Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Implementation Science 1/2014

Open Access 01.12.2014 | Research

Discriminant content validity of a theoretical domains framework questionnaire for use in implementation research

verfasst von: Johanna M Huijg, Winifred A Gebhardt, Mathilde R Crone, Elise Dusseldorp, Justin Presseau

Erschienen in: Implementation Science | Ausgabe 1/2014

Abstract

Background

To improve the implementation of innovations in healthcare settings, it is important to understand factors influencing healthcare professionals’ behaviors. We aimed to develop a generic questionnaire in English and in Dutch assessing the 14 domains of behavioral determinants from the revised TDF (Cane et al., 2012) that can be tailored to suit different targets, actions, contexts, and times of interest, and to investigate questionnaire items’ discriminant content validity.

Methods

We identified existing questionnaires including items assessing constructs within TDF domains and developed new items where needed. Nineteen judges allocated 79 items to one or more TDF domains. One-sample t-tests were used to examine the discriminant content validity of each item, i.e., whether items measured intended domains or whether items measured a combination of domains.

Results

We identified items judged to discriminately measure 11 out of 14 domains. Items measuring the domains Reinforcement, Goals, and Behavioral regulation were judged to measure a combination of domains.

Conclusions

We have developed a questionnaire in English and in Dutch able to discriminately assess the majority of TDF domains. The results partly support Cane et al.’s (2012) 14-domain validation of the TDF and suggest that Michie et al.’s (2005) 12-domain original version might be more applicable in developing a TDF-based questionnaire. The identified items provide a robust basis for developing a questionnaire to measure TDF-based determinants of healthcare professionals’ implementation behaviors to suit different targets, actions, contexts, and times. Future research should investigate the concurrent and predictive validity and reliability of such a questionnaire in practice.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1748-5908-9-11) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

JP and JMH are equally responsible for the design of the study, development of the questionnaire, and data analysis and interpretation. JMH collected the data and wrote the initial draft of the manuscript. Subsequent drafts of the manuscript were written together. WAG and MRC were involved in the development of the questionnaire and critically revised the manuscript. ED was involved in data analysis and interpretation, and commented on the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Abkürzungen
PA
Physical activity
TDF
Theoretical domains framework
DCV
Discriminant content validation
DIBQ
Determinants of implementation behavior questionnaire

Background

Healthcare professionals routinely deliver pharmacological and behavior change interventions to their patients to promote health and prevent disease. However, as the evidence-base for effective interventions is continuously developing, the transfer of such evidence into routine practice often does not happen as desired[13]. For example, primary care-based interventions for increasing physical activity (PA) are effective[47], yet rates of PA counseling by healthcare professionals are suboptimal[8, 9], as is the fidelity of delivery of PA interventions[2, 10, 11]. This gap between research and practice reduces the impact that effective behavior change interventions can have on public health[12, 13]. Implementation research aims to bridge this gap by investigating methods to promote healthcare professionals’ uptake of research findings, including the study of factors influencing healthcare professional behavior[14, 15].
Improving the adoption and implementation of evidence-based interventions into routine practice involves changes in healthcare professionals’ behaviors that may be influenced by a range of individual, organizational, and social factors[1620]. Identifying the key factors associated with healthcare professional behavior can provide a basis for developing interventions to help healthcare professionals to use research findings more effectively[14]. Given the range of potential factors associated with behavior, many advocate the use of theory to guide the selection of factors to investigate[15, 2123]. In addition, the UK Medical Research Council guidance on developing and evaluating complex interventions recommends the use of theory in the intervention development phase[24]. The advantages of a theory-based approach are numerous: theory allows for a shared understanding, for the development of a cumulative science that limits the re-invention of existing concepts, and importantly is based on constructs which have been investigated, for which measures can be validated and standardized and have been shown to provide a useful account of behavior[25]. Furthermore, investigating the relationship between theory-based factors and healthcare professional behavior provides an opportunity to identify factors that can be targeted by implementation interventions to change healthcare professional behavior[15, 23, 26, 27].
The number and heterogeneity of potential theories that might be used to guide implementation research poses a challenge to researchers wanting to assess and identify theory-based factors underlying healthcare professional behavior[22, 2830]. The Theoretical Domains Framework (TDF)[31] was developed as an integrative framework of theories of behavior change to overcome these challenges. The framework includes 12 theoretical domains of potential behavioral determinants and provides exemplar questions for the theoretical assessment of implementation problems. The framework has been used in a number of studies and was demonstrated to be useful for the development of qualitative[32, 33] and quantitative[3436] measurement tools to assess potential implementation behavior determinants. However, factor analysis implied that only one out of these three questionnaires was able to measure the theoretical domains independently[36]. Furthermore, the questionnaires were developed to assess determinants of specific implementation behaviors in specific settings (i.e., tobacco use prevention and smoking cessation in dental healthcare[34], smoking cessation in maternal care[35], and different types of patient safety behaviors in hospitals[36]) and internal consistency reliability was low[34] or could be improved[35, 36].
Since its original development, the consensus study that produced the TDF[31] has been validated, leading to Cane et al.’s[37] refined TDF. It extends the original TDF to include the following 14 domains: Knowledge; Skills; Social/professional role and identity; Beliefs about capabilities; Optimism; Beliefs about consequences; Reinforcement; Intentions; Goals; Memory, attention and decision processes; Environmental context and resources; Social influences; Emotions; and Behavioral regulation. Main differences between the original and the revised framework include the separation of the domain Optimism from the domain Beliefs about capabilities and the domain Reinforcement from the domain Beliefs about consequences. In addition, the domain Motivation and goals was divided into two separate domains, i.e., Intentions and Goals, and the domain Nature of the behaviors was omitted in the revised framework. Although the framework is suggested to be useful for the development of theory-based questionnaires for use in implementation research, the content of the TDF has not yet been validated on item level. Therefore, it is not clear whether questionnaire items based on this recent version of the framework will be able to measure the 14 domains independently.
In the present study we aimed to develop a questionnaire assessing the 14 TDF domains, worded in such a way to provide researchers the capacity to tailor the items to the targets, actions, contexts and times of interest[38], whilst retaining the essential theoretical content in each item. Furthermore, we aimed to test the discriminant content validity of each item within the questionnaire.

Methods

Participants

Fifty-eight academics from the Netherlands were approached with details of the study and nineteen agreed to participate (response rate of 33%). They were either involved as experts in the field of behavior change, development of health behavior change interventions, or implementation of interventions in healthcare settings. They were recruited via the authors’ networks. The sample size was based on estimates of between three and 20 participants as adequate for judgment tasks[39, 40]. We included academics (instead of healthcare professionals) in this study, because the discriminant content validation (DCV) exercise of allocating items to TDF domains requires theoretical knowledge and experience with the specific domains.

Materials

We developed a questionnaire that initially included 79 items assessing each of the domains through their related key constructs (see Additional file1). Constructs within domains were selected based on conceptual relatedness to the content of the domain (i.e., Knowledge, Procedural knowledge, Skills, Professional role, and Memory); inclusion in relevant theories frequently used in the field of behavior change (and thus ready access to existing items): the Theory of Planned Behavior[41] (i.e., Perceived behavioral control, Attitudes, Subjective norm, and Intention) and Social Cognitive Theory[42] (i.e., Self-efficacy, Outcome expectancies, and Social support); existence of validated scales (i.e., Optimism, Pessimism, Action planning, Attention, Affect, Stress, Automaticity, and Self-monitoring); and/or relevance to the implementation of PA interventions in routine healthcare by mapping factors resulting from previous research[43, 44] onto the TDF domains. JP and JMH independently identified that the constructs Reinforcement, Priority, Resources/materials, and Descriptive norm were salient in the previous PA-based research and thus these constructs were also included as construct-indicators of their respective domains.
Items measuring constructs within the domains Knowledge, Beliefs about capabilities, Optimism, Beliefs about consequences, Intentions, Social influences, Emotion, and Behavioral regulation were adapted from previously published questionnaires (i.e.,[34, 35, 41, 42, 4553]). Given lack of available questionnaires in the literature for some domains, new items were created for the domains Skills, Social/professional role and identity, Reinforcement, and Environmental context and resources. With regard to the domain Goals, items were newly developed for the construct Priority (as none could be located in the literature), while items measuring the construct Action planning were adapted from a previously published questionnaire[46]. With regard to the domain Memory, attention, and decision making, items measuring the construct Attention were adapted from a previously published questionnaire[51] and items measuring the construct Memory were newly developed. New items were developed based on discussions between JP and JMH. These discussions were informed by the academic literature on the concept and definition of specific domains and constructs, questions to identify behavior change processes as formulated by Michie et al.[31], and themes emerging from interviews on the implementation of PA interventions[43]. WAG and MRC supervised the development of the questionnaire and reviewed items’ face validity.
To develop a questionnaire which could be used by researchers in different fields of implementation research, items were formulated in a generic way using a '[action] in [context, time] with [target]’ construction based on the 'TACT principle’[38], whereby researchers can specify the target, action, context, and time relevant to their research. The questionnaire was developed in English, then translated to Dutch and back-translated to English by an independent translator. The small amount of differences between the original and back-translated version of the questionnaire were discussed and adaptations were made.

Procedure

In May and June 2012 participants were sent an email including the link to the online DCV exercise[54, 55]. After one and two weeks non respondents received a reminder. Participants were provided with the aim of the study and an explanation of the DCV exercise. Then, they were asked to report their expertise on each of the 14 TDF domains on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = I am a layman with regard to this domain; 7 = I am an expert with regard to this domain).
We used Cane et al.’s[37] definitions of the 14 TDF domains (see Table 1), which were presented at the top of each rating page. The items of the questionnaire were listed below the definitions, in a random order. Participants were asked to consider carefully the meaning of each item and allocate it to the domain they perceived the item measures using the domain definitions provided. To determine whether items were deemed to discriminately measure domains or if they measure a combination of domains, participants were asked to allocate each of the 79 items to up to three domains. Upon allocating items, judges were asked to rate their confidence in each allocation between 0% and 100% (0% = not at all confident; 100% = extremely confident). For example, a judge could allocate an item to the domain Knowledge and rate their confidence 60% and allocate the same item to the domain Skills and rate their confidence 20%.
Table 1
Definitions of the domains of the TDF[37] 1
Domain
Definition
D1 Knowledge
An awareness of the existence of something
D2 Skills
An ability or proficiency acquired through practice
D3 Social/professional role and identity
A coherent set of behaviors and displayed personal qualities of an individual in a social or work setting
D4 Beliefs about capabilities
Acceptance of the truth, reality, or validity about an ability, talent, or facility that a person can put to constructive us
D5 Optimism
The confidence that things will happen for the best or that desired goals will be attained
D6 Beliefs about consequences
Acceptance of the truth, reality, or validity about outcomes of a behavior in a given situation
D7 Reinforcement
Increasing the probability of a response by arranging a dependent relationship, or contingency, between the response and a given stimulus
D8 Intentions
A conscious decision to perform a behavior or a resolve to act in a certain way
D9 Goals
Mental representations of outcomes or end states that an individual wants to achieve
D10 Memory, attention and decision processes
The ability to retain information, focus selectively on aspects of the environment and choose between two or more alternatives
D11 Environmental context and resources
Any circumstance of a person’s situation or environment that discourages or encourages the development of skills and abilities, independence, social competence, and adaptive behavior
D12 Social influences
Those interpersonal processes that can cause individuals to change their thoughts, feelings, or behaviors
D13 Emotion
A complex reaction pattern, involving experiential, behavioral, and physiological elements, by which the individual attempts to deal with a personally significant matter or event
D14 Behavioral regulation
Anything aimed at managing or changing objectively observed or measured actions
1As described in Cane et al.[37] definitions are based on definitions from the American Psychological Associations’ Dictionary of Psychology[56].

Data analysis

Classification of items

Ratings for matching items and domains (i.e., items judged to assess the domain they were designed to assess) were coded 1 (a 'match’), whereas items judged to assess a different domain were coded -1 (a 'no match’); missing variables were scored 0. Each judgment was multiplied by its accompanied confidence rating (e.g., .20, .40, .80). As a consequence, the weighted judgments ranged from -1 to 1.

DCV analysis

Following Dixon et al.[54, 55], we used one-sample one-tailed t-tests to investigate whether each item was classified by the judges to represent the domain that the item aimed to measure. Judges were provided with three possibilities to allocate an item to a domain, therefore, the sum of the three weighted judgments was used for the one-sample t-tests. An item was classified as measuring a domain if its weighted judgment against that domain was significantly greater than zero (p < .05)[54]. The false discovery rate controlling procedure[57] was used to correct for multiple tests. Items that were classified to the correct (i.e., intended) domain were included in the final questionnaire, whereas items that were allocated to more than one domain or that were classified to a domain other than the intended domain were not included. Analyses were performed in IBM SPSS Statistics version 19.0[58].

Inter-rater agreement

A generalization of Cohen’s kappa (i.e., Light’s Kappa[59]) was calculated to assess agreement between judges across their allocation of all items to domains. For this calculation, we used the first domain that judges selected to represent the item. This was justified as the data indicated that judges used the first selected domain as the most preferable domain (i.e., domain with the highest confidence ratings) to allocate an item to. As a consequence, the 79 items were scored between 1 and 14 (representing the domain it was allocated to) for each judge separately. This resulted in a data matrix composed of 79 rows (i.e., the items) and 19 columns (i.e., the judges). We also assessed inter-rater agreement for allocation of items to each domain. For this calculation, the 79 items were scored between 1 and 0 for each domain separately (representing if it was selected to the specific domain or not) and for each judge separately. This resulted in 14 data matrices, one for each domain, consisting of 79 rows and 19 columns. These analyses were repeated for the final set of items that was selected based on the DCV analysis. In line with previous research, κ-values of between .00 and .20 were labeled as slight agreement, values from .21 to .40 as fair agreement, values from .41 to .60 as moderate agreement, values from .61 to .80 as substantial, and values from .81 to 1.00 as almost perfect[60]. Analyses were performed in the R software environment[61], using the R-package 'Psy’[62].

Ethics

The Medical Ethics Committee of the Leiden University Medical Centre gave ethics approval for this study (reference number NV/CME 09/081).

Results

Judges’ expertise in the use of domains

Descriptive statistics of judges’ expertise in the use of each domain are shown in Table 2. Mean scores indicated that judges had at least some expertise on each domain. On average, judges rated that they had most expertise on the domains Intentions and Goals, whereas lowest expertise ratings were given to the domains Social/professional role and identity, and Memory, attention, and decision processes. Only three judges indicated to be a layman on, respectively, one, two, and seven domains.
Table 2
Judges’ expertise on domains
Domains
Mean (SD)
D1 Knowledge
4.63 (1.01)
D2 Skills
5.21 (0.71)
D3 Social/professional role and identity
3.47 (1.81)
D4 Beliefs about capabilities
5.26 (1.45)
D5 Optimism
3.68 (1.70)
D6 Beliefs about consequences
4.68 (1.49)
D7 Reinforcement
4.63 (1.50)
D8 Intentions
5.53 (1.31)
D9 Goals
5.47 (1.02)
D10 Memory, attention, and decision processes
3.58 (1.68)
D11 Environmental context and resources
4.11 (2.08)
D12 Social influences
5.32 (1.20)
D13 Emotion
4.11 (1.60)
D14 Behavioral regulation
5.26 (1.45)
Note. 1 = I am a layman with regard to this domain, 7 = I am an expert with regard to this domain.
Neither judges’ expertise with TDF domains nor their academic level (i.e., PhD student, PhD, Professor) was related to their performance on the classification of items to domains calculated as the number of 'matches’. Pearson’s correlations were respectively r = -.35 (p = .14) and r = -.16 (p = .52).

DCV results

Table 3 shows the results of the DCV analysis. Of 79 items, 32 were classified as measuring the intended domain and therefore included in the final questionnaire. Forty-seven items were allocated to more than one domain, of which 39 items were allocated to the intended domain as well as additional domains, while eight items were classified as measuring a domain other than the item aimed to measure. Table 4 shows Kappa values for the agreement between judges based on all 79 items of the initial questionnaire and the 32 items included in the final questionnaire. The final lists of items measuring TDF domains are shown in Table 5 (English) and Table 6 (Dutch).
Table 3
DCV analysis of the questionnaire
Domain
Construct
Item
Mean
t-value
Domain allocation if not classified to right domain (> 1)
D1 Knowledge
Knowledge (3)
I am aware of the content and objectives of [innovation/guideline]
.82
9.99*
-
I know the content and objectives of [innovation/guideline]
.88
17.58*
-
I am familiar with the content and objectives of [innovation/guideline]
.82
8.76*
-
Procedural knowledge (3)
I am aware of how to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.74
5.51*
-
 
I know how to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.20
1.03
D1, D2, D4, D14
 
I am familiar with how to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.44
2.63
D1, D2
D2 Skills
Skills (4)
I have been trained how to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.86
16.42*
-
I have the proficiency to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
-.01
-0.05
D2, D4
I have the skills to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.53
3.84*
-
I have practiced [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.67
5.28*
-
D3 Social/professional role and identity
Professional role (4)
[A] in [C, T] with [Ta] is part of my work as a [profession]
.85
9.62*
-
As a [profession], it is my job to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.89
29.74*
-
It is my responsibility as a [profession] to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.82
7.90*
-
Doing [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] is consistent with my [profession]
.81
8.70*
-
D4 Beliefs about capabilities
Self-efficacy (2)
I am confident that I can [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] even when [Ta] is not motivated
71
5.80*
-
I am confident that I can [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] even when there is little time
.67
5.31*
-
Perceived behavioral control (4)
I am confident that if I wanted I could [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.78
7.07*
-
How much control do you have over [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]?
.02
0.11
D4, D2, D14
For me, [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] is… (Very difficult – very easy)
.40
2.17
D4, D2, D5
For me, [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] is… (Impossible – possible)
.33
2.21
D4, D2, D5, D6
D5 Optimism
Optimism (3)
With regard to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] in uncertain times, I usually expect the best
.64
5.01*
-
With regard to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] I’m always optimistic about the future
.65
4.35*
-
With regard to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] overall, I expect more good things to happen than bad
.13
0.65
D5, D6
Pessimism (3)
With regard to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] if something can go wrong, it will
.43
3.13
D5, D4, D6
With regard to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] I hardly ever expect things to go my way
.03
0.14
D5, D4, D6
With regard to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] I rarely count on good things happening to me
.44
2.60
D5, D6
D6 Beliefs about consequences
Attitudes (2)
For me, [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] is… (Useless – useful)
.45
2.71
D6, D5
For me, [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] is… (bad – good)
.42
2.82
D6, D1, D3
Outcome expectancies (2)
If I [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] it will benefit public health
.60
4.44*
-
If I [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] it will have disadvantages for my relationship with [Ta]
.58
4.14*
-
D7 Reinforcement
Reinforcement (3)
Whenever I [A] in [C, T] with [Ta], I get financial reimbursement
.42
2.38
D7, D6
Whenever I [A] in [C, T] with [Ta], I get recognition from professionals who are important to me
-.51
-3.77*
D7, D3, D6, D12
Whenever I [A] in [C, T] with [Ta, I feel like I am making a difference
-.68
-7.14*
D7, D4, D6, D13
D8 Intentions
Intention (4)
For how many of the next 10 [Ta] do you intend to [A] in [C]?
.73
6.92*
-
I will definitely [A] in [C] with [Ta] in the next [T]
.63
3.89*
-
I intend to [A] in [C] with [Ta] in the next [T]
.66
5.83*
-
How strong is your intention to [A] with [Ta] in [C] in the next [T]?
.89
20.60*
-
D9 Goals
Action planning (4)
I have a clear plan of how I will [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.47
2.75
D14, D8, D9
I have a clear plan under what circumstances I will [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.26
1.22
D14, D8, D9
I have a clear plan when I will [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.43
2.26
D14, D8, D9
I have a clear plan how often I will [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
-.18
-0.83
D14, D8, D9
Priority (4)
Generally, in [C, T] with [Ta], how often is covering something else on your agenda a higher priority than [A]
-.58
-4.10*
D9, D3, D10, D11
Generally, in [C, T] with [Ta], how often does covering something else on your agenda take precedence over [A]
-.58
-4.30*
D9, D3, D10, D11
Generally, in [C, T] with [Ta], how often is covering something else on your agenda more urgent than [A]
-.49
-4.32*
D9, D3, D10, D11, D14
Generally, in [C, T] with [Ta], how often is covering something else on your agenda more pressing than [A]
-.63
-4.82*
D9, D3, D10, D11
D10 Memory, attention and decision processes
Memory (4)
[A] in [C, T] with [Ta] is easy to remember
.32
1.64
D10, D1, D4
How often do you forget [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]?
.63
4.55*
-
How often do you have to check the [innovation/guideline] before [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]?
-.66
-5.52*
D10, D1, D4
To what extent do you know [innovation/guideline] by heart to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]?
-.91
-30.09*
D1
Attention (4)
When I need to concentrate to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta], I have no trouble focusing my attention
.77
7.10*
-
When I am working hard on [A] in [C, T] with [Ta], I still get distracted by events around me
.52
3.24
-
When trying to focus my attention on [A] in [C, T] with [Ta], I have difficulty blocking out distracting thoughts
.68
5.06*
-
When concentrating on [A] in [C, T] with [Ta], I can focus my attention so that I become unaware of what’s going on around me
.68
6.03*
-
D11 Environmental context and resources
Resources/material (8)
[Innovation/guideline] has a good fit with routine practice
.22
1.44
D11, D1, D3
[Innovation/guideline] provides the possibility to adapt it to the [Ta]’s needs (e.g., culture)
.25
1.55
D11, D3, D6, D12
In the organization I work [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] is routine
-.02
-0.11
D11, D2, D3, D12, D14
In the organization I work there is enough time to [A ] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.42
2.24
D11, D3
Within the socio-political context there is sufficient financial support (e.g., from local authorities, insurance companies, the government) for [innovation/guideline]
.86
13.48*
-
Within the socio-political context there are good networks between parties involved in [innovation/guideline]
.74
9.35*
-
Prior to delivery of [innovation/guideline] professionals are provided with a training to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
-.51
-3.29
D11, D2
During the delivery of [innovation/guideline] professionals are provided with sufficient financial reimbursement to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.13
0.69
D11, D7
D12 Social influences
Social support (4)
I can rely on the team of professionals with whom I deliver [innovation] when things get tough on [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
-.35
-1.96
D12, D3, D11
My colleagues are willing to listen to my problems related to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.22
1.30
D12, D3, D11
The team of professionals with whom I deliver [innovation] is helpful in getting [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] done
.14
0.74
D12, D11
I can rely on my colleagues when things get tough on [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.07
0.38
D12, D3, D11
Subjective norm (2)
Most people who are important to me think that I should [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.84
9.04*
-
Most people whose opinion I value would approve me of [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.61
3.97*
-
Descriptive norm (2)
The team of professionals with whom I deliver [innovation/guideline] [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.35
2.13
D12, D3, D11
Respected colleagues [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.24
1.26
D12, D3, D11
D13 Emotion
Affect (2)
Thinking about yourself and how you normally feel as a professional that delivers [innovation/guideline], to what extent do you generally feel inspired with regard to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
-.09
-0.49
D13, D3, D14
Thinking about yourself and how you normally feel as a professional that delivers [innovation/guideline], to what extent do you generally feel nervous with regard to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
-.01
-0.05
D13, D3, D4
Stress (2)
Have you recently, during the past two weeks been able to enjoy your normal day-to-day activities?
.55
3.55*
-
Have you recently, during the past two weeks been feeling unhappy and depressed?
.78
6.95*
-
D14 Behavioral regulation
Automaticity (2)
[A] in [C, T] with [Ta] is something I do automatically
-.31
-0.20
D14, D2, D10
[A] in [C, T] with [Ta] is something I do without thinking
-.45
-3.29
D2, D10
Self-monitoring (4)
I keep track of my overall progress towards [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.27
1.57
D14, D7, D9, D13
I tend to notice my successes while working towards [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
-.39
-2.53
D14, D9
I am aware of my day-to-day behavior as I work towards [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
-.09
-0.49
D14, D8, D9, D10
I check regularly whether I am getting closer to attaining [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.49
3.45
D14, D9
Action planning (4)
I have a clear plan of how I will [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.47
2.75
D14, D8, D9
I have a clear plan under what circumstances I will [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.26
1.22
D14, D8, D9
I have a clear plan when I will [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
.43
2.26
D14, D8, D9
I have a clear plan how often I will [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
-.18
-0.83
D14, D8, D9
Note. [A], action; [C], context; [T], time; [Ta], target; *, significant at .05 level, after false discovery rate controlling procedure for multiple tests; D1, Knowledge; D2, Skills; D3, Social/professional role and identity; D4, Beliefs about capabilities; D5, Optimism; D6, Beliefs about consequences; D7, Reinforcement; D8, Intentions; D9, Goals; D10, Memory, attention, and decision processes; D11, Environmental context and resources; D12, Social influences; D13, Emotion; D14, Behavioral regulation; D, domain the item intended to measure; D, domain the item is systematically allocated to other than the item intended to measure.
Table 4
Light’s κ-values for all items and the items included in the final questionnaire
Domains
All 79 items κ (95% C.I.)
32 final items κ (95% C.I.)
All items and domains
.56 (.50–.62)
.82 (.79–.85)
D1 Knowledge
.76 (.63–.87)
.88 (.77–.96)
D2 Skills
.58 (.35–.71)
.80 (.73–.87)
D3 Social/professional role and identity
.59 (.37–.75)
.86 (.72–.93)
D4 Beliefs about capabilities
.55 (.41–.71)
.73 (.60–.81)
D5 Optimism
.60 (.49–.69)
.68 (.63–.72)
D6 Beliefs about consequences
.49 (.34–.62)
.70 (.67–.73)
D7 Reinforcement
.59 (.50–.68)
-
D8 Intentions
.75 (.56–.87)
.93 (.89-1.00)
D9 Goals
.11 (.07–.14)
-
D10 Memory, attention, and decision processes
.63 (.48–.75)
.85 (.79–.90)
D11 Environmental context and resources
.48 (.34–.65)
.82 (.73–.87)
D12 Social influences
.53 (.43–.67)
.78 (.69–.86)
D13 Emotion
.58 (.44–.70)
.90 (.83–.96)
D14 Behavioral regulation
.36 (.20–.52)
-
Note. C.I., biased-corrected bootstrapped confidence interval of Light’s Kappa (based on 200 bootstrap samples).
With regard to the 32 final items, κ-values could not be calculated for the domains Reinforcement, Goals, and Behavioral regulation, because none of the items measuring these domains was included in the final questionnaire.
Table 5
Final list of items measuring TDF domains (English)
Domain
Item
D1 Knowledge (4)
I am aware of the content and objectives of [innovation/guideline]
I know the content and objectives of [innovation/guideline]
I am familiar with the content and objectives of [innovation/guideline]
I am aware of how to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
D2 Skills (3)
I have been trained how to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
I have the skills to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
I have practiced [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
D3 Social/professional role and identity (4)
[A] in [C, T] with [Ta] is part of my work as a [profession]
As a [profession], it is my job to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
It is my responsibility as a [profession] to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
Doing [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] is consistent with my [profession]
D4 Beliefs about capabilities (3)
I am confident that I can [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] even when [Ta] is not motivated
I am confident that I can [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] even when there is little time
I am confident that if I wanted I could [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
D5 Optimism (2)
With regard to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] in uncertain times, I usually expect the best
With regard to [A] in [C, time] with [Ta] I’m always optimistic about the future
D6 Beliefs about consequences (2)
If I [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] it will benefit public health
If I [A] in [C, T] with [Ta] it will have disadvantages for my relationship with [Ta]
D7 Reinforcement (0)
**
D8 Intentions (4)
For how many of the next 10 [Ta] do you intend to [A] in [C]?
I will definitely [A] in [C] with [Ta] in the next [T]
I intend to [A] in [C] with [Ta] in the next [T]
How strong is your intention to [A] with [Ta] in [C] in the next [T]?
D9 Goals (0)
**
D10 Memory, attention and decision processes (4)
How often do you forget [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]?
When I need to concentrate to [A] in [C, T] with [Ta], I have no trouble focusing my attention
When trying to focus my attention on [A] in [C, T] with [Ta], I have difficulty blocking out distracting thoughts
When concentrating on [A] in [C, T] with [Ta], I can focus my attention so that I become unaware of what’s going on around me
D11 Environmental context and resources (2)
Within the socio-political context there is sufficient financial support (e.g., from local authorities, insurance companies, the government) for [innovation/guideline]
Within the socio-political context there are good networks between parties involved in [innovation/guideline]
D12 Social influences (2)
Most people who are important to me think that I should [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
Most people whose opinion I value would approve me of [A] in [C, T] with [Ta]
D13 Emotion (2)
Have you recently, during the past two weeks been able to enjoy your normal day-to-day activities?
Have you recently, during the past two weeks been feeling unhappy and depressed?
D14 Behavioral regulation (0)
**
Note. [A], action; [C], context; [T], time; [Ta], target; **, discriminant content validity of the items measuring these domains was not demonstrated.
Table 6
Final list of items measuring TDF domains (Dutch)
Domain
Item
D1 Knowledge (4)
Ik ben op de hoogte van de inhoud en doelstellingen van [innovatie/richtlijn]
Ik ken de inhoud en doelstellingen van [innovatie/richtlijn]
Ik ben bekend met de inhoud en doelstellingen van [innovatie/richtlijn]
Ik ben op de hoogte van hoe ik [A] in [C, T] met [Ta]
D2 Skills (3)
Ik ben getraind hoe ik [A] in [C, T] met [Ta]
Ik heb de vaardigheden om [A] in [C, T] met [Ta]
Ik heb [A in [C, T] met [Ta] in [C, T] met [Ta] geoefend
D3 Social/professional role and identity (4)
[A] in [C, T] met [Ta] hoort bij mijn werk als [beroep]
Als [beroep] is het mijn taak om [A] in [C, T] met [Ta]
Het is mijn verantwoordelijkheid als [beroep] om [A] in [C, T] met [Ta]
Het doen van [A] in [C, T] met [Ta] is overeenkomend met mijn [beroep]
D4 Beliefs about capabilities (3)
Ik heb er vertrouwen in dat ik in staat ben om [A] in [C, T] met [Ta], zelfs wanneer [Ta] niet gemotiveerd is
Ik heb er vertrouwen in dat ik in staat ben om [A] in [C, T] met [Ta], zelfs wanneer er weinig tijd is
Ik heb er vertrouwen in dat als ik het wil, ik in staat ben om [A] in [C, T] met [Ta]
D5 Optimism (2)
Als het gaat om [A] in [C, T] met [Ta] dan verwacht ik in onzekere tijden, toch meestal het beste
Als het gaat om [A] in [C, T] met [Ta] dan ben ik altijd optimistisch over de toekomst
D6 Beliefs about consequences (2)
Als ik [A] in [C, T] met [Ta], dan zal dit voordelig zijn voor de publieke gezondheid
Als ik [A] in [C, T] met [Ta], dan zal het nadelig zijn voor mijn relatie met [Ta]
D7 Reinforcement (0)
**
D8 Intentions (4)
Voor hoeveel van de komende 10 [Ta] heb je de intentie om [A] in [C]?
Ik zal zeker [A] in [C] met [Ta] in de komende [T]
Ik ben van plan om [A] in [C] met [Ta] in de komende [T]
Hoe sterk is uw intentie om [A] in [C] met [Ta] in de komende [T]?
D9 Goals (0)
**
D10 Memory, attention and decision processes (4)
Hoe vaak vergeet u [A] in [C, T] met [Ta]?
Als ik me moet concentreren om [A] in [C, T] met [Ta], lukt het mij gemakkelijk om mijn aandacht hierop te richten.
Als ik mijn aandacht probeer te richten op [A] in [C, T] met [Ta], vind ik het moeilijk afleidende gedachten uit te schakelen
Als ik me concentreer op [A] in [C, T] met [Ta], kan ik mijn aandacht zo richten dat ik niet merk wat er om me heen gebeurt
D11 Environmental context and resources (2)
Binnen de sociaal-politieke context is er voldoende financiële ondersteuning (bijv. van gemeente, zorgverzekeraars, de overheid) voor [innovatie/richtlijn]
Binnen de sociaal politieke context zijn er goede netwerken tussen partijen betrokken bij [innovatie/richtlijn]
D12 Social influences (2)
De meeste mensen die belangrijk voor mij zijn vinden dat ik [A] in [C, T] met [Ta] zou moeten doen
De meeste mensen van wie ik hun mening waardeer, zouden [A] in [C, T] met [Ta] goedkeuren
D13 Emotion (2)
Heeft u de laatste tijd (de afgelopen twee weken) plezier kunnen beleven aan gewone, dagelijkse bezigheden?
Heeft u zich de laatste tijd (de afgelopen twee weken) ongelukkig en neerslachtig gevoeld?
D14 Behavioral regulation (0)
**
Note. [A], actie; [C], context; [T], tijd; [Ta], target; **, discriminant content validity of the items measuring these domains could was not demonstrated.

Knowledge

The domain Knowledge was defined as 'an awareness of the existence of something’[37]. Of the six Knowledge items included in the DCV exercise, four items were classified as measuring the domain Knowledge (Table 4) and were included in the final questionnaire. Two items were allocated to more than one domain. In addition to the domain Knowledge, these items were amongst others allocated to the domain Skills. The extent to which judges agreed on which items measured the domain was substantial when including all items (κ = .76; 95% C.I. .63-.87; Table 4) and almost perfect for the 32 final items (κ = .88; 95% C.I. .77-.96; Table 4).

Skills

The domain Skills was defined as 'an ability or proficiency acquired through practice’[37]. Three out of four Skills items included in the DCV were classified as measuring the intended domain (Table 3) and were included in the final questionnaire. In addition to the domain Skills, nine judges allocated the item 'I have the proficiency to…’ to the domain Beliefs about capabilities. With all items included, moderate agreement between judges was found for their allocation of items to the domain (κ = .58; 95% C.I. .35-.71; Table 4), while substantial agreement was found for the 32 final items (κ = .80; 95% C.I. .73-.87; Table 4).

Social/professional role and identity

The domain Social/professional role and identity was defined as 'a coherent set of behaviors and displayed personal qualities of an individual in a social or work setting’[37]. All four Social/professional role and identity items included in the DCV were classified as measuring the intended domain (Table 3) and were included in the final questionnaire. The extent to which judges agreed on which items measured the domain was moderate with all items included (κ = .59; 95% C.I. .37-.75; Table 4) and almost perfect for the 32 final items (κ = .86; 95% C.I. .72-.93; Table 4).

Beliefs about capabilities

The domain Beliefs about capabilities was defined as 'acceptance of the truth, reality, or validity about an ability, talent, or facility that a person can put to constructive use’[37]. Six Beliefs about capabilities items were included in the DCV exercise. The three items containing the word 'confident’ were classified as measuring the intended domain (Table 3) and were included in the final questionnaire. The items measuring the difficulty and possibility of [action] in [context, time] with [target] were allocated to more than one domain. In addition to the domain Beliefs about capabilities, they were often allocated to the domain Skills. The item 'How much control do you have over…’ was allocated to the intended domain, but also to the domains Skills and Behavioral regulation. With all items included, moderate agreement between judges was found for their allocation of items to the domain (κ = .55; 95% C.I. .41-.71; Table 4), while substantial agreement was found for the 32 final items (κ = .73; 95% C.I. .60-.81; Table 4).

Optimism

The domain Optimism was defined as 'the confidence that things will happen for the best or that desired goals will be attained’[37]. Two out of six Optimism items included in the DCV were classified as measuring the domain Optimism (Table 3). These were included in the final questionnaire. Four items were allocated to more than one domain, including the domains Beliefs about capabilities and Beliefs about consequences. The extent to which judges agreed on which items measured the domain was moderate with all items included (κ = .60; 95% C.I. .49-.69; Table 4) and substantial for the final 32 items (κ = .68; 95% C.I. .63-.72; Table 4).

Beliefs about consequences

The domain Beliefs about consequences was defined as 'acceptance of the truth, reality, or validity about outcomes of a behavior in a given situation’[37]. Of the four Beliefs about consequences items included in the DCV, only two items were classified as measuring the intended domain (Table 3) and included in the questionnaire. These were the items measuring the construct Outcome expectancies. The two items measuring the construct Attitudes were allocated to a variety of domains, including Social/professional role and identity and Optimism. With all items included, moderate agreement between judges was found for their allocation of items to the domain (κ = .49; 95% C.I. .34-.62; Table 4), while substantial agreement was found for the final 32 items (κ = .70; 95% C.I. .67-.73; Table 4).

Reinforcement

The domain Reinforcement was defined as 'increasing the probability of a response by arranging a dependent relationship, or contingency, between the response and a given stimulus’[37]. The DCV exercise included three items intended to measure Reinforcement, but none of them was classified as measuring the domain (Table 3) and so none of them was included in the final questionnaire. The item '…I get financial reimbursement’ was, in addition to the intended domain, allocated to the domain Beliefs about consequences. Two items were classified as measuring domains they were not intended to measure. The item '…I get recognition from professionals who are important to me’ was classified as measuring the domain Social influences and the item '…I feel like I am making a difference’ was classified as measuring the domain Beliefs about consequences. Five judges did not allocate any item to the domain. Without these judges taken into account Cohen’s kappa indicated moderate agreement (κ = .59; 95% C.I. .50-.68; Table 4).

Intentions

The domain Intentions was defined as 'a conscious decision to perform a behavior or a resolve to act in a certain way’[37]. All four items included in the DCV to measure Intentions were classified as measuring the domain (Table 3) and included in the final questionnaire. The extent to which judges agreed on which items measured the domain was substantial with all items included (κ = .75; 95% C.I. .56-.87; Table 4) and almost perfect for the final 32 items (κ = .93; 95% C.I. .89-1.00; Table 4).

Goals

The domain Goals was defined as 'mental representations of outcomes or end states that an individual wants to achieve’[37]. Eight Goals items were included in the DCV exercise. None of them were classified to the right domain (Table 3) and thus Goals items were not included in the final questionnaire. Items measuring the construct Priority were classified as measuring the domain Memory, attention, and decision processes. The four items measuring the construct Action planning were included in the DCV as measuring both the domain Goals and Behavioral regulation. They were not classified as measuring these two domains, because they were also often allocated to the domain Intentions. Three judges did not allocate items to the domain. Without these judges taken into account kappa indicated slight agreement (κ = .11; 95% C.I. .07-.14; Table 4).

Memory, attention, and decision processes

The domain Memory, attention, and decision processes was defined as 'the ability to retain information, focus selectively on aspects of the environment and choose between two or more alternatives’[37]. Eight items were included in the DCV exercise to measure the domain Memory, attention, and decision processes. Four of these items were classified to measure the intended domain (Table 3) and were included in the final questionnaire. Two items were allocated to more than one domain and two items measuring the construct Memory were classified as measuring a domain other than they were intended to measure (i.e., Knowledge and Beliefs about capabilities). The extent to which judges agreed on which items measured the domain was substantial with all items included (κ = .63; 95% C.I. .48-.75; Table 4) and almost perfect for the final 32 items (κ = .85; 95% C.I. .79-.90; Table 4).

Environmental context and resources

The domain Environmental context and resources was defined as 'any circumstance of a person’s situation or environment that discourages or encourages the development of skills and abilities, independence, social competence, and adaptive behavior’[37]. Eight items were included in the DCV to measure this domain, while only two items were classified as measuring the domain (Table 3) and therefore could be included in the final questionnaire. Other items, not including the word 'socio-political context’ were, in addition to the intended domain, foremost allocated to the domains Skills, Social/professional role and identity, and Social influences. With all items included, moderate agreement between judges was found for their allocation of items to the domain (κ = .48; 95% C.I. .34-.65; Table 4), while almost perfect agreement was found for the final 32 items (κ = .82; 95% C.I. .73-.87; Table 4).

Social influences

The domain Social influences was defined as 'those interpersonal processes that can cause individuals to change their thoughts, feelings, or behaviors’[37]. Two out of eight Social influences items included in the DCV, were classified as measuring the intended domain (Table 3) and therefore included in the final questionnaire. These were the items measuring the construct Subjective norm. In addition to the domain Social influences, the other six items were mostly allocated to the domains Social/professional role and identity and Environmental context and resources. The extent to which judges agreed on which items measured the domain was moderate with all items included (κ = .53; 95% C.I. .43-.67; Table 4) and substantial for the final 32 items (κ = .78; 95% C.I. .69-.86; Table 4).

Emotion

The domain Emotion was defined as 'a complex reaction pattern, involving experiential, behavioral, and physiological elements, by which the individual attempts to deal with a personally significant matter or event’[37]. Of the four Emotion items included in the DCV exercise, the two items measuring the construct Stress were classified as measuring the intended domain (Table 3). These items were included in the final questionnaire. The two items measuring the construct Affect were allocated to more than one domain, including Emotion, Social/professional role and identity, and Beliefs about capabilities. With all items included, moderate agreement between judges was found for their allocation of items to the domain (κ = .58; 95% C.I. .44-.70; Table 4), while almost perfect agreement was found for the final 32 items (κ = .90; 95% C.I., .83-.96; Table 4).

Behavioral regulation

The domain Behavioral regulation was defined as 'anything aimed at managing or changing objectively observed or measured actions’[37]. Ten items, including Action planning items also aimed to measure the domain Goals, were included in the DCV to measure Behavioral regulation. None of them were classified to the right domain (Table 3) and therefore Behavioral regulation items were not included in the final questionnaire. The six items measuring the constructs Automaticity and Self-monitoring were allocated to more than one domain including Behavioral regulation, Skills, Goals, and Memory attention, and decision processes. Two judges did not allocate any of the 79 items to the domain. Without these judges taken into account kappa indicated fair agreement (κ = .36; 95% C.I. .20-.52; Table 4).

All items and domains

Overall, moderate agreement was found for the allocation of all 79 items to the 14 domains (κ = .56; 95% C.I. .50-.62; Table 4), while almost perfect agreement was found for the allocation of the final 32 items to the 14 domains (κ = .82; 95% C.I. .79-.85; Table 4).

Discussion

We have developed a TDF-based questionnaire in both English and Dutch able to discriminately assess the majority of domains. For the first time, items have been operationalized to assess TDF domains using theoretical constructs within each domain and these items were judged to be either pure measures of the domain, or else also measuring other domains. Our findings provide an additional level of validation for the content of the TDF: not only do judges agree about the constructs within each domain and the domain structure as demonstrated by Cane et al.[37], but the majority of TDF domains have now been shown to be largely discriminately measurable. These results correspond with Taylor et al.[36, 63] who found good discriminant validity of TDF domains in a questionnaire measuring influences on patient safety behaviors[36] and in the Determinants of Physical Activity Questionnaire[63]. While Taylor et al.[36, 63] used specific items (i.e., related to a specific application), our items are generic and allow for application within a range of different contexts in which implementation research takes place. In summary, the development of our questionnaire provides important evidence of content validity and is a first step towards the development of a valid and reliable questionnaire to measure TDF-based factors underlying healthcare professionals’ specific implementation behaviors.
Of the 79 items assessed, 32 items were able to discriminately measure the following 11 domains: Knowledge, Skills, Social/professional role and identity, Beliefs about capabilities, Optimism, Beliefs about consequences, Intentions, Memory, attention and decision processes, Environmental context and resources, Social influences, and Emotion. For each of these domains at least two items were identified that can be used in the development of a TDF-based questionnaire.
Following judges allocations, items were not able to measure the domains Reinforcement, Goals, and Behavioral regulation. Items intended to measure these domains were allocated to multiple domains or classified to a domain other than the item intended to measure. This may be due to a few reasons. First, it is possible that the items used to operationalize the constructs within these domains were not appropriate, which might be related to the fact that some of Reinforcement and Goals items were newly developed by the researchers rather than previously-validated items. Nevertheless, items intended to measure the domain Behavioral regulation through the constructs Automaticity, Self-monitoring, and Action planning were adapted from previously published questionnaires, and thus it is unlikely that the existing level of validation of items is responsible for challenges in allocating items to particular domains. Second, it might be that items could not be classified to measure these three domains, because the domain definitions were not fit for purpose. This is associated with the finding that five, three, and two judges did not allocate any of the items to, respectively, the domains Reinforcement, Goals, and Behavioral regulation. The findings may also be explained by the use of domain definitions instead of construct definitions to allocate items, while items were previously developed to target individual constructs rather than broader domains. The allocation of items to domain definitions might therefore be influenced by the closeness of the definition of the domain to the definition of its constituent constructs. Finally, it could be that the remaining domains themselves cannot be discriminately measured. This seems a plausible explanation, as the domain Reinforcement is a refinement of the Beliefs about consequences domain and was originally included within the latter domain in the original TDF[31]. It is then perhaps not surprising that the Reinforcement items were judged to be assessing Beliefs about consequences, and arguably, such assignment is theoretically appropriate. Furthermore, the refinement of the domain Motivation and goals of the original TDF[31] into the domains Goals and Intentions in the recent version of the TDF and the classification of multiple goal-related constructs to the domains Goals, Intentions, and Behavioral regulation imply overlap between these domains. Therefore, it is perhaps also not surprising that the items measuring these domains were allocated to all three domains, and thus are not able to discriminately measure them. From a discriminant content validity perspective, taken together these results support keeping to the 12 original domains as a basis for the development of TDF questionnaires. When using the 12-domain framework[31] to develop a TDF-based questionnaire, items measuring the domains Behavioral regulation and Nature of the behaviors should be identified to maintain the comprehensive nature of the TDF. This could be done by selecting domains’ related key constructs as provided by Michie et al.[31] and selecting items from existing validated scales.
Lastly, the findings indicate that further refinement of the final questionnaire is required. In general, the amount of items measuring most of the domains could be increased to at least three items for each domain (at least three items with a loading above .80 will give a reliable component[64]). With regard to the specific domains, the final items measuring the domain Environmental context and resources are framed entirely in terms of the socio-political context, while there may be additional environmental and resources influences that remain unmeasured. The initial version of the questionnaire included items related to characteristics of the innovation, organization, socio-political context, and innovation strategies[1620], however, only the items assessing the socio-political context were judged to discriminately assess this domain. Lack of discriminant content validity of items measuring characteristics of the innovation, organization, and innovation strategies might be due to our method of developing a generic questionnaire based on factors related to a specific implementation behavior (i.e., the implementation of PA interventions). Moreover, the domain Environmental context and resources is arguably among the least well conceptualized domains of the TDF, which may partly explain challenges that judges faced in allocating items to this domain. Nevertheless, potential users of the final questionnaire may wish to incorporate additional more contextually sensitive items focusing on the environment and resources whilst recognizing that their discriminant content validity has not yet been demonstrated. In the initial questionnaire, items measuring the domain Emotion were adapted from previously published questionnaires. Specifically, items measuring the construct Affect were based on the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule[49] and Stress items were based on the General Health Questionnaire[48]. Items measuring the construct Stress demonstrated to be able to discriminately assess the domain Emotions, while Affect items did not. Therefore, the final questionnaire includes items concerning healthcare professionals’ general feelings (i.e., Stress) instead of their emotions related to performing a specific behavior (i.e., Affect). Yet, when investigating determinants of healthcare professionals’ implementation behaviors, items assessing emotions in relation to performing a specific behavior should also be taken into account as these have been found to be linked to implementation behaviors in previous research[6567]. Although initial TACT-specific items assessing the construct Affect were not judged to discriminately assess the domain Emotions, potential users of the final questionnaire may want to consider using such items by including other emotions such as pride, empathy[67], fear[6567], and embarrassment[66]. Furthermore, the assessment of the domain Knowledge could be improved by adding items to test healthcare professionals’ knowledge on a certain implementation behavior[66, 68].

Strengths and limitations of assessing TDF domains using questionnaires

Limitations with regard to the use of the TDF for questionnaire development involve the large amount of domains and underlying constructs that can only be assessed by a large amount of items. Quantitative TDF-based research might preclude measuring all constructs within each domain due to time constraints as described earlier by Amemori et al.[34]. As a result, it is not clear which constructs to choose when measuring a given domain. In this study, constructs were selected based on close relatedness to the content of the domains, being a part of important theories of behavior change, existence of validated scales, and/or relevance to the implementation of PA interventions in routine healthcare as determined in previous studies[43, 44]. However, it is unclear to what extent the constructs that we selected measure the full breadth of the domains instead of a part of them. This questionnaire strove to balance representation of the constructs within the domains with a parsimonious questionnaire that could be feasibly used in the field. However, some domains cover a wider breath of constructs than others and future work could investigate the broader range of constructs within each domain. In addition, the TDF domains are potential behavioral determinants, instead of factors proven to influence implementation behavior and the framework does not specify relationships between domains[30]. On the other hand, quantitative applications of the framework can be beneficial for use in exploratory research and to guide theory selection.
Corresponding with the major rationale for the development of the original TDF, the framework can be used to assess a broad range of factors from a multitude of behavior change theories, helpful when little a priori information is available to base the selection of appropriate theories on. In comparison with other frameworks used in implementation research, e.g.,[16, 17, 20], and empirical work on the introduction of PA interventions in primary healthcare[43, 44] the TDF[37], however, mainly focuses on factors related to the adopting person, instead of taking into account a variety of factors related to characteristics of the innovation, patient, social setting, organizational context, and innovation methods and strategies[1620]. This implies factors outside psychological behavior change theory are not adequately elaborated in the framework. We believe that these factors may be included in the domain Environmental context and resources or multiple 'environmental’ domains should be incorporated in the TDF.

Strengths and limitations of our methods

While we used a rigorous DCV approach to validate the content of items in the questionnaire, some limitations of our study need to be taken into account. The DCV exercise of allocating 79 items to 14 domains was a challenging task for judges, requiring consideration of multiple possible definitions. This approach is a degree of magnitude more challenging than how DCVs have typically been applied in the past (to a much smaller number of constructs). A larger number of judges and a less complex task would have possibly increased information on discriminant content validity of the items. Major strengths of this study include the sample of academics with expertise on TDF domains and the formulation of items using the 'TACT principle’[38], which allows potential users of the questionnaire to tailor the content to their own target, action, context, and time. However, the operationalization and validation of the domains of the TDF are limited to these specific methods. It could be, for example, that in 'real life’ the validity of the domains would differ from the one perceived by an academic audience. Therefore, this study represents an important first step in the thorough development of a questionnaire to measure TDF-based factors underlying healthcare professionals’ implementation behaviors. As a next step we tested the Determinants of Implementation Behavior Questionnaire (DIBQ) on a sample of 270 healthcare professionals with specification of a particular target, action, context, and time, and showed good construct validity, with the majority of domains showing high internal consistency reliability and discriminant validity (Huijg et al., submitted).

Conclusions

To our knowledge, this study is the first to develop a generic (i.e., formulation of items following the 'TACT principle’[38]) TDF-based questionnaire in both English and Dutch including items which are able to discriminately measure a majority of the domains. The results partly support Cane et al.’s validation of the TDF[37] and suggest that the 12-domain version[31] might be more applicable in developing a TDF-based questionnaire. The items of this questionnaire can be used for the development of a questionnaire to measure TDF-based determinants of healthcare professionals’ specific implementation behaviors. Future research should investigate the concurrent and predictive validity and reliability of such a questionnaire in practice, among a large healthcare professional sample.
In general, a valid TDF-based questionnaire will increase the use of theory in the assessment of barriers and facilitators for implementation problems[31, 69, 70], which can inform the selection of possible techniques that can be used to change healthcare professionals’ behaviors[15, 23, 26]. Consequently, research on the development of a generic TDF questionnaire will improve our understanding of factors influencing healthcare professionals’ implementation and advance theory and methods in implementation research.

Acknowledgements

Furthermore, the authors wish to acknowledge Emma Massey for her involvement in the back-translation of the questionnaire. This research was funded by the European Health Psychology Society Tandem Grant.
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

JP and JMH are equally responsible for the design of the study, development of the questionnaire, and data analysis and interpretation. JMH collected the data and wrote the initial draft of the manuscript. Subsequent drafts of the manuscript were written together. WAG and MRC were involved in the development of the questionnaire and critically revised the manuscript. ED was involved in data analysis and interpretation, and commented on the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Grimshaw JM, Eccles MP, Lavis JN, Hill SJ, Squires JE: Knowledge translation of research findings. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 50-10.1186/1748-5908-7-50.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Grimshaw JM, Eccles MP, Lavis JN, Hill SJ, Squires JE: Knowledge translation of research findings. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 50-10.1186/1748-5908-7-50.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Glasgow RE, Lichtenstein E, Marcus AC: Why don’t we see more translation of health promotion research to practice? Rethinking the efficacy-to-effectiveness transition. Am J Public Health. 2003, 93: 1261-1267. 10.2105/AJPH.93.8.1261.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Glasgow RE, Lichtenstein E, Marcus AC: Why don’t we see more translation of health promotion research to practice? Rethinking the efficacy-to-effectiveness transition. Am J Public Health. 2003, 93: 1261-1267. 10.2105/AJPH.93.8.1261.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Haines A, Kuruvilla S, Borchert M: Policy and Practice Bridging the implementation gap between knowledge and action for health. B World Health Organization. 2004, 82: 724-732. Haines A, Kuruvilla S, Borchert M: Policy and Practice Bridging the implementation gap between knowledge and action for health. B World Health Organization. 2004, 82: 724-732.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Eakin EG, Glasgow RE, Riley KM: Review of primary care-based physical activity intervention studies. J Fam Practice. 2000, 49: 158-168. Eakin EG, Glasgow RE, Riley KM: Review of primary care-based physical activity intervention studies. J Fam Practice. 2000, 49: 158-168.
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Petrella RJ, Lattanzio CN: Does counseling help patients get practice? Systematic review of the literature. Can Fam Physician. 2002, 48: 72-80.PubMedPubMedCentral Petrella RJ, Lattanzio CN: Does counseling help patients get practice? Systematic review of the literature. Can Fam Physician. 2002, 48: 72-80.PubMedPubMedCentral
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Sørensen JB, Skovgaard T, Puggaard L: Exercise on prescription in general practice: a systematic review. Scand J Prim Health. 2006, 24: 69-74. 10.1080/02813430600700027.CrossRef Sørensen JB, Skovgaard T, Puggaard L: Exercise on prescription in general practice: a systematic review. Scand J Prim Health. 2006, 24: 69-74. 10.1080/02813430600700027.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Orrow G, Kinmonth A-L, Sanderson S, Sutton S: Effectiveness of physical activity promotion based in primary care: systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. BMJ. 2012, 344: e1389-10.1136/bmj.e1389.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Orrow G, Kinmonth A-L, Sanderson S, Sutton S: Effectiveness of physical activity promotion based in primary care: systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. BMJ. 2012, 344: e1389-10.1136/bmj.e1389.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Glasgow RE, Eakin EG, Fisher EB, Bacak SJ, Brownson RC: Physician advice and support for physical activity: results from a national survey. Am J Prev Med. 2001, 21: 189-196. 10.1016/S0749-3797(01)00350-6.CrossRefPubMed Glasgow RE, Eakin EG, Fisher EB, Bacak SJ, Brownson RC: Physician advice and support for physical activity: results from a national survey. Am J Prev Med. 2001, 21: 189-196. 10.1016/S0749-3797(01)00350-6.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Fallon EA, Wilcox S, Laken M: Health care provider advice for African American adults not meeting health behavior recommendations. Prev Chron Dis. 2006, 3: 1-12. Fallon EA, Wilcox S, Laken M: Health care provider advice for African American adults not meeting health behavior recommendations. Prev Chron Dis. 2006, 3: 1-12.
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Helmink JHM, Meis JJM, de Weerdt I, Visser FN, de Vries NK, Kremers SPJ: Development and implementation of a lifestyle intervention to promote physical activity and healthy diet in the Dutch general practice setting: the BeweegKuur programme. Int J Behav Nutr Phy. 2010, 7: 49-10.1186/1479-5868-7-49.CrossRef Helmink JHM, Meis JJM, de Weerdt I, Visser FN, de Vries NK, Kremers SPJ: Development and implementation of a lifestyle intervention to promote physical activity and healthy diet in the Dutch general practice setting: the BeweegKuur programme. Int J Behav Nutr Phy. 2010, 7: 49-10.1186/1479-5868-7-49.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Verheijden MW, Bakx JC, Delemarre ICG, Wanders AJ, Woudenbergh NM, Bottema BJAM, van Weel C, van Staveren WA: GPs’ assessment of patients’ readiness to change diet, activity and smoking. Brit J Gen Pract. 2005, 55: 452-457. Verheijden MW, Bakx JC, Delemarre ICG, Wanders AJ, Woudenbergh NM, Bottema BJAM, van Weel C, van Staveren WA: GPs’ assessment of patients’ readiness to change diet, activity and smoking. Brit J Gen Pract. 2005, 55: 452-457.
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Glasgow RE, Bull SS, Gilette C, Klesges LM, Dzewaltowski DA: Behavior change intervention research in healthcare settings: a review of recent reports with emphasis on external validity. Am J Prev Med. 2002, 23: 62-69. 10.1016/S0749-3797(02)00437-3.CrossRefPubMed Glasgow RE, Bull SS, Gilette C, Klesges LM, Dzewaltowski DA: Behavior change intervention research in healthcare settings: a review of recent reports with emphasis on external validity. Am J Prev Med. 2002, 23: 62-69. 10.1016/S0749-3797(02)00437-3.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Bartholomew KL, Parcel GS, Kok G, Gottlieb NH, Fernández ME: Planning Health Promotion Programs: An Intervention Mapping Approach. 2011, San Fransisco: Jossey-Bass Bartholomew KL, Parcel GS, Kok G, Gottlieb NH, Fernández ME: Planning Health Promotion Programs: An Intervention Mapping Approach. 2011, San Fransisco: Jossey-Bass
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Foy R, Eccles M, Grimshaw J: Why does primary care need more implementation research?. Fam Pract. 2001, 18: 353-355. 10.1093/fampra/18.4.353.CrossRefPubMed Foy R, Eccles M, Grimshaw J: Why does primary care need more implementation research?. Fam Pract. 2001, 18: 353-355. 10.1093/fampra/18.4.353.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Eccles M, Grimshaw J, Walker A, Johnston M, Pitts N: Changing the behavior of healthcare professionals: the use of theory in promoting the uptake of research findings. J Clin Epidemiol. 2005, 58: 107-112. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2004.09.002.CrossRefPubMed Eccles M, Grimshaw J, Walker A, Johnston M, Pitts N: Changing the behavior of healthcare professionals: the use of theory in promoting the uptake of research findings. J Clin Epidemiol. 2005, 58: 107-112. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2004.09.002.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Fleuren M, Wiefferink K, Paulussen T: Determinants of innovation within health care organizations: literature review and Delphi study. Int J Qual Health C. 2004, 16: 107-123. 10.1093/intqhc/mzh030.CrossRef Fleuren M, Wiefferink K, Paulussen T: Determinants of innovation within health care organizations: literature review and Delphi study. Int J Qual Health C. 2004, 16: 107-123. 10.1093/intqhc/mzh030.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Greenhalgh T, Robert G, Macfarlane F, Bate P, Kyriakidou O: Diffusion of innovations in service organizations: systematic review and recommendations. Milbank Q. 2004, 82: 581-629. 10.1111/j.0887-378X.2004.00325.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Greenhalgh T, Robert G, Macfarlane F, Bate P, Kyriakidou O: Diffusion of innovations in service organizations: systematic review and recommendations. Milbank Q. 2004, 82: 581-629. 10.1111/j.0887-378X.2004.00325.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Grol R, Wensing M, Eccles M: Improving Patient Care: The Implementation of Change in Clinical Practice. 2005, Oxford: Elsevier Grol R, Wensing M, Eccles M: Improving Patient Care: The Implementation of Change in Clinical Practice. 2005, Oxford: Elsevier
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Grol R, Grimshaw J: From best evidence to best practice: effective implementation of change in patients’ care. Lancet. 2003, 362: 1225-1230. 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)14546-1.CrossRefPubMed Grol R, Grimshaw J: From best evidence to best practice: effective implementation of change in patients’ care. Lancet. 2003, 362: 1225-1230. 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)14546-1.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Rogers EM: Diffusion of Innovations. 1983, New York: The Free Press Rogers EM: Diffusion of Innovations. 1983, New York: The Free Press
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Francis JJ, Tinmouth A, Stanworth SJ, Grimshaw JM, Johnston M, Hyde C, Stockton C, Brehaut JC, Fergusson D, Eccles MP: Using theories of behaviour to understand transfusion prescribing in three clinical contexts in two countries: development work for an implementation trial. Implement Sci. 2009, 4: 70-10.1186/1748-5908-4-70.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Francis JJ, Tinmouth A, Stanworth SJ, Grimshaw JM, Johnston M, Hyde C, Stockton C, Brehaut JC, Fergusson D, Eccles MP: Using theories of behaviour to understand transfusion prescribing in three clinical contexts in two countries: development work for an implementation trial. Implement Sci. 2009, 4: 70-10.1186/1748-5908-4-70.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Grol RPTM, Bosch MC, Hulscher MEJL, Eccles MP, Wensing M: Planning and studying improvement in patient care: the use of theoretical perspectives. Milbank Q. 2007, 85: 93-138. 10.1111/j.1468-0009.2007.00478.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Grol RPTM, Bosch MC, Hulscher MEJL, Eccles MP, Wensing M: Planning and studying improvement in patient care: the use of theoretical perspectives. Milbank Q. 2007, 85: 93-138. 10.1111/j.1468-0009.2007.00478.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Michie S, Johnston M, Francis J, Hardeman W, Eccles M: From theory to intervention: mapping theoretically derived behavioural determinants to behaviour change techniques. Appl Psychol. 2008, 57: 660-680. 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00341.x.CrossRef Michie S, Johnston M, Francis J, Hardeman W, Eccles M: From theory to intervention: mapping theoretically derived behavioural determinants to behaviour change techniques. Appl Psychol. 2008, 57: 660-680. 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00341.x.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Craig P, Dieppe P, Macintyre S, Michie S, Nazareth I, Petticrew M: Developing and evaluationg complex interventions: the new medical research council guidance. BMJ. 2008, 337: a1655-10.1136/bmj.a1655.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Craig P, Dieppe P, Macintyre S, Michie S, Nazareth I, Petticrew M: Developing and evaluationg complex interventions: the new medical research council guidance. BMJ. 2008, 337: a1655-10.1136/bmj.a1655.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Francis J, Johnston M, Bonetti D: Health psychology research advances theory by addressing problems: illustrations from theory-based studies of health professional behaviour. Eur Health Psychol. 2007, 2: 20-25. Francis J, Johnston M, Bonetti D: Health psychology research advances theory by addressing problems: illustrations from theory-based studies of health professional behaviour. Eur Health Psychol. 2007, 2: 20-25.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat French SD, Green SE, O’Connor DA, McKenzie JE, Francis JJ, Michie S, Buchbinder R, Schattner P, Spike N, Grimshaw JM: Developing theory-informed behaviour change interventions to implement evidence into practice: a systematic approach using the Theoretical Domains Framework. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 38-10.1186/1748-5908-7-38.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral French SD, Green SE, O’Connor DA, McKenzie JE, Francis JJ, Michie S, Buchbinder R, Schattner P, Spike N, Grimshaw JM: Developing theory-informed behaviour change interventions to implement evidence into practice: a systematic approach using the Theoretical Domains Framework. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 38-10.1186/1748-5908-7-38.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Presseau J, Johnston M, Francis J, Hrisos S, Stamp E, Steen N, Hawthorne G, Grimshaw J, Elovainio M, Hunter M, Eccles M: Theory-based predictors of multiple clinician behaviors in the management of diabetes. J Behav Med. 2013, epub ahead of print Presseau J, Johnston M, Francis J, Hrisos S, Stamp E, Steen N, Hawthorne G, Grimshaw J, Elovainio M, Hunter M, Eccles M: Theory-based predictors of multiple clinician behaviors in the management of diabetes. J Behav Med. 2013, epub ahead of print
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Chaudoir SR, Dugan AG, Barr CHI: Measuring factors affecting implementation of health innovations: a systematic review of structural, organizational, provider, patient, and innovation level. Implement Sci. 2013, 8: 22-10.1186/1748-5908-8-22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Chaudoir SR, Dugan AG, Barr CHI: Measuring factors affecting implementation of health innovations: a systematic review of structural, organizational, provider, patient, and innovation level. Implement Sci. 2013, 8: 22-10.1186/1748-5908-8-22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Rothman AJ: 'Is there nothing more practical than a good theory?’: why innovations and advances in health behavior change will arise if interventions are used to test and refine theory. Int J Behav Nutr Phy. 2004, 1: 11-10.1186/1479-5868-1-11.CrossRef Rothman AJ: 'Is there nothing more practical than a good theory?’: why innovations and advances in health behavior change will arise if interventions are used to test and refine theory. Int J Behav Nutr Phy. 2004, 1: 11-10.1186/1479-5868-1-11.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Francis JJ, Stockton C, Eccles MP, Johnston M, Cuthbertson BH, Grimshaw JM, Hyde C, Tinmouth A, Stanworth SJ: Evidence-based selection of theories for designing behaviour change interventions: using methods based on theoretical construct domains to understand clinicians’ blood transfusion behaviour. Brit J Health Psychol. 2009, 14: 625-646. 10.1348/135910708X397025.CrossRef Francis JJ, Stockton C, Eccles MP, Johnston M, Cuthbertson BH, Grimshaw JM, Hyde C, Tinmouth A, Stanworth SJ: Evidence-based selection of theories for designing behaviour change interventions: using methods based on theoretical construct domains to understand clinicians’ blood transfusion behaviour. Brit J Health Psychol. 2009, 14: 625-646. 10.1348/135910708X397025.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Michie S, Johnston M, Abraham C, Lawton R, Parker D, Walker A: Making psychological theory useful for implementing evidence based practice: a consensus approach. Qual Saf Health Care. 2005, 14: 26-33. 10.1136/qshc.2004.011155.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Michie S, Johnston M, Abraham C, Lawton R, Parker D, Walker A: Making psychological theory useful for implementing evidence based practice: a consensus approach. Qual Saf Health Care. 2005, 14: 26-33. 10.1136/qshc.2004.011155.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Patey AM, Islam R, Francis JJ, Bryson GL, Grimshaw JM: Anesthesiologists’ And surgeons’ perceptions about routine pre-operative testing in low-risk patients: application of the theoretical domains framework (TDF) to identify factors that influence physicians’ decisions to order pre-operative tests. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 52-10.1186/1748-5908-7-52.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Patey AM, Islam R, Francis JJ, Bryson GL, Grimshaw JM: Anesthesiologists’ And surgeons’ perceptions about routine pre-operative testing in low-risk patients: application of the theoretical domains framework (TDF) to identify factors that influence physicians’ decisions to order pre-operative tests. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 52-10.1186/1748-5908-7-52.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
33.
Zurück zum Zitat McSherry LA, Dombrowski SU, Francis JJ, Murphy J, Martin CM, O’Leary JJ, Sharp L, ATHENS Group: 'It’s a can of worms’: understanding primary care practitioners’ behaviours in relation to HPV using the theoretical domains framework. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 73-10.1186/1748-5908-7-73.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral McSherry LA, Dombrowski SU, Francis JJ, Murphy J, Martin CM, O’Leary JJ, Sharp L, ATHENS Group: 'It’s a can of worms’: understanding primary care practitioners’ behaviours in relation to HPV using the theoretical domains framework. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 73-10.1186/1748-5908-7-73.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Amemori M, Michie S, Korhonen T, Murtomaa H, Kinnunen TH: Assessing implementation difficulties in tobacco use prevention and cessation counselling among dental providers. Implement Sci. 2011, 6: 50-10.1186/1748-5908-6-50.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Amemori M, Michie S, Korhonen T, Murtomaa H, Kinnunen TH: Assessing implementation difficulties in tobacco use prevention and cessation counselling among dental providers. Implement Sci. 2011, 6: 50-10.1186/1748-5908-6-50.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Beenstock J, Sniehotta FF, White M, Bell R, Milne EM, Araujo-Soares V: What helps and hinders midwives in engaging with pregnant women about stopping smoking? A cross-sectional survey of perceived implementation difficulties among midwives in the northeast of England. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 36-10.1186/1748-5908-7-36.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Beenstock J, Sniehotta FF, White M, Bell R, Milne EM, Araujo-Soares V: What helps and hinders midwives in engaging with pregnant women about stopping smoking? A cross-sectional survey of perceived implementation difficulties among midwives in the northeast of England. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 36-10.1186/1748-5908-7-36.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor N, Parveen S, Robins V, Slater B, Lawton R: Development and initial validation of the Influences on Patient Safety Behaviours Questionnaire. Implement Sci. 2013, 8: 81-10.1186/1748-5908-8-81.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Taylor N, Parveen S, Robins V, Slater B, Lawton R: Development and initial validation of the Influences on Patient Safety Behaviours Questionnaire. Implement Sci. 2013, 8: 81-10.1186/1748-5908-8-81.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Cane J, O’Connor D, Michie S: Validation of the theoretical domains framework for use in behaviour change and implementation research. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 37-10.1186/1748-5908-7-37.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Cane J, O’Connor D, Michie S: Validation of the theoretical domains framework for use in behaviour change and implementation research. Implement Sci. 2012, 7: 37-10.1186/1748-5908-7-37.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Fishbein M: Attitude and the Prediction of Behavior. Readings in Atttiude Theory and Measurement. Edited by: Fishbein M. 1967, New York: Wiley, 477-492. Fishbein M: Attitude and the Prediction of Behavior. Readings in Atttiude Theory and Measurement. Edited by: Fishbein M. 1967, New York: Wiley, 477-492.
39.
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Rubio DM, Berg-Weger M, Tebb SS, Lee ES, Rauch S: Objectifying content validity: conducting a content validity study in social work research. Soc Work Res. 2003, 27: 94-104. 10.1093/swr/27.2.94.CrossRef Rubio DM, Berg-Weger M, Tebb SS, Lee ES, Rauch S: Objectifying content validity: conducting a content validity study in social work research. Soc Work Res. 2003, 27: 94-104. 10.1093/swr/27.2.94.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Ajzen I: The theory of planned behavior. Organ Behav Hum Dec. 1991, 50: 179-211. 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T.CrossRef Ajzen I: The theory of planned behavior. Organ Behav Hum Dec. 1991, 50: 179-211. 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T.CrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Bandura A: Health promotion from the perspective of social cognitive theory. Psychol Health. 1998, 13: 623-649. 10.1080/08870449808407422.CrossRef Bandura A: Health promotion from the perspective of social cognitive theory. Psychol Health. 1998, 13: 623-649. 10.1080/08870449808407422.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Huijg JM, van der Zouwe N, Gebhardt WA, Crone MR, Verheijden MW, Middelkoop BJC: Introducing physical activity interventions in primary health care: a qualitative study of perceived facilitators and barriers [abstract]. Int J Behav Med. 2012, 19: S1-S341. Huijg JM, van der Zouwe N, Gebhardt WA, Crone MR, Verheijden MW, Middelkoop BJC: Introducing physical activity interventions in primary health care: a qualitative study of perceived facilitators and barriers [abstract]. Int J Behav Med. 2012, 19: S1-S341.
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Huijg JM, Crone MR, Verheijden MW, van der Zouwe N, Middelkoop BJC, Gebhardt WA: Factors influencing the adoption, implementation, and continuation of physical activity interventions in primary health care: a delphi study. BMC Fam Pract. 2013, 14: 142-10.1186/1471-2296-14-142.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Huijg JM, Crone MR, Verheijden MW, van der Zouwe N, Middelkoop BJC, Gebhardt WA: Factors influencing the adoption, implementation, and continuation of physical activity interventions in primary health care: a delphi study. BMC Fam Pract. 2013, 14: 142-10.1186/1471-2296-14-142.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Maes S, Karoly P, de Gucht V, Ruehlman LS, Heiser W: The self regulation skills battery. 2006, Leiden/Phoenix: Leiden University/Arizona State University Maes S, Karoly P, de Gucht V, Ruehlman LS, Heiser W: The self regulation skills battery. 2006, Leiden/Phoenix: Leiden University/Arizona State University
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Sniehotta FF, Schwarzer R, Scholz U, Schüz B: Action planning and coping planning for long-term lifestyle change: theory and assessment. Eur J Soc Psycho. 2005, 35: 565-576. 10.1002/ejsp.258.CrossRef Sniehotta FF, Schwarzer R, Scholz U, Schüz B: Action planning and coping planning for long-term lifestyle change: theory and assessment. Eur J Soc Psycho. 2005, 35: 565-576. 10.1002/ejsp.258.CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Gardner B, Abraham C, Lally P, de Bruijn G-J: Towards parsimony in habit measurement: Testing the convergent and predictive validity of an automaticity subscale of the Self-Report Habit Index. Int J Behav Nutr Phys. 2012, 9: 102-10.1186/1479-5868-9-102.CrossRef Gardner B, Abraham C, Lally P, de Bruijn G-J: Towards parsimony in habit measurement: Testing the convergent and predictive validity of an automaticity subscale of the Self-Report Habit Index. Int J Behav Nutr Phys. 2012, 9: 102-10.1186/1479-5868-9-102.CrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Goldberg DP, Blackwell B: Psychiatric illness in general practice: a detailed study using a new method of case identification. Brit Med J. 1970, 1: 439-443.CrossRefPubMed Goldberg DP, Blackwell B: Psychiatric illness in general practice: a detailed study using a new method of case identification. Brit Med J. 1970, 1: 439-443.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Thompson ER: Development and Validation of an Internationally Reliable Short-Form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). J Cross Cult Psychol. 2007, 38: 227-242. 10.1177/0022022106297301.CrossRef Thompson ER: Development and Validation of an Internationally Reliable Short-Form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). J Cross Cult Psychol. 2007, 38: 227-242. 10.1177/0022022106297301.CrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Frese M: Social support as a moderator of the relationship between work stressors and psychological dysfunctioning: a longitudinal study with objective measures. J Occup Health Psych. 1999, 4: 179-192.CrossRef Frese M: Social support as a moderator of the relationship between work stressors and psychological dysfunctioning: a longitudinal study with objective measures. J Occup Health Psych. 1999, 4: 179-192.CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Derryberry D, Reed MA: Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002, 111: 225-236.CrossRefPubMed Derryberry D, Reed MA: Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002, 111: 225-236.CrossRefPubMed
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Scheier MF, Carver CS, Bridges MW: Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): a reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994, 67: 1063-1078.CrossRefPubMed Scheier MF, Carver CS, Bridges MW: Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): a reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994, 67: 1063-1078.CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Cialdini R, Kallgren C, Reno R: A Focus Theory of Normative Conduct: A Theoretical Refinement and Reevaluation of the Role of Norms in Human Behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 1991, San Diego: Academic Press, 202-232. Cialdini R, Kallgren C, Reno R: A Focus Theory of Normative Conduct: A Theoretical Refinement and Reevaluation of the Role of Norms in Human Behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 1991, San Diego: Academic Press, 202-232.
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Dixon D, Pollard B, Johnston M: What does the chronic pain grade questionnaire measure?. Pain. 2007, 130: 249-253. 10.1016/j.pain.2006.12.004.CrossRefPubMed Dixon D, Pollard B, Johnston M: What does the chronic pain grade questionnaire measure?. Pain. 2007, 130: 249-253. 10.1016/j.pain.2006.12.004.CrossRefPubMed
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Dixon D, Johnston M, McQueen M, Court-Brown C: The disabilities of the Arm, shoulder and hand questionnaire (DASH) can measure the impairment, activity limitations and participation restriction constructs from the international classification of functioning, disability and health (ICF). BMC Musculoskel Dis. 2008, 9: 114-10.1186/1471-2474-9-114.CrossRef Dixon D, Johnston M, McQueen M, Court-Brown C: The disabilities of the Arm, shoulder and hand questionnaire (DASH) can measure the impairment, activity limitations and participation restriction constructs from the international classification of functioning, disability and health (ICF). BMC Musculoskel Dis. 2008, 9: 114-10.1186/1471-2474-9-114.CrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychological Association (APA): APA Dictionary of Psychology. 2007, Washington: American Psychological Association American Psychological Association (APA): APA Dictionary of Psychology. 2007, Washington: American Psychological Association
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y: Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J Roy Stat Soc B Met. 1995, 57: 289-300. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y: Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J Roy Stat Soc B Met. 1995, 57: 289-300.
58.
Zurück zum Zitat IBM Corp: IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 19.0. 2010, Armonk, NY: IBM Corp IBM Corp: IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 19.0. 2010, Armonk, NY: IBM Corp
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Conger A: Integration and generalization of kappas for multiple raters. Psychol Bull. 1980, 88: 322-328.CrossRef Conger A: Integration and generalization of kappas for multiple raters. Psychol Bull. 1980, 88: 322-328.CrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Landis JR, Koch GG: The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977, 33: 159-174. 10.2307/2529310.CrossRefPubMed Landis JR, Koch GG: The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977, 33: 159-174. 10.2307/2529310.CrossRefPubMed
61.
Zurück zum Zitat R Development Core Team R: R: a Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. 2012, Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing R Development Core Team R: R: a Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. 2012, Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Falissard B: Psy: Various Procedures Used in Psychometry. 2012, : R Package Version 1.1 Falissard B: Psy: Various Procedures Used in Psychometry. 2012, : R Package Version 1.1
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor N, Lawton R, Connor M: Development and initial validation of the determinants of physical activity questionnaire. Int J Behav Nutr Phy. 2013, 10: 74-10.1186/1479-5868-10-74.CrossRef Taylor N, Lawton R, Connor M: Development and initial validation of the determinants of physical activity questionnaire. Int J Behav Nutr Phy. 2013, 10: 74-10.1186/1479-5868-10-74.CrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Stevens J: Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences. 1992, Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Stevens J: Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences. 1992, Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor N, Lawton R, Slater B, Foy R: The demonstration of a theory-based approach to the design of localized patient safety interventions. Implement Sci. 2013, 8: 123-10.1186/1748-5908-8-123.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Taylor N, Lawton R, Slater B, Foy R: The demonstration of a theory-based approach to the design of localized patient safety interventions. Implement Sci. 2013, 8: 123-10.1186/1748-5908-8-123.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Dyson J, Lawton R, Jackson C, Cheater F: Development of a theory-based instrument to identify barriers and levers to best hand hygiene practice among healthcare practitioners. Implement Sci. 2013, 8: 111-10.1186/1748-5908-8-111.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Dyson J, Lawton R, Jackson C, Cheater F: Development of a theory-based instrument to identify barriers and levers to best hand hygiene practice among healthcare practitioners. Implement Sci. 2013, 8: 111-10.1186/1748-5908-8-111.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Dyson J, Lawton R, Jackson C, Cheater F: Does the use of a theoretical approach tell us more about hand hygiene behavior? The barriers and levers to hand hygiene. J Infect Prev. 2011, 12: 17-24. 10.1177/1757177410384300.CrossRef Dyson J, Lawton R, Jackson C, Cheater F: Does the use of a theoretical approach tell us more about hand hygiene behavior? The barriers and levers to hand hygiene. J Infect Prev. 2011, 12: 17-24. 10.1177/1757177410384300.CrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Bonetti D, Johnston M, Clarkson JE, Grimshaw J, Pitts NB, Eccles M, Steen N, Thomas R, Maclennan G, Glidewell L, Walker A: Applying psychologial theories to evidence-based clinical practice: identifying factors predictive of placing preventive fissure sealants. Implement Sci. 2010, 5: 25-10.1186/1748-5908-5-25.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Bonetti D, Johnston M, Clarkson JE, Grimshaw J, Pitts NB, Eccles M, Steen N, Thomas R, Maclennan G, Glidewell L, Walker A: Applying psychologial theories to evidence-based clinical practice: identifying factors predictive of placing preventive fissure sealants. Implement Sci. 2010, 5: 25-10.1186/1748-5908-5-25.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Davies P, Walker AE, Grimshaw JM: A systematic review of the use of theory in the design of guideline dissemination and implementation strategies and interpretation of the results of rigorous evaluations. Implement Sci. 2010, 5: 14-10.1186/1748-5908-5-14.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Davies P, Walker AE, Grimshaw JM: A systematic review of the use of theory in the design of guideline dissemination and implementation strategies and interpretation of the results of rigorous evaluations. Implement Sci. 2010, 5: 14-10.1186/1748-5908-5-14.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Grimshaw JM, Thomas RE, MacLennan G, Fraser C, Ramsay CR, Vale L, Whitty P, Eccles MP, Matowe L, Shirran L, Wensing M, Dijkstra R, Donaldson C: Effectiveness and efficiency of guideline dissemination and implementation strategies. Health Technol Assess. 2004, 8: 1-72.CrossRef Grimshaw JM, Thomas RE, MacLennan G, Fraser C, Ramsay CR, Vale L, Whitty P, Eccles MP, Matowe L, Shirran L, Wensing M, Dijkstra R, Donaldson C: Effectiveness and efficiency of guideline dissemination and implementation strategies. Health Technol Assess. 2004, 8: 1-72.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Discriminant content validity of a theoretical domains framework questionnaire for use in implementation research
verfasst von
Johanna M Huijg
Winifred A Gebhardt
Mathilde R Crone
Elise Dusseldorp
Justin Presseau
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2014
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Implementation Science / Ausgabe 1/2014
Elektronische ISSN: 1748-5908
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1748-5908-9-11

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2014

Implementation Science 1/2014 Zur Ausgabe