Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation 1/2019

Open Access 01.12.2019 | Research

Complexity based measures of postural stability provide novel evidence of functional decline in fragile X premutation carriers

verfasst von: Clodagh O’Keeffe, Laura P. Taboada, Niamh Feerick, Louise Gallagher, Timothy Lynch, Richard B. Reilly

Erschienen in: Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation | Ausgabe 1/2019

Abstract

Background

Fragile X Associated Tremor/Ataxia Syndrome (FXTAS) is a neurodegenerative movement disorder characterized by tremor, ataxic gait, and balance issues resulting from a premutation of the Fragile X Mental Retardation 1 (FMR1) gene. No biomarkers have yet been identified to allow early diagnosis of FXTAS, however, recent studies have reported subtle issues in the stability of younger premutation carriers, before disease onset. This study investigates the efficacy of multiscale entropy analysis (MSE) in detecting early changes in the motor system of premutation carriers without FXTAS.

Methods

Sway complexity of 12 female Premutation carriers and 15 healthy Controls were measured under four conditions: eyes open, closed, and two dual-task conditions. A Sustained Attention Response Task (SART) and a working memory based N-Back task were employed to increase cognitive load while standing on the forceplate. A Complexity Index (Ci) was calculated for anterior-posterior (AP) and mediolateral (ML) sway. Independent t-tests were used to assess between-group differences and Oneway repeated measures ANOVA were used to assess within group differences with Bonferroni corrections to adjust for multiple comparisons.

Results

Group performances were comparable with eyes open and closed conditions. The Carrier group’s Ci was consistent across tasks and conditions while the Control group’s AP Ci increased significantly during the cognitive dual-task (p = 0.001). There was also a strong correlation between CGG repeat length and complexity for the Carrier group (p = 0.004).

Significance

Increased sway complexity is believed to stem from reallocation of attention to facilitate the increased cognitive demands of dual-tasks. Carriers’ complexity did not change during dual-tasks, possibly indicating capacity interference and inefficient division of attention. Lower sway complexity in carriers suggests diminished adaptive capacity under stress as well as degradation of motor functioning. Therefore, sway complexity may be a useful tool in identifying early functional decline in FMR1 premutation carriers as well as monitoring progression towards disease onset.
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
Ci
Complexity Index
EC
Eyes closed
EMD
Empirical mode decomposition
EO
Eyes open
FXTAS
Fragile X Associated Tremor/Ataxia Syndrome
IMF
Intrinsic mode function
MSE
Multiscale entropy analysis
SampleEn
Sample Entropy
SART
Sustained attention response task

Introduction

Fragile X Associated Tremor/Ataxia Syndrome (FXTAS) is a late-onset neurodegenerative movement disorder characterized by progressive cognitive decline, intention tremor, ataxic gait as well as impaired postural control and balance, which subsequently leads to increased risk of falls. Although highly penetrant, with 45% of males and 16% of female FMR1 premutation carriers over the age of 50 developing the disorder, as of yet, it is unknown why some carriers develop FXTAS while others do not [1, 2]. Unambiguous diagnosis of FXTAS and monitoring of disease progression relies on accurate assessment of movement and stability.
FXTAS has been associated with neurophysiological changes such as reduced cerebellar volume, and aberrant structural connectivity among the superior and middle cerebellar peduncles [35]. Such regions are heavily involved in the cortico-ponto-cerebellar feedback loop, imperative for adaptive postural control [6]. Emerging evidence has shown carriers both male and female carriers with FXTAS and those younger than the typical age of onset to exhibited subtle changes in postural stability [7] and that such changes were mediated by reduced cerebellar volume and disruption in vulnerable cerebellar circuits [8]. However, due to the increased prevalence and greater symptom severity of motor symptoms observed in male carriers, the motor phenotype associated with female carriers requires further exploration.
Emerging evidence has found, however, that impairments in postural control were exacerbated when conducting a concurrent cognitive task [911]. for example, female Premutation carriers without FXTAS, exhibit larger sway area during concurrent working memory tasks [9]. A similar study found not only that a dual-task resulted in greater attentional cost during the cognitive tasks but that the dual-task effect on the motor task was strongly predicted by working memory capacity [10, 11]. This suggests that postural control may be contingent on the attentional load of the cognitive task combined with individual differences in executive control. As postural control involves constant maintenance by higher order attentional systems and multisensory integration such as vestibular, visual, and somatosensory inputs [12, 13], changes or impairments any of these faculties, may alter stability. Manipulating these faculties through dual cognitive/motor tasks offers a sensitive approach to evaluate cognitive, attentional demands on motor control in FMR1 premutation.
Postural control is regulated through multisensory integration, various cognitive functions, and musculoskeletal outputs, communicating through spinal and supraspinal circuits, each operating over different timescales [12, 14]. Therefore the postural control system is intrinsically non-linear. The influence of each of these cognitive and sensory inputs and muscular outputs are constantly reattuned and reweighted, to maintain stability [14, 15]. These continuous adjustments and changes in control strategy generate seemingly spontaneous fluctuations in postural sway. Multiscale Entropy analysis (MSE) employs Sample Entropy (SampleEN) to quantify the degree of irregularity or ‘complexity’ in a time series across multiple scales. Postural sway complexity has been found to reflect an individual’s capability to respond and adapt to balance perturbations and stressors, such that greater postural sway complexity reflects a greater ability to adapt to such unexpected stressors [16, 17].
Measuring this physiological complexity of postural sway will reveal the integrity of the postural control system, and better characterize the impact of the FMR1 premutation on balance and stability, subsequently enabling the detection of early changes in the motor system of premutation carriers. A loss or reduction in complexity is believed to stem from the degradation of the physiological systems underpinning postural control or deterioration in the efficiently of communication between these systems [18].
The aim of the current study was, therefore, to examine postural control in a cohort of young female Fragile X premutation carriers by probing sensory and cognitive deficits previously reported to be impaired in younger carriers. This may then detect the subtle differences in postural stability and provide insight into the discriminating power of Entropy-based measures in order to identify parameter(s) which are sensitive to postural instability in the Fragile X premutation carriers. Previous studies have shown the degree of postural sway complexity to be largely independent of classical parameters [19]. Therefore, it was hypothesised that non-linear measures of postural sway will more accurately reflect the integrity of the postural control system in younger Fragile X Premutation Carriers. Specifically, it was hypothesised that Premutation Carriers would exhibit reduced sway complexity compared to the Control group and that the increased cognitive load associated with the dual-task paradigm would exacerbate differences in sway complexity in carriers.

Methods

Participants

Twelve female Fragile X Premutation Carriers (37–45 years) were recruited through advocacy groups and social media. Inclusion criteria for carriers included a positive genetic test for the FMR1 gene premutation. Fifteen Controls (28–47 years) were recruited through similar means. See Table 1 for summary demographics. All subjects had normal or corrected to normal vision and no intellectual disability as assessed using the Weschler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (FSIQ> 70) [20]. Subjects were excluded from the Control group if a first-degree relative had a neurological condition. No subject reported any transient, neurological or musculoskeletal problem that may affect balance. Informed consent was obtained from all participants and all procedures followed were in accordance with ethical requirements of the Tallaght Hospital and St. James’s Hospital Joint Research Ethics Committee as well as the School of Medicine Ethics Committee, Trinity College Dublin.
Table 1
Summary of descriptive data
 
12 PM carriers
Mean (SD)
15 Healthy Controls
Mean (SD)
P-value
Age
41.2
(3.12)
37.1
(5.5)
0.012*
Education (years)
17.7
(3.7)
19.2
(2.5)
0.214
FSIQa
109.6
(16.3)
118.7
(14.81)
0.14
Verbal comprehensiona
114
(15.28)
123
(12.6.)
0.119
Perceptual reasoninga
102.4
(16.24)
114.2
(19.66)
0.108
Weight (Kg)
75.1
(20.77)
65.8
(9.51)
0.136
CGG repeat length
83.1
(8.12)
SART
 Correct (%)
89.44
(7.4)
89.84
(10.0)
0.909
 No. Of Errors
6.45
(4.89)
6.6
(7.5)
0.663
N-backb
 Correct (%)
91.36
(11.3)
90.44
(10.26)
0.826
 No. Of Errors
3.17
(3.43)
2.6
(3.22)
0.956
*p < 0.05
aParameters derived from the WASI-II
bn = 11 for PM carriers

Procedure

Subjects stood, without shoes, in a comfortable bilateral stance near the center of the force platform with arms by their sides. To ensure consistency between trials, an outline of their feet was made on the platform surface. An eye-level visual reference was presented on a screen at a distance of 1.5 m. Subjects were advised to be aware of their balance and to stand as still as possible for the duration of the trial. Three 90-s trials were conducted with eyes open (EO) and three 90-s trials were carried out with eyes closed (EC). Two dual-task conditions were completed while standing: a three-minute letter variant N-back task [21] and a number variant Sustained Attention Response Task (SART) [22]. During the N-back task, participants were told to respond when the number on screen was the same as the number presented immediately previous (1-Back) while the SART involved displaying the numbers 0 to 9 on screen while participants responded for each number except the number three. The duration of this task was 5 min and subjects were reminded to stay as still as possible during both dual-tasks. Both tasks required subjects to provide a response using a handheld touchpad. The order of the cognitive tasks was randomised, with EO and EC trials repeated between each cognitive task. Subjects were encouraged to take a break between trials to avoid fatigue. Force plate data were acquired after the completion of each task trial to avoid any detectable end effects. The first and last 15 s of each EO and EC trial were removed to avoid initial transient, anticipatory and impact effects. Although the N-back and SART had a duration of 3 and 5 min respectively, only the second 60 s of each cognitive task was used for analyses, to ensure each time series was of equal length as well as to mitigate practice and fatigue-related effects.

Data processing and analysis

Postural data was collected using a Biosignals Plux Forceplate (Plux Wireless Biosignals, S.A. Portugal) at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz. The reaction force generated from the application of pressure to each of the four cells of the forceplate was converted to cartesian coordinates, each relating to directional subcomponents (Y-axis = Anterior-Posterior(AP); X-axis = Medio-Lateral(ML)).

Classical parameters of sway

Postural sway was characterized using Classical sway parameters such as path length, sway area (as measured by a 95% confidence ellipse), VelocityAP and VelocityML. Data collected from the forceplate were filtered using a 4th order low-pass Butterworth filter with a cut off of 10 Hz and downsampled to 50 Hz before sway parameters were calculated [23]. All analyses were carried out using Matlab_R2016a (The Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA).

Multiscale entropy analysis

To quantify postural complexity, both the AP and ML subcomponents of the time series were subjected to MSE analysis. Prior to MSE analysis, the forceplate data was downsampled to 250 Hz and detrended using empirical mode decomposition (EMD) [24]. The lowest frequency suitable for inclusion in the analyses was determined using the algorithmic procedure outlined by Gow et al. [25]. High-frequency noise (above 20 Hz) and low-frequency trends (below 0.8 Hz) were filtered to ensure a sufficient number of physiologically meaningful dynamic patterns were available for analysis [16, 26]. To capture system dynamics over multiple time scales, a ‘course graining’ procedure was used to divide the time series into non-overlapping windows of length equal to a scaling factor Ƭ, ranging from 1 to 40 data points. As the second 60 s epoch of each task was used for analyses, at the largest scale, each course-grained series contained 375 data points (15000 points/40). Based on previous recommendations, SampleEn was computed for each course grained time series, where m = 2 and r = 15% [16].
To construct MSE curves, SampleEn was plotted as a function of time-scale Ƭ. The postural sway complexity Index (Ci) was measured as the area under the MSE curve, such that higher SampleEn values over multiple time series results in larger area, which reflects greater complexity (see Fig. 1). All data processing was carried out using Matlab R2016b (Mathworks, Natick, MA; [27]) and the Waveform database V.0.9.10 (Wfdb toolbox, [28]).

Statistical analysis

A Shapiro-Wilk test of normality determined that the distribution of the data. Mann-Whitney U tests were used to compare mean performances of classical parameters between both groups and the Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used to conduct within-group comparisons, with Bonferroni corrections to adjust for multiple comparisons. Independent t-tests were used to assess between-group differences and Oneway repeated measures ANOVA were used to assess within group differences with Bonferroni corrections to adjust for multiple comparisons, thus reducing the significance level to p < 0.007. Pearsons correlations were used to assess the relationship between age and repeat length, and outcome variables.

Results

Both groups were comparable in terms of demographic data. The Premutation group was significantly older than the Control group, (p = 0.012). Data from only 10 Premutation Carriers were available for the N-back task due to an error in recording. There was no significant difference between Premutation Carriers and Controls in terms of their performance during either SART or N-back task (Table 1).

Classical parameters

There were no differences between groups in path length, sway area, or velocity in either ML or AP direction, during the EO and EC conditions (p > 0.05). Both groups were also comparable in terms of classical sway parameter during both the SART and N-back tasks (p > 0.05 for all comparisons). Within-group analysis also did not reveal a significant change in sway area or velocity between the baseline EO condition and either dual-task conditions (p > 0.05 for both). Path Length was found to increase during the EC condition of the Control group (p = 0.001) and the Premutation group (p = 0.005). There was also a strong correlation between the error rate during the SART task and sway velocity in the ML direction for the Carrier group, whereby velocity increased with the number of errors committed (r = 0.672 p = 0.01).

Complexity index

An independent sample t-test revealed no difference in Ci between groups either with EO or EC in the AP or ML direction (p > .05). Ci of both groups were also also comparable during the SART task (AP: t (25)= − 1.297, p = .184, d = .67; ML: t (25) =0.601, p = .553, d = .23). Although the mean N-back AP Ci of the control group was greater than that of the PM group, this difference did not reach significance, (t (25)= − 1.774, p = .089, d = .72) See Table 2.
Table 2
Complexity indices of premutation carriers and control subjects across conditions
 
EO
Mean (STD)
EC
Mean (STD)
N-Back
Mean (STD)
SART
Mean (STD)
AP
ML
AP
ML
AP
ML
AP
ML
PM
26.2 (6.7)
24.6 (5.71)
27.6 (4.78)
28.7 (6.78)
29.532 (4.8)
25.377 (5.68)
27.31 (5.13)
30.04 (5.79)
Ctrl
24.8 (8.02)
26.43 (4.63)
28.5 (4.4)
27.7 (6.25)
33.9 (7.96)
29.33 (8.21)
30.88 (8.33)
28.24 (3.96)
p-value
0.64
0.38
0.62
0.71
0.09
0.19
0.18
0.55
PM Premtuation carriers, Ctrl Controls, STD Standard deviation
*p < 0.05
Within-group analysis did not reveal a change in AP nor ML postural stability complexity between the EO and EC condition for Premutation Carriers or Controls. The ML complexity index of Premutation Carriers remained consistent across each task (F (3, 27)= 2.1,p = .124, ηp2 = .19). Although the Carriers’ ML Ci increased during the N-back task compared to EO (p = .029, d = 0.41), this did not survive correction for multiple comparisons. The Control group, however, did exhibit a change in complexity across conditions an increase in AP Ci during the N-Back task (p = .001, d = 0.9). See Fig. 2. The ML Ci of the Control group did not differ significantly from the EO condition (F(3,42) = .509, p = .67, ηp2 = .1).

Correlations

Pearson’s Correlations were carried out to assess the relationship between performance on the cognitive dual-tasks and the complexity of postural sway. During the SART task, there was a strong positive correlation between AP Ci of carriers and the number correct responses (r = 0.71, p = 0.014) No such correlation observed in the Control group or ML Ci for Premutation Carriers. There was no relationship found between the number of correct responses or error rate during the N-back task and Ci of Premutation Carriers in either AP or ML direction (p > 0.05 for all correlations).
Pearson’s Correlations revealed a strong inverse correlation between CGG repeat length and ML Ci during the N-back task, where Complexity decreased as repeat length increased (r = − 0.819, p = 0.004.) See Fig. 3. There was no correlation found between age and Ci of either the Premutation or the Control group.

Discussion

This study employs multiscale entropy-based measures of postural complexity in FMR1 Premutation Carriers, across specifically designed balance tasks, including manipulated sensory and cognitive conditions. The current study aimed to employ a unique analysis of postural control in young female Fragile X Premutation Carriers to detect subtle differences and trends in postural stability which may distinguish younger Carriers from Control subjects. The findings of this study indicate differences between groups in the complexity index of postural sway, particularly with regard to dual-task interference, which were not readily apparent using only classical sway parameters. Additionally, the study also reaffirms the efficacy of the dual-task paradigm in distinguishing age or genetically modulated changes in stability of Premutation Carriers which may be useful in predicting the onset of FXTAS.
Stability is based on complex interactions between the individual, the environment, and the task at hand [14]. During the eyes closed condition, reducing the visual information available to subjects did not reveal a change in sway complexity when compared with standing with eyes open. This suggests removing visual cues of stability did not produce a striking effect on the complexity of postural control systems, which reflects the findings of previous studies involving healthy subjects [29]. Classical sway parameters did reveal an increase in path length of both groups during the EC conditions. Taken together, the results of both Complexity based measures and the classical parameters suggest that although balance was challenged by reduced visual input, both groups were equally capable of adapting to this increased demand on the postural control systems.
Classical measures of sway did not exhibit changes in balance or stability during either of the dual-task conditions. This is contrary to emerging reports on postural control in female Fragile X Premutation Carriers [9]. This may be due to the smaller sample size involved in this study or perhaps classical parameters were not sufficiently sensitive to detect subtle subclinical changes in stability. While MSE analysis revealed that the complexity of the Premutation group was not significantly different from that of Controls, complexity of Carriers’ sway was less during the working memory based dual-task conditions. Such findings reported in other studies in similar cohorts, which have associated lower sway complexity with diminished capacity to adapt to environmental and cognitive stressors [16, 30], as well as being linked to degradation of motor functionality including slow gait, frailty, and increased falls [31], all of which are common symptoms of FXTAS [32].
Increasing cognitive load with a dual-task paradigm adds stress to the postural control system. Performance of a secondary cognitive task causes a shift or division of attention between multiple competing demands, which resulted in a change in the postural complexity in the Control group during the dual-task condition. This interpretation of change in complexity during dual-task is in line with the “facilitatory-control” view of dual-task complexity [33]. This states that an increase in randomness of postural sway may stem from the reallocation of attention to facilitate the supraspinal cortical tasks, such as letter recall or sustaining attention as in the case of this study. The Ci of the Control group increased during the working memory task, perhaps indicating the recruitment of additional attentional/postural resources to cope with the increase in cognitive demand during the dual-task [16]. The Premutation group did not, however, show a change in complexity compared to baseline measures, suggesting a reduction in capacity to adapt to the increase in cognitive demand during the dual-task. However, as there was no difference between groups in terms of cognitive performance, perhaps, indicating the carrier group prioritised performance of the cognitive task over postural performance. This may suggest an inability to divide attention efficiently between both tasks. Similar dual-task related impairments were reported in relation to gait parameters [34], balance and postural control [9] in younger carriers while carrying out memory-based verbal fluency task. Such results may be indicative of capacity interference in Premutation Carriers, whereby several competing demands contend for limited attentional resources. Similar dual-task methodologies in other cohorts have shown that a secondary cognitive task diverts attention from the primary motor task, compounding impairments in already weakened systems [13].
The differences in postural sway complexity, particularly during dual-tasks may, therefore, serve as a marker by which to distinguish Premutation Carriers from unaffected Control subjects. However, large-scale longitudinal studies would be necessary to determine whether these changes in complexity are the result of stable neurodevelopmental changes caused by the neurotoxic effect of the FMR1 premutation or are indicative of progressive neurodegeneration. Further exploration would be required to establish the efficacy of postural sway complexity as a biomarker for FXTAS.
The results of this study did not reveal age-modulated effect on complexity scores of Premutation Carriers during either single or dual-task conditions. This is not consistent with previous findings that carriers experience worsening of symptoms with age [1, 35, 36]. This is also not in line with the complexity theory of aging, which states that disease and age diminish the quality and/or quantity of inputs that regulate system behavior, subsequently reducing system functionality [30]. These antithetical findings may be due to the modest sample size employed in this study and the relatively narrow age range of the participants who took part.
Our results do, however, indicate a link between CGG repeat length and postural complexity. This is in line with previous findings which suggest phenotype severity increases with repeat expansions [9, 32, 37]. Higher CGG repeat lengths are associated with increased levels of FMR1 mRNA, the proposed cause of neuropathology seen in FXTAS [38]. Larger repeat expansions have also been linked with a reduction in FMRP levels, a protein vital for neuronal maturation and plasticity. It is believed that decreased levels of FMRP found in Premutation Carriers, impacts cognitive development, therefore, underlining many of the cognitive issues associated with the premutation. The severity of the FMR1 phenotype has also been linked with activation ratio in female carriers (the ratio of cells with the normal allele present on the active X chromosome [39]. Higher activation ratios have been found to have protective or compensatory effects on both cognitive and motor symptoms in carriers. Future research would benefit from taking activation ration data into account as this may shed additional light on such findings.

Conclusion

To conclude, the current study revealed novel evidence of differences in the physiological complexity of postural stability in female Premutation Carriers. Most importantly, the preliminary findings of this study demonstrate the feasibility and subtlety of multiscale entropy analysis in identifying distinguishing features of functional decline in younger Premutation Carriers, which may not be distinguishable by classical sway parameters. Given the primary motor features of FXTAS include balance and gait issues, early detection of these motor changes will be vital for the detecting carriers who may be at risk of FXTAS, and monitoring progression before the onset of the disorder.

Acknowledgements

The authors of this study would like to offer their sincere gratitude to all participants for their contribution to this research.
Informed consent was obtained from all participants and all procedures followed were in accordance with ethical requirements of the Tallaght University Hospital and St. James’s Hospital Joint Research Ethics Committee as well as the School of Medicine Ethics Committee, Trinity College Dublin.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Jacquemont S, Hagerman RJ, Leehey MA, Hall DA, Levine RA, Brunberg JA, et al. Penetrance of the fragile X–associated tremor/ataxia syndrome in a premutation carrier population. JAMA. 2004;291(4):460–9.PubMedCrossRef Jacquemont S, Hagerman RJ, Leehey MA, Hall DA, Levine RA, Brunberg JA, et al. Penetrance of the fragile X–associated tremor/ataxia syndrome in a premutation carrier population. JAMA. 2004;291(4):460–9.PubMedCrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Rodriguez-Revenga L, Madrigal I, Pagonabarraga J, Xunclà M, Badenas C, Kulisevsky J, et al. Penetrance of FMR1 premutation associated pathologies in fragile X syndrome families. Eur J Hum Genet. 2009;17(10):1359–62.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Rodriguez-Revenga L, Madrigal I, Pagonabarraga J, Xunclà M, Badenas C, Kulisevsky J, et al. Penetrance of FMR1 premutation associated pathologies in fragile X syndrome families. Eur J Hum Genet. 2009;17(10):1359–62.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang JY, Hessl D, Hagerman RJ, Simon TJ, Tassone F, Ferrer E, et al. Abnormal trajectories in cerebellum and brainstem volumes in carriers of the fragile X premutation. Neurobiol Aging. 2017;55:11–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wang JY, Hessl D, Hagerman RJ, Simon TJ, Tassone F, Ferrer E, et al. Abnormal trajectories in cerebellum and brainstem volumes in carriers of the fragile X premutation. Neurobiol Aging. 2017;55:11–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen S, Masyn K, Adams J, Hessl D, Rivera S, Tassone F, et al. Molecular and imaging correlates of the fragile X–associated tremor/ataxia syndrome. Neurology. 2006;67(8):1426–31.PubMedCrossRef Cohen S, Masyn K, Adams J, Hessl D, Rivera S, Tassone F, et al. Molecular and imaging correlates of the fragile X–associated tremor/ataxia syndrome. Neurology. 2006;67(8):1426–31.PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang JY, Hessl D, Hagerman RJ, Tassone F, Rivera SM. Age-dependent structural connectivity effects in fragile x premutation. Arch Neurol. 2012;69(4):482–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wang JY, Hessl D, Hagerman RJ, Tassone F, Rivera SM. Age-dependent structural connectivity effects in fragile x premutation. Arch Neurol. 2012;69(4):482–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Keefe JA, Robertson-Dick E, Dunn EJ, Li Y, Deng Y, Fiutko AN, et al. Characterization and early detection of balance deficits in fragile X premutation carriers with and without fragile X-associated tremor/ataxia syndrome (FXTAS). Cerebellum. 2015;14(6):650–62.PubMedCrossRef O’Keefe JA, Robertson-Dick E, Dunn EJ, Li Y, Deng Y, Fiutko AN, et al. Characterization and early detection of balance deficits in fragile X premutation carriers with and without fragile X-associated tremor/ataxia syndrome (FXTAS). Cerebellum. 2015;14(6):650–62.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Hocking DR, Birch RC, Bui QM, Menant JC, Lord SR, Georgiou-Karistianis N, et al. Cerebellar volume mediates the relationship between FMR1 mRNA levels and voluntary step initiation in males with the premutation. Neurobiol Aging. 2017;50:5–12.PubMedCrossRef Hocking DR, Birch RC, Bui QM, Menant JC, Lord SR, Georgiou-Karistianis N, et al. Cerebellar volume mediates the relationship between FMR1 mRNA levels and voluntary step initiation in males with the premutation. Neurobiol Aging. 2017;50:5–12.PubMedCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Kraan CM, Hocking DR, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Metcalfe SA, Archibald AD, Fielding J, et al. Cognitive-motor interference during postural control indicates at-risk cerebellar profiles in females with the FMR1 premutation. Behav Brain Res. 2013;253:329–36.PubMedCrossRef Kraan CM, Hocking DR, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Metcalfe SA, Archibald AD, Fielding J, et al. Cognitive-motor interference during postural control indicates at-risk cerebellar profiles in females with the FMR1 premutation. Behav Brain Res. 2013;253:329–36.PubMedCrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Kraan CM, Hocking DR, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Metcalfe SA, Archibald AD, Fielding J, et al. Age and CGG-repeat length are associated with neuromotor impairments in at-risk females with the FMR1 premutation. Neurobiol Aging. 2014;35(9):2179. e7–e13.CrossRef Kraan CM, Hocking DR, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Metcalfe SA, Archibald AD, Fielding J, et al. Age and CGG-repeat length are associated with neuromotor impairments in at-risk females with the FMR1 premutation. Neurobiol Aging. 2014;35(9):2179. e7–e13.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Hocking DR, Kraan CM, Godler DE, Bui QM, Li X, Bradshaw JL, et al. Evidence linking FMR1 mRNA and attentional demands of stepping and postural control in women with the premutation. Neurobiol Aging. 2015;36(3):1400–8.PubMedCrossRef Hocking DR, Kraan CM, Godler DE, Bui QM, Li X, Bradshaw JL, et al. Evidence linking FMR1 mRNA and attentional demands of stepping and postural control in women with the premutation. Neurobiol Aging. 2015;36(3):1400–8.PubMedCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A. Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture. 2002;16(1):1–14.PubMedCrossRef Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A. Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture. 2002;16(1):1–14.PubMedCrossRef
14.
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Peterka R. Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88(3):1097–118.PubMedCrossRef Peterka R. Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88(3):1097–118.PubMedCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Costa M, Goldberger AL, Peng C-K. Multiscale entropy analysis of complex physiologic time series. Phys Rev Lett. 2002;89(6):068102.PubMedCrossRef Costa M, Goldberger AL, Peng C-K. Multiscale entropy analysis of complex physiologic time series. Phys Rev Lett. 2002;89(6):068102.PubMedCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Manor B, Lipsitz LA. Physiologic complexity and aging: implications for physical function and rehabilitation. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2013;45:287–93.CrossRef Manor B, Lipsitz LA. Physiologic complexity and aging: implications for physical function and rehabilitation. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2013;45:287–93.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Lipsitz LA. Dynamics of stability: the physiologic basis of functional health and frailty. J Gerontol Ser A Biol Med Sci. 2002;57(3):B115–B25.CrossRef Lipsitz LA. Dynamics of stability: the physiologic basis of functional health and frailty. J Gerontol Ser A Biol Med Sci. 2002;57(3):B115–B25.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Manor B, Costa MD, Hu K, Newton E, Starobinets OV, Kang HG, et al. Physiological complexity and system adaptability: evidence from postural control dynamics of older adults. Am J Phys Heart Circ Phys. 2010;109(6):1786-91. Manor B, Costa MD, Hu K, Newton E, Starobinets OV, Kang HG, et al. Physiological complexity and system adaptability: evidence from postural control dynamics of older adults. Am J Phys Heart Circ Phys. 2010;109(6):1786-91.
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D. WASI (Wechsler adult scale–reduced). New York: The Psychological Corporation; 1999. Wechsler D. WASI (Wechsler adult scale–reduced). New York: The Psychological Corporation; 1999.
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Kirchner WK. Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. J Exp Psychol. 1958;55(4):352.PubMedCrossRef Kirchner WK. Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. J Exp Psychol. 1958;55(4):352.PubMedCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Robertson IH, Manly T, Andrade J, Baddeley BT, Yiend J. ‘Oops!’: performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35(6):747–58.PubMedCrossRef Robertson IH, Manly T, Andrade J, Baddeley BT, Yiend J. ‘Oops!’: performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35(6):747–58.PubMedCrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Scoppa F, Capra R, Gallamini M, Shiffer R. Clinical stabilometry standardization: basic definitions–acquisition interval–sampling frequency. Gait Posture. 2013;37(2):290–2.PubMedCrossRef Scoppa F, Capra R, Gallamini M, Shiffer R. Clinical stabilometry standardization: basic definitions–acquisition interval–sampling frequency. Gait Posture. 2013;37(2):290–2.PubMedCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Huang NE, Shen Z, Long SR, Wu MC, Shih HH, Zheng Q, et al. The empirical mode decomposition and the Hilbert spectrum for nonlinear and non-stationary time series analysis. In: Proceedings of the Royal Society of London A: mathematical, physical and engineering sciences: The Royal Society; 1998. Huang NE, Shen Z, Long SR, Wu MC, Shih HH, Zheng Q, et al. The empirical mode decomposition and the Hilbert spectrum for nonlinear and non-stationary time series analysis. In: Proceedings of the Royal Society of London A: mathematical, physical and engineering sciences: The Royal Society; 1998.
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Gow BJ, Peng C-K, Wayne PM, Ahn AC. Multiscale entropy analysis of center-of-pressure dynamics in human postural control: methodological considerations. Entropy. 2015;17(12):7926–47.CrossRef Gow BJ, Peng C-K, Wayne PM, Ahn AC. Multiscale entropy analysis of center-of-pressure dynamics in human postural control: methodological considerations. Entropy. 2015;17(12):7926–47.CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhou J, Lipsitz L, Habtemariam D, Manor B. Sub-sensory vibratory noise augments the physiologic complexity of postural control in older adults. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2016;13(1):44.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Zhou J, Lipsitz L, Habtemariam D, Manor B. Sub-sensory vibratory noise augments the physiologic complexity of postural control in older adults. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2016;13(1):44.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Silvia I, Moody G. An open-source toolbox for analysing and processing PhysioNet databases in MATLAB and octave. J Open Res Softw. 2014;2(1):27. Silvia I, Moody G. An open-source toolbox for analysing and processing PhysioNet databases in MATLAB and octave. J Open Res Softw. 2014;2(1):27.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Goldberger A, Amaral L, L G, Hausdorff J, PCh I, Mark R, et al. PhysioBank, PhysioToolkit, and PhysioNet: components of a new research resource for complex physiologic signals. Circulation. 2000;101(23):215–20.CrossRef Goldberger A, Amaral L, L G, Hausdorff J, PCh I, Mark R, et al. PhysioBank, PhysioToolkit, and PhysioNet: components of a new research resource for complex physiologic signals. Circulation. 2000;101(23):215–20.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Sasaki O, Usami S-i, Gagey P-M, Martinerie J, Le Van Quyen M, Arranz P. Role of visual input in nonlinear postural control system. Exp Brain Res. 2002;147(1):1–7.PubMedCrossRef Sasaki O, Usami S-i, Gagey P-M, Martinerie J, Le Van Quyen M, Arranz P. Role of visual input in nonlinear postural control system. Exp Brain Res. 2002;147(1):1–7.PubMedCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Lipsitz LA, Goldberger AL. Loss of'complexity'and aging: potential applications of fractals and chaos theory to senescence. JAMA. 1992;267(13):1806–9.PubMedCrossRef Lipsitz LA, Goldberger AL. Loss of'complexity'and aging: potential applications of fractals and chaos theory to senescence. JAMA. 1992;267(13):1806–9.PubMedCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Kang HG, Costa MD, Priplata AA, Starobinets OV, Goldberger AL, Peng C-K, et al. Frailty and the degradation of complex balance dynamics during a dual-task protocol. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64(12):1304–11.PubMedCrossRef Kang HG, Costa MD, Priplata AA, Starobinets OV, Goldberger AL, Peng C-K, et al. Frailty and the degradation of complex balance dynamics during a dual-task protocol. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64(12):1304–11.PubMedCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Keefe JA, Robertson-Dick EE, Hall DA, Berry-Kravis E. Gait and functional mobility deficits in fragile X-associated tremor/ataxia syndrome. Cerebellum. 2016;15(4):475–82.PubMedCrossRef O’Keefe JA, Robertson-Dick EE, Hall DA, Berry-Kravis E. Gait and functional mobility deficits in fragile X-associated tremor/ataxia syndrome. Cerebellum. 2016;15(4):475–82.PubMedCrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Stoffregen TA, Smart LJ, Bardy BG, Pagulayan RJ. Postural stabilization of looking. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1999;25(6):1641.CrossRef Stoffregen TA, Smart LJ, Bardy BG, Pagulayan RJ. Postural stabilization of looking. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1999;25(6):1641.CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Hocking DR, Kogan CS, Cornish KM. Selective spatial processing deficits in an at-risk subgroup of the fragile X premutation. Brain Cogn. 2012;79(1):39–44.PubMedCrossRef Hocking DR, Kogan CS, Cornish KM. Selective spatial processing deficits in an at-risk subgroup of the fragile X premutation. Brain Cogn. 2012;79(1):39–44.PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Narcisa V, Aguilar D, Nguyen DV, Campos L, Brodovsky J, White S, et al. A quantitative assessment of tremor and ataxia in female FMR1 premutation carriers using CATSYS. Curr Gerontol Geriatr Res. 2011;2011:484713.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Narcisa V, Aguilar D, Nguyen DV, Campos L, Brodovsky J, White S, et al. A quantitative assessment of tremor and ataxia in female FMR1 premutation carriers using CATSYS. Curr Gerontol Geriatr Res. 2011;2011:484713.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Allen EG, Juncos J, Letz R, Rusin M, Hamilton D, Novak G, et al. Detection of early FXTAS motor symptoms using the CATSYS computerised neuromotor test battery. J Med Genet. 2008;45(5):290–7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Allen EG, Juncos J, Letz R, Rusin M, Hamilton D, Novak G, et al. Detection of early FXTAS motor symptoms using the CATSYS computerised neuromotor test battery. J Med Genet. 2008;45(5):290–7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Hagerman R, Hagerman P. Advances in clinical and molecular understanding of the FMR1 premutation and fragile X-associated tremor/ataxia syndrome. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12(8):786–98.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hagerman R, Hagerman P. Advances in clinical and molecular understanding of the FMR1 premutation and fragile X-associated tremor/ataxia syndrome. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12(8):786–98.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Leehey M, Berry-Kravis E, Goetz C, Zhang L, Hall D, Li L, et al. FMR1 CGG repeat length predicts motor dysfunction in premutation carriers. Neurology. 2008;70(16 Part 2):1397–402.PubMedCrossRef Leehey M, Berry-Kravis E, Goetz C, Zhang L, Hall D, Li L, et al. FMR1 CGG repeat length predicts motor dysfunction in premutation carriers. Neurology. 2008;70(16 Part 2):1397–402.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Complexity based measures of postural stability provide novel evidence of functional decline in fragile X premutation carriers
verfasst von
Clodagh O’Keeffe
Laura P. Taboada
Niamh Feerick
Louise Gallagher
Timothy Lynch
Richard B. Reilly
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2019
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation / Ausgabe 1/2019
Elektronische ISSN: 1743-0003
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-019-0560-6

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2019

Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation 1/2019 Zur Ausgabe

Neu in den Fachgebieten Neurologie und Psychiatrie

Akuter Schwindel: Wann lohnt sich eine MRT?

28.04.2024 Schwindel Nachrichten

Akuter Schwindel stellt oft eine diagnostische Herausforderung dar. Wie nützlich dabei eine MRT ist, hat eine Studie aus Finnland untersucht. Immerhin einer von sechs Patienten wurde mit akutem ischämischem Schlaganfall diagnostiziert.

Niedriger diastolischer Blutdruck erhöht Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Komplikationen

25.04.2024 Hypotonie Nachrichten

Wenn unter einer medikamentösen Hochdrucktherapie der diastolische Blutdruck in den Keller geht, steigt das Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Ereignisse: Darauf deutet eine Sekundäranalyse der SPRINT-Studie hin.

Frühe Alzheimertherapie lohnt sich

25.04.2024 AAN-Jahrestagung 2024 Nachrichten

Ist die Tau-Last noch gering, scheint der Vorteil von Lecanemab besonders groß zu sein. Und beginnen Erkrankte verzögert mit der Behandlung, erreichen sie nicht mehr die kognitive Leistung wie bei einem früheren Start. Darauf deuten neue Analysen der Phase-3-Studie Clarity AD.

Viel Bewegung in der Parkinsonforschung

25.04.2024 Parkinson-Krankheit Nachrichten

Neue arznei- und zellbasierte Ansätze, Frühdiagnose mit Bewegungssensoren, Rückenmarkstimulation gegen Gehblockaden – in der Parkinsonforschung tut sich einiges. Auf dem Deutschen Parkinsonkongress ging es auch viel um technische Innovationen.