional health on seniors’ driving risk. J Transp Health. 2017;6:237–44.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Hohberger B, Laemmer R, Adler W, Juenemann AGM, Horn FK. Measuring contrast sensitivity in normal subjects with OPTEC 6500: influence of age and glare. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 2007;245:1805–14.PubMedCrossRef Hohberger B, Laemmer R, Adler W, Juenemann AGM, Horn FK. Measuring contrast sensitivity in normal subjects with OPTEC 6500: influence of age and glare. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 2007;245:1805–14.PubMedCrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Watson AB. Visual detection of spatial contrast patterns: evaluation of five simple models. Opt Express. 2000;6:12–33.PubMedCrossRef Watson AB. Visual detection of spatial contrast patterns: evaluation of five simple models. Opt Express. 2000;6:12–33.PubMedCrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Watson AB, Ahumada AJ. A standard model for foveal detection of spatial contrast. J Vis. 2005;5:717–40.PubMedCrossRef Watson AB, Ahumada AJ. A standard model for foveal detection of spatial contrast. J Vis. 2005;5:717–40.PubMedCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Pelli DG, Bex P. Measuring contrast sensitivity. Vis Res. 2013;2013:10–4.CrossRef Pelli DG, Bex P. Measuring contrast sensitivity. Vis Res. 2013;2013:10–4.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Dingus TA, Hankey J, Antin JF, Lee SE, Eichelberger L, Stulce K, McGraw D, Perez M, Stowe L. Naturalistic driving study: technical coordination and quality control. Washington DC: Transportation Research Board; 2014. (accessed 2019 May 17). https://doi.org/10.17226/22362CrossRef Dingus TA, Hankey J, Antin JF, Lee SE, Eichelberger L, Stulce K, McGraw D, Perez M, Stowe L. Naturalistic driving study: technical coordination and quality control. Washington DC: Transportation Research Board; 2014. (accessed 2019 May 17). https://​doi.​org/​10.​17226/​22362CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Committee for the Strategic Highway Research Program 2. Implementation TRB. Implementing the Results of the Second Strategic Highway Research Program: Saving Lives, Reducing Congestion, Improving Quality of Life. Washington DC: The National Academies; 2009. (accessed 2019 May 17). http://onlinepubs.trb.org/Onlinepubs/sr/sr296.pdf, 2009 Committee for the Strategic Highway Research Program 2. Implementation TRB. Implementing the Results of the Second Strategic Highway Research Program: Saving Lives, Reducing Congestion, Improving Quality of Life. Washington DC: The National Academies; 2009. (accessed 2019 May 17). http://​onlinepubs.​trb.​org/​Onlinepubs/​sr/​sr296.​pdf, 2009
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Hitchcock EM, Dick RB, Krieg EF. Visual contrast sensitivity testing: a comparison of two F.a.C.T. test types. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2004;26:271–7.PubMedCrossRef Hitchcock EM, Dick RB, Krieg EF. Visual contrast sensitivity testing: a comparison of two F.a.C.T. test types. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2004;26:271–7.PubMedCrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Chung ST, Legge GE. Comparing the shape of contrast sensitivity functions for normal and low vision. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2016;57:198–207.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Chung ST, Legge GE. Comparing the shape of contrast sensitivity functions for normal and low vision. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2016;57:198–207.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Lagarias JC, Reeds JA, Wright MH, Wright PE. Convergence properties of the Nelder-Mead simplex method in low dimensions. SIAM J Optim. 1998;9:112–47.CrossRef Lagarias JC, Reeds JA, Wright MH, Wright PE. Convergence properties of the Nelder-Mead simplex method in low dimensions. SIAM J Optim. 1998;9:112–47.CrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Stutts J, Feaganes J, Reinfurt D, Rodgman E, Hamlett C, Gish K, Staplin L. Driver’s exposure to distractions in their natural driving environment. Accid Anal Prev. 2005;37:1093–101.PubMedCrossRef Stutts J, Feaganes J, Reinfurt D, Rodgman E, Hamlett C, Gish K, Staplin L. Driver’s exposure to distractions in their natural driving environment. Accid Anal Prev. 2005;37:1093–101.PubMedCrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Hankey JM, Perez MA, McClafferty J. Naturalistic database and the crash, near-crash and baseline datasets. Blacksburg VA: Virginia Tech Transportation Institute; 2016. Hankey JM, Perez MA, McClafferty J. Naturalistic database and the crash, near-crash and baseline datasets. Blacksburg VA: Virginia Tech Transportation Institute; 2016.
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Dingus TA, Guo F, Lee SE, Antin JF, Perez M, Buchanan-King M, Hankey J. Driver crash risk factors and prevalence evaluation using naturalistic driving data. PNAS. 2016;113:2636–41.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral Dingus TA, Guo F, Lee SE, Antin JF, Perez M, Buchanan-King M, Hankey J. Driver crash risk factors and prevalence evaluation using naturalistic driving data. PNAS. 2016;113:2636–41.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral
53.
Zurück zum Zitat De Valois RL, Morgan H, Snodderly DM. Psychophysical studies of monkey vision-III. Spatial luminance contrast sensitivity tests of macaque and human observers. Vis Res. 1974;14:75–81.PubMedCrossRef De Valois RL, Morgan H, Snodderly DM. Psychophysical studies of monkey vision-III. Spatial luminance contrast sensitivity tests of macaque and human observers. Vis Res. 1974;14:75–81.PubMedCrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat McGwin G Jr, Chapman V, Owsley C. Visual risk factors for driving difficulty among older drivers. Accid Anal Prev. 2000;32:735–44.PubMedCrossRef McGwin G Jr, Chapman V, Owsley C. Visual risk factors for driving difficulty among older drivers. Accid Anal Prev. 2000;32:735–44.PubMedCrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Freeman EE, Munoz B, Turano KA, West SK. Measures of visual function and their association with driving modification in older adults. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2006;47:514–20.PubMedCrossRef Freeman EE, Munoz B, Turano KA, West SK. Measures of visual function and their association with driving modification in older adults. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2006;47:514–20.PubMedCrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Ball K, Owsley C, Stalvey B, Roenker DL, Sloane M, Graves M. Driving avoidance and functional impairment in older drivers. Accid Anal Prev. 1998;30:313–22.PubMedCrossRef Ball K, Owsley C, Stalvey B, Roenker DL, Sloane M, Graves M. Driving avoidance and functional impairment in older drivers. Accid Anal Prev. 1998;30:313–22.PubMedCrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Freeman EE, Munoz B, Turano K, West SK. Measures of visual function and time to driving cessation in older adults. Optom Vis Sci. 2005;82:765–73.PubMedCrossRef Freeman EE, Munoz B, Turano K, West SK. Measures of visual function and time to driving cessation in older adults. Optom Vis Sci. 2005;82:765–73.PubMedCrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Keay L, Munoz B, Turano KA, Hassan SE, Munro CA, Duncan DD, Baldwin K, Jasti S, Gower EW, West SK. Visual and cognitive deficits predict stopping or restricting driving: the Salisbury eye evaluation driving study (SEEDS). Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2009;50:107–13.PubMedCrossRef Keay L, Munoz B, Turano KA, Hassan SE, Munro CA, Duncan DD, Baldwin K, Jasti S, Gower EW, West SK. Visual and cognitive deficits predict stopping or restricting driving: the Salisbury eye evaluation driving study (SEEDS). Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2009;50:107–13.PubMedCrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Owsley C, McGwin G Jr, Sloane ME, Wells J, Stalvey BT, Gauthreaux S. Impact of cataract surgery on motor vehicle crash involvement by older adults. JAMA. 2002;288:841–9.PubMedCrossRef Owsley C, McGwin G Jr, Sloane ME, Wells J, Stalvey BT, Gauthreaux S. Impact of cataract surgery on motor vehicle crash involvement by older adults. JAMA. 2002;288:841–9.PubMedCrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Lachenmayr B, Berger J, Buser A, Keller O. Reduced visual capacity increases the risk of accidents in street traffic. Ophthalmologe. 1998;95:44–50.PubMedCrossRef Lachenmayr B, Berger J, Buser A, Keller O. Reduced visual capacity increases the risk of accidents in street traffic. Ophthalmologe. 1998;95:44–50.PubMedCrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat von Hebenstreit B. Visual acuity and traffic accident. Klin Monatsbl Augenheilkd. 1984;185:86–90.CrossRef von Hebenstreit B. Visual acuity and traffic accident. Klin Monatsbl Augenheilkd. 1984;185:86–90.CrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Kimlin JA, Black AA, Wood JM. Nighttime driving in older adults: effects of glare and association with mesopic visual function. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2017;58:2796–803.PubMedCrossRef Kimlin JA, Black AA, Wood JM. Nighttime driving in older adults: effects of glare and association with mesopic visual function. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2017;58:2796–803.PubMedCrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Black AA, Wood JM, Colorado LH, Collins MJ. The impact of uncorrected astigmatism on night driving performance. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2019;39:350–7.PubMedCrossRef Black AA, Wood JM, Colorado LH, Collins MJ. The impact of uncorrected astigmatism on night driving performance. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2019;39:350–7.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Association of Photopic and Mesopic Contrast Sensitivity in older drivers with risk of motor vehicle collision using naturalistic driving data
verfasst von
Cynthia Owsley
Thomas Swain
Rong Liu
Gerald McGwin Jr
Mi Young Kwon
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2020
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Ophthalmology / Ausgabe 1/2020
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2415
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12886-020-1331-7

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2020

BMC Ophthalmology 1/2020 Zur Ausgabe

Neu im Fachgebiet Augenheilkunde

Ophthalmika in der Schwangerschaft

Die Verwendung von Ophthalmika in der Schwangerschaft und Stillzeit stellt immer eine Off-label-Anwendung dar. Ein Einsatz von Arzneimitteln muss daher besonders sorgfältig auf sein Risiko-Nutzen-Verhältnis bewertet werden. In der vorliegenden …

Operative Therapie und Keimnachweis bei endogener Endophthalmitis

Vitrektomie Originalie

Die endogene Endophthalmitis ist eine hämatogen fortgeleitete, bakterielle oder fungale Infektion, die über choroidale oder retinale Gefäße in den Augapfel eingeschwemmt wird [ 1 – 3 ]. Von dort infiltrieren die Keime in die Netzhaut, den …

Bakterielle endogene Endophthalmitis

Vitrektomie Leitthema

Eine endogene Endophthalmitis stellt einen ophthalmologischen Notfall dar, der umgehender Diagnostik und Therapie bedarf. Es sollte mit geeigneten Methoden, wie beispielsweise dem Freiburger Endophthalmitis-Set, ein Keimnachweis erfolgen. Bei der …

So erreichen Sie eine bestmögliche Wundheilung der Kornea

Die bestmögliche Wundheilung der Kornea, insbesondere ohne die Ausbildung von lichtstreuenden Narben, ist oberstes Gebot, um einer dauerhaften Schädigung der Hornhaut frühzeitig entgegenzuwirken und die Funktion des Auges zu erhalten.   

Update Augenheilkunde

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.