Skip to main content
Erschienen in: European Journal of Epidemiology 9/2013

Open Access 01.09.2013 | MORTALITY

Adverse childhood experiences and premature all-cause mortality

verfasst von: Michelle Kelly-Irving, Benoit Lepage, Dominique Dedieu, Mel Bartley, David Blane, Pascale Grosclaude, Thierry Lang, Cyrille Delpierre

Erschienen in: European Journal of Epidemiology | Ausgabe 9/2013

Abstract

Events causing stress responses during sensitive periods of rapid neurological development in childhood may be early determinants of all-cause premature mortality. Using a British birth cohort study of individuals born in 1958, the relationship between adverse childhood experiences (ACE) and mortality ≤50 year was examined for men (n = 7,816) and women (n = 7,405) separately. ACE were measured using prospectively collected reports from parents and the school: no adversities (70 %); one adversity (22 %), two or more adversities (8 %). A Cox regression model was carried out controlling for early life variables and for characteristics at 23 years. In men the risk of death was 57 % higher among those who had experienced 2+ ACE compared to those with none (HR 1.57, 95 % CI 1.13, 2.18, p = 0.007). In women, a graded relationship was observed between ACE and mortality, the risk increasing as ACE accumulated. Women with one ACE had a 66 % increased risk of death (HR 1.66, 95 % CI 1.19, 2.33, p = 0.003) and those with ≥2 ACE had an 80 % increased risk (HR 1.80, 95 % CI 1.10, 2.95, p = 0.020) versus those with no ACE. Given the small impact of adult life style factors on the association between ACE and premature mortality, biological embedding during sensitive periods in early development is a plausible explanatory mechanism.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1007/​s10654-013-9832-9) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Introduction

Early life exposure to adverse childhood experiences (ACE), like trauma, abuse or maltreatment in childhood has been linked to alteration of the brain structure and the neurobiological stress-response systems which have consequences for health and emotional well-being [1]. The hypothesis being tested in this paper is based on an ecobiodevelopmental approach postulating that early adverse events causing toxic stress responses [2], occurring during sensitive periods of rapid neurological and cognitive development in childhood, may be early determinants of all-cause premature mortality. Three overarching and intertwining pathways across the lifecourse may lead towards premature mortality: (a) a health behaviours pathway, whereby individuals adopt stress-reducing behaviours, (b) a direct physiological pathway, via alterations to neuroendocrine, inflammatory, immune and epigenetic mechanisms, (c) and a socioeconomic/materialist pathway, via toxic environmental exposures—or an intertwining of the three.
The burden of mortality is unequally spread across social groups [3, 4]. Disadvantage in early life has been considered an important determinant of mortality for many years, but the variables used to characterize childhood circumstances were often non-specific and the mechanisms involved remain to be clarified. Galobardes et al. [5] have shown that the mechanisms of sensitive periods and accumulation, as underlined in the life course epidemiology framework [6], are likely to link early life conditions to adult mortality [5, 7]. Premature death, conceptualised as avoidable mortality [8], is characterized both by chronic diseases [9] and by external and often violent causes, including accidents, self-harm or alcohol-related deaths often implicating psychological distress.
Stressful events are likely to be experienced differently depending on an individual’s position on the social gradient. Individuals lower on the social gradient may be more vulnerable to the physiological or behavioural effects of stressful environmental exposures with fewer resources and coping strategies at their disposal compared to individuals with a higher social position [10]. Intra-familial conditions or events occurring from conception into adolescence may program physiological responses during sensitive periods of development, altering an individual’s biology, rendering them poorly adapted to their environment and subsequent exposures later in life [11, 12].
The objective of this study is to examine the relationship between adversity in childhood and adult premature mortality whilst also considering the effects of material disadvantage, health status, health behaviours and education level using a large prospective cohort study.

Methods

Data are from the 1958 National Child Development Study (NCDS) which included all live births during 1 week in 1958 (n = 18,555) in Great Britain. Subsequent data collections (sweeps) were carried out on cohort members aged 7, 11, 16, 23, 33, 42, 46 and 50. At the last sweep, carried out in 2008, 9,790 individuals participated in the self-reported questionnaire and face to face interview, representing 53 % of the original sample. The NCDS has been described in detail elsewhere [13]. Follow-up has been good over time, 84 % participated at the age of 16, with a gradual dwindling in participation throughout adulthood (72 % at age 23, and 65 % at age 42) when participants were most likely to have moved [13]. Refusal rates have been low, with 7 % at age 23 and 13 % at age 42 for example [13].

Adverse childhood experiences (ACE)

In this study we have identified ACE as a set of traumatic and stressful psychosocial conditions that are out of the child’s control, that tend to co-occur [14] and often persist over time [15, 16]. Our definition of ACE is: intra-familial events or conditions causing chronic stress responses in the child’s immediate environment. These include notions of maltreatment and deviation from societal norms, where possible to be distinguished from conditions in the socioeconomic and material environment. The NCDS has an immense wealth of prospectively collected data. ACE have already been extracted from these datasets and used to examine their impact on psychopathology [15, 17]. There are many ways in which the adversity can be conceptualised, including financial difficulties, family dissention, and child physical or sexual abuse. Here, we have attempted to construct a theoretical framework to define ACE prior to extracting any data. Our definition has been influenced by previous epidemiological studies of ACE, notably the San Diego study [18] the Australian study [14], other more general studies related to adversity [19, 20] as well as discussions on ACE by a WHO expert committee [21] in 2009.
Information was extracted on events and conditions that corresponded with the above definition and previous literature from the NCDS datasets. Data were collected in childhood at 7, 11 and 16 years of age from questions posed to the child’s parent or their teacher. Sources of adversity were divided into six categories:
1.
Child in care: child has ever been in public/voluntary care services or foster care at age 7, 11 or 16.
 
2.
Physical neglect: child appears undernourished/dirty aged 7 or 11, information collected from the response from child’s teacher to the Bristol Social Adjustment Guide.
 
Household dysfunction, as described by Felitti et al. [16], is a dimension of adversity consisting of four categories each contributing to the score:
3.
Offenders: The child lived in a household where a family member (person living in the same household as the child) was in prison or on probation (age 11 years) or is in contact with probation service at 7 or 11 years; the child has ever been to prison or been on probation at 16 years.
 
4.
Parental separation: The child has been separated from their father or mother due to death, divorce, or separation at 7, 11 or 16 years.
 
5.
Mental illness: Household has contact with mental health services at 7 or 11 years; Family member has mental illness at 7 and 11 or 16 years.
 
6.
Alcohol abuse: Family member has alcohol abuse problem at 7 years.
 
Exposure to adversity was identified by a positive response to any of the above categories. Respondents were excluded if they had missing data for all six categories. Respondents were considered as having no adversities if they answered ‘no’ all the categories or if they answered ‘no’ to one or more category and the other categories were missing. The main reason for this was to maximise the power of the study by including individuals if they provided any information on adversity variables. Using this construction the variable is a conservative one, whereby the ‘no’ category may consist of some misclassified individuals with adversities. ACE was measured by counting the reports of: child in care, physical neglect, offenders, parental separation, mental illness and alcohol abuse. A three category variable was then constructed (no adversities/one adversity/two or more adversities).

Prior confounders

To examine the relationship between ACE and premature death, prior confounding variables potentially associated with both ACE and mortality were adjusted for in the multivariate models. Among the variables available at baseline, collected from the cohort member’s mothers via a questionnaire at birth, we identified those most likely to be social or biological confounding factors. Household and parental characteristics were included: mother’s age at birth, overcrowding (people-per-room), mother’s partner’s social class (manual/non-manual), and if this was unavailable the mother’s father’s social class was used, mother’s education level (left school before/after minimum leaving age), and maternal smoking during pregnancy (no smoking/sometimes/often/heavy). The respondent’s characteristics and birth variables were also included: sex (male/female), gestational age (calculated as the duration between the first day of the mother’s last menstrual period and childbirth/using the first day of the last menstrual period, and categorized into the following groups: ≤38 weeks, 39–41 weeks, >41 weeks), parity (mother’s number of previous pregnancies, including miscarriges after 28 weeks), birthweight measured at birth (transformed from pounds and ounces into kilograms), and breastfeeding reported by mother (no/1 month or less/more than 1 month). To control for health problems in childhood, a child pathologies variable was constructed using data collected at 7, 11 and 16 years of age. It took into account both mother’s report and medical examinations including congenital conditions, moderate/severe disabilities, chronic respiratory or circulatory conditions, sensory impairments and special schooling (childhood pathology: yes/no).

Mediators across the lifecourse

In order to determine whether any observed associations between adversity and mortality were due to subsequent adult mediating factors, these were added to the model: respondent’s educational attainment at 23 years (high-school diploma/middle-school diploma/no qualification), respondent’s occupational social class at 23 years (non-manual active/manual active/inactive). The ‘malaise inventory’ was used to identify symptoms of depression at the age of 23. It was based on a set of 24 questions identifying symptoms, if the respondent reported experiencing more than 7 of the symptoms they were considered as having psychological malaise (no malaise/malaise), characterized by symptoms of depression and/or anxiety. The health behaviour variables included were: alcohol consumption at 23 years (normal drinking (women: between 1 and 14 units in the previous week, men: between 1 and 21 units in the previous week)/abstinence (reported not consuming any alcohol in the previous week)/heavy drinking (women: >14 units in the previous week, men: >21 units in the previous week [22]); smoking status at 23 years (never smoked/past smoker/current smoker), and BMI (kg/m2) as a continuous variable based on height and weight measurements at 23 years [4]. Adult life-style variables are available at other points along the lifecourse, however in our model, these adult variables at the age of 23 are a proxy for behavioural patterns in early adulthood and controlling for them serves as a first step to understanding possible mechanisms.

All-cause premature mortality

Deaths were ascertained systematically through receipt of death certificates to the Centre for Longitudinal Studies from the National Health Service Central Register. The mortality data currently available to researchers provided information on date-of-death up to December 2008. Since information on death was obtained from the register, even when individuals were lost to follow-up in the cohort, information on their death will have been received. The baseline sample includes participants who were alive from 16 years (1974).

Subsamples, missing data and imputation

For the purposes of these analyses individuals who died during or before information on the exposure of interest was collected, or those with missing data on all ACE (15 % of n = 18,555) categories were excluded. When compared to the rest of the sample (n = 15,221) using the variables collected at baseline, individuals in each group did not differ in terms of mother’s education, mother’s partner’s occupation, or breastfeeding. Respondents who were excluded from the analyses (see additional table a) were more likely to be men (53.4 vs 51.4 %, p = 0.033), to come from overcrowded households (15.9 % vs 14.2 %, p = 0.018), more likely to be first born or from single child families (39.6 vs 36.2 %, p = 0.003), their mothers were more likely to have been smokers in pregnancy (66.5 vs 65.5 %, p = 0.012). Their birthweight was 240 g lighter on average (p < 0.0001), and their gestational age 6.3 days earlier on average (p < 0.0001).
To control for possible bias due to missing data, we imputed data for covariates with missing data using the multiple imputation by chained equations method (using ICE in STATA v11). Twenty imputations were conducted taking the missing at random (MAR) assumption for the covariates: mother’s education, father’s social class, overcrowding, birthweight, gestational age, parity, smoking during pregnancy, mother’s age at birth, breastfeeding, child pathologies, educational attainment at 23, social class at 23, malaise inventory at 23, drinking at age 23 and smoking at age 23. Neither the exposure variable of interest (ACE) nor the outcome were included in the multivariate imputation model. Comparisons were made between complete-case analyses and those run on estimates obtained by imputation. The models yielded the same results until the inclusion of variables at age 23 (model 4). The differences observed in the results for model 4 indicate selection bias in the complete case sample, where individuals who had experienced ACE in childhood were more likely to have missing data at age 23. The multiple imputation model therefore enables adjustment for this bias.

Statistical analyses

Cox regression was used to estimate hazard ratios (HR) for the associations between ACE and mortality controlling for early life potential confounders (socioeconomic, birth and pregnancy characteristics) and adult life potential mediating factors. Participants were censored at the date of death. The proportional hazard assumptions associated with Cox regression were tested separately for each gender on the full model for each imputation and found not to be violated at p = 0.05. All analyses were performed using STATA V11. Four models were run separately by gender, entering the variables in chronological order as they would have occurred over the lifecourse. First the association between early life socioeconomic circumstances and mortality risk was tested. Variables related to perinatal conditions were entered along with childhood pathologies in model 2. In model 3 ACE was added. Finally, model 4 additionally controlled for education, social class, psychological malaise and health behaviours at 23 years.

Results

Given the differences between men and women in premature mortality [8], and the possible sex-specific differences regarding stress [23], the analyses were run separately on 7,816 men and 7,405 women, their overall characteristics are described in Table 1. Among men, 4.1 % of the cohort had died between the age of 16 and 50, versus 2.4 % for women. In Table 2 the distribution of all cause premature mortality by age group is shown. Over half the observed deaths occurred after the age of 40 for both men and women.
Table 1
Frequencies and percentages for men and women in the National Child Development Study born in 1958
Variables
Categories
Male (7,816)
Female (7,405)
 
Mother’s education level % (n)
Left school at 15 or later
22.9 (1,788)
23.9 (1,768)
0.165
 
Left school before 14
72.4 (5,656)
71.6 (5,301)
 
 
Missing
4.8 (372)
4.5 (336)
 
Father’s social class at birth % (n)
Non-manual
25.3 (1,979)
25.8 (1,907)
0.652
 
Manual
69.1 (5,403)
69.1 (5,119)
 
 
Missing
5.6 (434)
5.1 (379)
 
Overcrowding % (n)
<1.5 people per room
80.3 (6,275)
80.0 (5,925)
0.314
 
≥1.5 people per room
13.0 (1,012)
13.5 (1,003)
 
 
Missing
6.8 (529)
6.4 (477)
 
Parity % (n)
Primiparous
34.4 (2,689)
35.0 (2,594)
0.237
 
2 children
30.4 (2,375)
29.2 (2,162)
 
 
3 or more
31.0 (2,426)
31.8 (2,353)
 
 
Missing
4.2 (326)
4.0 (296)
 
Mother smoked during pregnancy % (n)
No
63.0 (4,926)
63.1 (4,673)
0.422
 
Sometimes
5.7 (449)
5.3 (391)
 
 
Moderately
14.4 (1,125)
15.1 (1,117)
 
 
Heavily
11.6 (903)
11.4 (843)
 
 
Missing
5.3 (413)
5.2 (381)
 
Breastfed % (n)
Yes, for more than 1 month
39.3 (3,074)
40.3 (2,986)
0.304
 
Yes, for up to one month
22.5 (1,761)
23.1 (1,708)
 
 
No
29.6 (2,312)
28.7 (2,122)
 
 
Missing
8.6 (669)
8.0 (589)
 
Childhood illness % (n)
No
58.4 (4,563)
62.3 (4,613)
<0.001
 
Yes
24.5 (1,911)
19.7 (1,455)
 
 
Missing
17.2 (1,342)
18.1 (1,337)
 
ACE % (n)
0
68.5 (5,357)
71.9 (5,321)
<0.001
 
1
22.2 (1,735)
21.1 (1,561)
 
 
≥2
9.3 (724)
7.1 (523)
 
Child in care % (n)
No
52.6 (4,115)
54.0 (4,000)
0.033
 
Yes
4.9 (386)
4.3 (317)
 
 
Missing
42.4 (3,315)
41.7 (3,088)
 
Family member on probation or prison % (n)
No
62.0 (4,849)
65.1 (4,817)
<0.001
 
Yes
11.9 (930)
7.7 (570)
 
 
Missing
26.1 (2,037)
27.3 (2,018)
 
Separation from parent(s) % (n)
No
59.9 (4,682)
59.6 (4,411)
0.445
 
Yes
13.3 (1,041)
13.8 (1,018)
 
 
Missing
26.8 (2,093)
26.7 (1,976)
 
Family member with mental health
No
74.8 (5,843)
75.8 (5,612)
0.996
Problem % (n)
Yes
5.5 (426)
5.5 (409)
 
 
Missing
19.8 (1,547)
18.7 (1,384)
 
Family member has substance abuse
No
84.0 (6,563)
85.8 (6,350)
0.718
Problem % (n)
Yes
0.8 (64)
0.9 (66)
 
 
Missing
15.2 (1,189)
13.4 (989)
 
Child appears physically neglected % (n)
No
82.5 (6,446)
85.3 (6,318)
<0.001
 
Yes
7.8 (613)
5.5 (405)
 
 
Missing
9.7 (757)
9.2 (682)
 
Education level at 23 % (n)
Passed A levels
15.9 (1,243)
16.8 (1,245)
<0.001
 
Passed O levels
26.8 (2,093)
33.9 (2,510)
 
 
No qualifications
31.4 (2,454)
28.0 (2,071)
 
 
Missing
25.9 (2,026)
21.3 (1,579)
 
Social class at 23 % (n)
Non-manual active
24.7 (1,931)
39.7 (2,941)
<0.001
 
Manual active
34.6 (2,705)
10.7 (795)
 
 
Inactive
12.8 (998)
28.0 (2,075)
 
 
Missing
27.9 (2,182)
21.5 (1,594)
 
Smoking status age 23 % (n)
Never smoked
21.0 (1,638)
26.0 (1,923)
<0.001
Former smoker
22.6 (1,765)
20.8 (1,537)
 
 
Smoker
30.6 (2,391)
32.0 (2,371)
 
 
Missing
25.9 (2,022)
21.3 (1,574)
 
Alcohol consumption age 23 % (n)
Normal
49.5 (3,870)
43.6 (3,226)
 
Abstinence
3.7 (285)
6.7 (497)
<0.001
 
Heavy
14.0 (1,097)
6.6 (487)
 
 
Missing
32.8 (2,564)
43.2 (3,195)
 
Malaise inventory age 23 % (n)
None identified
70.8 (5,534)
69.8 (5,172)
<0.001
 
Malaise
3.1 (239)
8.6 (640)
 
 
Missing
26.1 (2,043)
21.5 (1,593)
 
Mortality
Alive
95.9 (7,493)
97.6 (7,225)
<0.001
 
Dead
4.1 (323)
2.4 (180)
 
Continuous variables
    
Birthweight (kg)
Mean (95 % CI)
3.41 (3.40–3.43)
3.27 (2.26–3.29)
<0.001
 
Missing % (n)
10.0 (959)
9.1 (816)
 
Gestational age (days)
Mean (95 % CI)
280.86 (280.51–281.21)
281.36 (281.02–281.70)
0.006
 
Missing % (n)
15.9 (1,529)
16.3 (1,529)
 
Mother’s age at birth (years)
Mean (95 % CI)
27.63 (27.47–27.78)
27.70 (27.55–27.86)
0.550
 
Missing % (n)
6.3 (601)
6.2 (555)
 
BMI age 23 (kg/m2)
Mean (95 % CI)
23.33 (23.24–23.42)
22.14 (22.05–22.23)
<0.001
 
Missing % (n)
35.9 (3,443)
31.2 (2,793)
 
Table 2
Percentage deaths by sex and age in the 1958 birth cohort study
Age groups
Male % (n)
Female % (n)
Total % (n)
16–19
8.36 (27)
5.56 (10)
7.36 (37)
20–24
6.19 (20)
7.78 (14)
6.76 (34)
25–29
8.98 (29)
5.56 (10)
7.75 (39)
30–34
7.74 (25)
6.11 (11)
7.19 (36)
35–39
10.53 (34)
15.56 (28)
12.33 (62)
40–44
23.22 (75)
22.78 (41)
23.06 (116)
45–50
34.98 (113)
36.67 (66)
35.59 (179)
Total
100 (323)
100 (180)
100 (503)
Descriptive and multivariate analyses are shown using the imputed data (Tables 3, 4, 5). In both men and women, a graded increase in the proportion of respondents reporting ACE is observed among the deceased, the gradient being steeper in men (Table 3). Figures 1 and 2 shows the proportion surviving by ACE group, outlining the significant decrease in survival between individuals with no ACE, one ACE or 2 or more ACE (log rank p < 0.0001).
Table 3
Descriptive analyses by all cause mortality in men and women
Variables
Categories
Male
p
Female
p
Alive (%)
Died before 50 (%)
Alive (%)
Died before 50 (%)
Sex
 
95.9
4.1
<0.0001
97.6
2.4
 
ACE
0
96.4
3.6
 
98.0
2.0
 
 
1
95.6
4.4
0.106
96.5
3.5
0.001
 
≥2
92.5
7.5
<0.0001
96.0
4.0
0.003
Mother’s education level
Left school at 15 or later
96.2
3.8
 
98.2
1.8
 
 
Left school before 14
95.8
4.2
0.421
97.4
2.6
0.063
Father’s social class at birth
Non-manual
96.7
3.4
 
98.0
2.0
 
 
Manual
95.6
4.4
0.032
97.4
2.6
0.163
Overcrowding
<1.5 people per room
96.1
3.9
 
97.7
2.3
 
 
≥1.5 people per room
94.2
5.8
0.004
97.1
3.0
0.251
Parity
Primiparous
96.2
3.8
 
97.5
2.5
 
 
2 children
96.5
3.5
0.585
97.6
2.4
0.809
 
3 or more
94.9
5.1
0.031
97.6
2.4
0.772
Mother smoked during pregnancy
No
96.1
3.9
 
97.7
2.3
 
 
Sometimes
94.5
5.5
0.095
97.9
2.1
0.794
 
Moderately
95.9
4.1
0.744
97.2
2.8
0.325
 
Heavily
95.4
4.7
0.296
96.9
3.1
0.139
Breastfed
Yes, for more than 1 month
96.3
3.7
 
97.7
2.3
 
 
Yes, for up to 1 month
95.7
4.3
0.262
97.7
2.3
0.896
 
No
95.5
4.5
0.133
97.3
2.7
0.344
Childhood illness
No
96.3
3.7
 
97.8
2.2
 
 
Yes
94.9
5.1
0.011
96.9
3.2
0.040
Education level at 23
Passed A levels
97.2
2.8
 
98.2
1.8
 
 
Passed O levels
96.2
3.8
0.139
97.6
2.4
0.255
 
No qualifications
95.0
5.0
0.004
97.2
2.8
0.058
Social class at 23
Non-manual active
96.8
3.2
 
97.7
2.3
 
 
Manual active
95.9
4.1
0.135
96.7
3.3
0.099
 
Inactive
94.4
5.6
0.002
97.7
2.3
0.993
Smoking status age 23
Never smoked
96.3
3.7
 
97.8
2.3
 
 
Former smoker
97.2
2.9
0.175
98.2
1.8
0.345
 
Smoker
94.7
5.3
0.023
97.0
3.0
0.151
Alcohol consumption age 23
Normal
96.0
4.1
 
97.6
2.4
 
 
Abstinence
95.6
4.4
0.820
97.6
2.4
0.940
 
Heavy
95.8
4.2
0.895
97.5
2.5
0.933
Malaise inventory age 23
None identified
96.0
4.0
 
97.8
2.2
 
 
Malaise
93.8
6.2
0.104
96.1
3.9
0.008
 
Mean (95 % CI)
Mean (95 % CI)
p
Mean (95 % CI)
Mean (95 % CI)
p
Birthweight (kg)
3.40 (3.39–3.41)
3.34 (3.28–3.40)
0.056
3.26 (3.25–3.27)
3.21 (3.13–3.30)
0.245
Gestational age (days)
280.52 (280.21–280.84)
280.27 (279.21–280.84)
0.743
281.12 (280.80–281.43)
280.02 (277.57–282.47)
0.295
Mother’s age at birth (years)
27.48 (27.35–27.61)
27.13 (26.47–27.79)
0.284
27.54 (27.40–27.67)
26.94 (26.16–27.72)
0.184
BMI age 23 (kg/m2)
23.36 (23.27–23.45)
23.45 (23.02–23.88)
0.681
22.16 (22.07–22.25)
22.04 (21.50–22.57)
0.666
Table 4
Multivariate Cox proportional hazard regression for men [Men n = 7,816 (of whom 323 dead)]
Variables
Categories
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Model 4
HR (95 % CI)
p
HR (95 % CI)
p
HR (95 % CI)
p
HR (95 % CI)
p
Mother’s education level
Left school at 15 or later
        
 
Left school before 14
0.99 (0.74–1.31)
0.919
0.93 (0.70–1.24)
0.605
0.91 (0.68–1.22)
0.529
0.87 (0.64–1.17)
0.345
Father’s social class at birth
Non-manual
        
 
Manual
1.29 (0.96–1.72)
0.088
1.20 (0.89–1.60)
0.235
1.18 (0.88–1.58)
0.280
1.12 (0.83–1.51)
0.465
Overcrowding
<1.5 people per room
        
 
≥1.5 people per room
1.45 (1.09–1.93)
0.010
1.30 (0.97–1.74)
0.078
1.23 (0.92–1.66)
0.168
1.18 (0.88–1.58)
0.281
Parity
Primiparous
        
 
2 children
  
1.03 (0.76–1.39)
0.867
1.01 (0.74–1.36)
0.974
0.97 (0.71–1.31)
0.837
 
3 or more
  
1.52 (1.10–2.10)
0.011
1.40 (1.01–1.95)
0.045
1.29 (0.92–1.82)
0.142
Gestational age (days)
   
1.00 (0.99–1.01)
0.590
1.00 (0.99–1.01)
0.622
1.0 (0.99–1.01)
0.615
Mother smoked during pregnancy
No
        
 
Sometimes
  
1.29 (0.85–1.96)
0.238
1.24 (0.81–1.89)
0.317
1.20 (0.78–1.83)
0.400
 
Moderately
  
0.96 (0.70–1.33)
0.827
0.95 (0.69–1.31)
0.740
0.93 (0.67–1.29)
0.655
 
Heavily
  
1.06 (0.76–1.48)
0.739
1.02 (0.73–1.43)
0.900
0.98 (0.69–1.38)
0.906
Mother’s age
   
0.97 (0.95–1.0)
0.036
0.98 (0.96–1.01)
0.074
0.98 (0.96–1.01)
0.182
Breastfed
Yes, for more than 1 month
        
 
Yes, for up to 1 month
  
1.12 (0.84–1.50)
0.429
1.12 (0.84–1.5)
0.436
1.10 (0.83–1.47)
0.500
 
No
  
1.13 (0.86–1.49)
0.392
1.12 (0.85–1.48)
0.423
1.11 (0.84–1.46)
0.479
Birthweight (kg)
   
0.82 (0.65–1.04)
0.108
0.84 (0.66–1.07)
0.162
0.85 (0.67–1.09)
0.204
Childhood illness
No
        
 
Yes
  
1.34 (1.04–1.71)
0.022
1.29 (1.01–1.66)
0.043
1.25 (0.97–1.6)
0.086
ACE
0
        
 
1
    
1.13 (0.86–1.47)
0.385
1.06 (0.81–1.39)
0.688
 
≥2
    
1.72 (1.25–2.36)
0.001
1.57 (1.13–2.18)
0.007
Education level at 23
Passed A levels
        
 
Passed O levels
      
1.25 (0.8–1.95)
0.319
 
No qualifications
      
1.32 (0.82–2.12)
0.254
Social class at 23
Non-manual active
        
 
Manual active
      
0.97 (0.66–1.41)
0.859
 
Inactive
      
1.29 (0.87–1.91)
0.205
Malaise inventory age 23
None identified
        
 
Malaise
      
1.15 (0.67–1.98)
0.618
Smoking status age 23
Never smoked
        
 
Former smoker
      
0.78 (0.52–1.16)
0.219
 
Smoker
      
1.25 (0.92–1.7)
0.158
Alcohol consumption age 23
Normal
        
 
Abstinence
      
0.98 (0.55–1.74)
0.935
 
Heavy
      
0.94 (0.67–1.32)
0.734
BMI age 23 (kg/m2)
       
1.01 (0.97–1.05)
0.706
% Imputed data in model
 
8
 
39
 
39
 
58
 
Table 5
Multivariate Cox proportional hazard regression for women [Female n = 7,405 (of whom 180 dead)]
Variables
Categories
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Model 4
HR (95 % CI)
p
HR (95 % CI)
p
HR (95 % CI)
p
HR (95 % CI)
p
Mother’s education level
Left school at 15 or later
        
 
Left school before 14
1.34 (0.89–2.01)
0.157
1.30 (0.86–1.96)
0.210
1.29 (0.86–1.94)
0.226
1.27 (0.83–1.93)
0.273
Father’s social class at birth
Non-manual
        
 
Manual
1.15 (0.78–1.7)
0.471
1.11 (0.75–1.65)
0.603
1.07 (0.72–1.59)
0.726
1.05 (0.70–1.57)
0.826
Overcrowding
<1.5 people per room
        
 
≥1.5 people per room
1.17 (0.78–1.76)
0.445
1.14 (0.75–1.74)
0.525
1.08 (0.71–1.64)
0.735
1.08 (0.71–1.64)
0.725
Parity
Primiparous
        
 
2 children
  
1.01 (0.69–1.46)
0.972
0.99 (0.68–1.44)
0.946
0.96 (0.66–1.4)
0.823
 
3 or more
  
0.98 (0.63–1.51)
0.922
0.89 (0.57–1.38)
0.602
0.85 (0.54–1.34)
0.491
Gestational age (days)
   
1.00 (0.98–1.01)
0.479
0.99 (0.98–1.01)
0.456
0.99 (0.98–1.01)
0.444
Mother smoked during pregnancy
No
        
 
Sometimes
  
0.81 (0.39–1.68)
0.576
0.76 (0.37–1.58)
0.466
0.76 (0.36–1.57)
0.452
 
Moderately
  
1.15 (0.77–1.73)
0.498
1.12 (0.75–1.68)
0.576
1.11 (0.74–1.67)
0.618
 
Heavily
  
1.31 (0.85–2.02)
0.219
1.26 (0.82–1.94)
0.298
1.24 (0.80–1.92)
0.329
Mother’s age
   
0.98 (0.95–1.01)
0.244
0.99 (0.96–1.02)
0.410
0.99 (0.96–1.02)
0.480
Breastfed
Yes, for more than 1 month
        
 
Yes, for up to 1 month
  
0.91 (0.62–1.35)
0.635
0.90 (0.61–1.33)
0.597
0.88 (0.60–1.31)
0.535
 
No
  
1.08 (0.76–1.54)
0.655
1.05 (0.74–1.49)
0.786
1.02 (0.72–1.46)
0.899
Birthweight (kg)
   
0.96 (0.68–1.35)
0.814
0.98 (0.7–1.38)
0.908
0.99 (0.70–1.40)
0.955
Childhood illness
No
        
 
Yes
  
1.41 (1.01–1.99)
0.052
1.36 (0.96–1.93)
0.082
1.35 (0.95–1.92)
0.092
ACE
0
        
 
1
    
1.70 (1.22–2.37)
0.002
1.66 (1.19–2.33)
0.003
 
≥2
    
1.85 (1.14–3.01)
0.013
1.80 (1.10–2.95)
0.020
Education level at 23
Passed A levels
        
 
Passed O levels
      
1.12 (0.67–1.86)
0.674
 
No qualifications
      
1.07 (0.61–1.89)
0.801
Social class at 23
Non-manual active
        
 
Manual active
      
1.17 (0.73–1.85)
0.516
 
Inactive
      
0.78 (0.52–1.19)
0.256
Malaise inventory age 23
None identified
        
 
Malaise
      
1.52 (0.99–2.33)
0.057
Smoking status age 23
Never smoked
        
 
Former smoker
      
0.80 (0.48–1.34)
0.397
 
Smoker
      
1.22 (0.81–1.84)
0.336
Alcohol consumption age 23
Normal
        
 
Abstinence
      
0.93 (0.52–1.65)
0.794
 
Heavy
      
0.97 (0.53–1.76)
0.909
BMI age 23 (kg/m2)
       
0.98 (0.93–1.04)
0.506
% imputed data in model
 
7
 
38
 
38
 
63
 
In terms of the early life social environment, father’s social class, overcrowding in the household and parity were all associated with mortality among men, whereas mother’s educational level was related to mortality for women, though not significant at the 5 % level. For both men and women the probability of premature death was higher for those who had had childhood pathologies. In men, education level at 23 years (no qualifications), social class at 23 years (being inactive), as well as smoking were related to increased premature mortality. In women, psychological malaise at 23 years was associated with increased premature mortality, and there was a borderline association with women’s education level (no qualifications).
Table 4 shows Cox regression models for men. Early life socioeconomic circumstances were associated with an increased premature mortality risk among men. Those who had lived in an overcrowded household had an increased premature all cause mortality risk of 45 % (HR 1.45, 95 % CI 1.1–1.9, p = 0.01) versus those from less overcrowded households. When pregnancy, perinatal and childhood variables were added (model 2), this association decreased to a 30 % risk, and was no longer statistically significant. Furthermore, men whose mothers had ≥3 children in 1958, had a 54 % (HR 1.54, 95 % CI 1.1–2.1, p = 0.01) increased risk of premature mortality versus those whose mother were primiparous. When ACE were added in model 3 the strength of the association between parity and early death decreased slightly (HR 1.40, 95 % CI 1.0–2.0, p = 0.05). Respondents who had experienced childhood pathologies had a 29 % increased risk of premature mortality (HR 1.29, 95 % CI 1.0–1.7, p = 0.04). Men who had experienced two or more adversities in childhood had a 72 % (HR 1.7, 95 % CI 1.3–2.4, p = 0.001) increased risk of all cause premature mortality versus those who had experienced no adversity in childhood. After controlling for adult mediating factors in model 4, the strength of this association diminished slightly to a 57 % increased risk (HR 1.57, 95 % CI 1.1–2.2, p = 0.01).
Table 5 shows Cox regression models for women. The results show that early life socioeconomic circumstances were not associated with premature mortality in women. In model 2 the risk of premature mortality was increased if the respondents had experienced childhood pathologies. In model 3 ACE was significantly related to premature mortality in a graded pattern. Women who had experienced one ACE had an increased premature mortality risk of 70 % (HR 1.70, 95 % CI 1.2–2.4, p = 0.002), and those who had accumulated two or more ACE had an increased premature mortality risk of 85 % (HR 1.85, 95 % CI 1.1–3.0, p = 0.013) versus those who had not experienced adversity. In the final model, the strength of these associations dropped slightly to 66 % (HR 1.66, 95 % CI 1.2–2.3, p = 0.003) and 80 % (HR 1.80, 95 % CI 1.1–3.0, p = 0.02) respectively, after controlling for adult mediating factors. Psychological malaise was also associated with a 52 % increase in the risk of premature mortality (HR 1.52, 95 % CI 1.0–2.3, p = 0.057).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this study is the first to highlight a relationship between a prospective measure of ACE and premature mortality in a longitudinal study. Indeed, a previous study used retrospective measure of childhood adversities collected from adults, therefore susceptible to recall bias [24]. Among women, childhood adversity had a graded relationship with early mortality risk. Women who had experienced one adversity had a 66 % increased risk, and those who had experienced two or more adversities had a 80 % increased risk of premature death versus women with no childhood adversities. Among men, those who had accumulated two or more adversities in childhood having a 57 % increased risk of early death.
These findings suggest that ACE should be examined as a potentially important initial exposure on a pathway towards adult ill health and early death. The association between ACE and premature mortality observed was only slightly attenuated after adjusting for socioeconomic and behavioural variables. These findings are in line with our hypothesis that exposure to toxic stress [2] in early life may leads to alterations to various biological systems and ultimately to poor health outcomes. It is not possible to pin-point which biological alterations may have occurred, however the association between ACE and premature mortality remained after taking mediating factors at the age of 23 into account, including social class, education level, psychological malaise, alcohol and tobacco consumption and BMI. Since childhood socioeconomic conditions are strong determinants of adult social position, this is may explain why the addition of adult socioeconomic variables to the model has little impact on the association observed. As the cohort ages, and their cause-specific mortality becomes driven to a larger extent by chronic diseases, lifestyle factors and behaviours is likely to explain more of observed differences in mortality.
This study has a number of limitations. Attrition is a problem in cohort studies, increasing the potential for selection bias in the remaining sample. In this study up to 30 % missing data was present among the adult variables collected at the age of 23. We dealt with this by imputing the missing data in the eligible sample, thereby adjusting the data for selection bias taking the missing at random (MAR) assumption. We consider this assumption to be reasonable because it allows missingness to depend on observed data, such as baseline characteristics and other measures occurring at different time points [25]. However, the assumption of MAR is unverifiable and we cannot rule-out that some data are ‘missing not at random’ (MNAR). Multiple imputation models, such as the one used on these data, include large numbers of covariates, helping to render the MAR assumption more plausible and to limit the impact of MNAR missingness [26]. Another limitation in this study relates to the choice of adult mediating variables at 23 years. Adult behavioural variables were collected subsequently at ages 33, 41, and 46. In order to simplify our analytic approach, we chose to use early adult variables as proxy indicators of behaviours typology, and mental health state, bearing in mind that the outcome of interest was mortality before <50 years. At present, cause-specific mortality linked with the cohort is unavailable. There is a risk of misclassification for the ACE variable. Individuals who had ‘no’ adversity for some items, and missing information for the others, were classified as having no adversities overall. Also, parents or teachers may have been reluctant, or ashamed to answer ‘yes’ to some adversities. In both cases this type of misclassification of individuals with ‘no’ adversities renders the variable conservative, meaning that findings showing a significantly increased risk for people identified with adversities are more likely to under-represent any real differences between those who experienced adversities versus those who did not.
The strengths of this study lie in its prospective design, allowing for some understanding of the causal sequencing of events. Furthermore, the ACE measure is a conservative one, meaning that observed relationships are likely to underestimate the true effect of childhood adversity. The use of multiply imputed data adjusts for biases and loss of power due to attrition in the cohort study.
We found differing associations between ACE and mortality for men and women, suggestive of gender-specific differences in possible mechanisms [27]. Our results show that, among men, socioeconomic variables such as overcrowding and parity are associated with the risk of premature death before adding ACE to the model. By adding ACE, any associations attributable to early life socioeconomic factors disappeared, suggesting that ACE may be on the explanatory pathway between early socioeconomic disadvantage and mortality, at least among men. Based on these results, adversity in childhood may explain some associations found elsewhere between childhood material and social disadvantage and early mortality. In a survival analysis of men and women combined, Kuh et al. [28] identified a strong cumulative impact (HR 4.9, 95 % CI 2.3–10.5) of manual social class in childhood combined with non-home ownership in adulthood on the risk of death between the ages of 26–54 in the 1946 birth cohort study. This result may also be cohort specific, highlighting differences with the older British birth cohort, who were subject to rationing and different social and health policies. Marked improvements have occurred in premature mortality rates, for both men and women since 1950 [9].
In women, our results showed that psychological malaise remained as a significant predictor of early mortality. This is in line with other studies showing associations between psychiatric symptoms [29] and psychological distress [30] and premature death. Given that the strength of the association between ACE and premature mortality change very little after including psychological malaise, it appears that separate etiological pathways may be operating. It is possible that malaise is associated with mortality via indirect behaviours not taken into account in the models such as, non compliance with prescribed treatments, or poorer access/use of health and social services. There may also be direct biological mechanisms such alterations of biological systems implicated in cardiovascular physiology leading to an increased risk of cardiovascular mortality among depressed individuals [31]. The mechanism linking malaise and early death may have a different etiological origin than that identified by ACE. Indeed, ACE may be associated with premature mortality via a socioeconomic/materialist pathway through occupational hazards/poor housing.
A possible interpretation of the results found here is that a mechanism akin to the concept of biological embedding [32] occurs during the sensitive periods of early brain development from the prenatal period into adolescence [33]. Exposure to prolonged activation of physiological stress responses due to events chronically unsupported by positive and secure relationships cause deleterious modifications to biological systems (neuroendocrine, inflammatory, immune) involving epigenetic modifications, that may or may not be reversible. These have a lasting impact on how well individuals adapt to subsequent exposures across their lifecourse, leading them along a trajectory towards increased morbidity and early mortality. Such physiological alterations are also potentially early causal precursors to damaging health behaviours which are adaptive in the short term; tobacco, alcohol and food being pharmacologically stress-reducing, but damaging in the medium and long term.
Though a number of studies have shown a dose–response association between an increasing number of accumulated adversities and a higher risk of morbidity [15, 34, 35], few have studied associations with mortality [16]. In all of these studies, the main methodological flaw is that ACE was self-reported retrospectively by adults who were asked about trauma experienced in childhood. Such questions are vulnerable to recall bias, where adults in poor health are more likely to report adversity during childhood [36].

Conclusion

The findings of this study reinforce the early childhood paradigm in terms of preventive medicine across the lifecourse [3739]. Our results, based on prospective data, point towards early life stressful events, particularly in a child’s intrafamilial environment, being risk factors for long term health across the lifecourse and premature mortality possibly via the mechanisms of biological embedding with may occur via social, neuro-cognitive, or behavioural pathways.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the Centre for Longitudinal Studies (CLS), Institute of Education for the use of the NCDS data and to the Economic and Social Data Service (ESDS) for making them available. However, neither CLS nor ESDS bear any responsibility for the analysis or interpretation of these data. MKI’s fellowship is funded by La Ligue nationale contre le cancer. This work was also funded by the Institut National du Cancer & the Institut de recherche en santé publique.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare having no conflict of interest.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 2.0 International License ( https://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Dent – Das Online-Abo der Zahnmedizin

Online-Abonnement

Mit e.Dent erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen zahnmedizinischen Fortbildungen und unseren zahnmedizinischen und ausgesuchten medizinischen Zeitschriften.

Anhänge

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Bremner JD, et al. The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood. A convergence of evidence from neurobiology and epidemiology. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;256(3):174–86.PubMedCrossRef Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Bremner JD, et al. The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood. A convergence of evidence from neurobiology and epidemiology. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;256(3):174–86.PubMedCrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Shonkoff JP, Garner AS, Siegel BS, et al. Early childhood adversity, toxic stress, and the role of the pediatrician: translating developmental science into lifelong health. Pediatrics. 2012;129(1):e224–31. doi:10.1542/peds.2011-2662.PubMedCrossRef Shonkoff JP, Garner AS, Siegel BS, et al. Early childhood adversity, toxic stress, and the role of the pediatrician: translating developmental science into lifelong health. Pediatrics. 2012;129(1):e224–31. doi:10.​1542/​peds.​2011-2662.PubMedCrossRef
5.
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Ben-Shlomo Y, Kuh D. A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology: conceptual models, empirical challenges and interdisciplinary perspectives. Int J Epidemiol. 2002;31:285–93.PubMedCrossRef Ben-Shlomo Y, Kuh D. A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology: conceptual models, empirical challenges and interdisciplinary perspectives. Int J Epidemiol. 2002;31:285–93.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Galobardes B, Lynch JW, Smith GD. Is the association between childhood socioeconomic circumstances and cause-specific mortality established? Update of a systematic review. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2008;62(5):387–90. doi:10.1136/jech.2007.065508.PubMedCrossRef Galobardes B, Lynch JW, Smith GD. Is the association between childhood socioeconomic circumstances and cause-specific mortality established? Update of a systematic review. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2008;62(5):387–90. doi:10.​1136/​jech.​2007.​065508.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Department of Health. Living well for longer: a call to action to reduce avoidable premature mortality. London: Williams Lea; 2013. Department of Health. Living well for longer: a call to action to reduce avoidable premature mortality. London: Williams Lea; 2013.
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Wheller L, Baker A, Griffiths C, Rooney C. Tends in avoidable mortality in England and Wales, 1993–2005. Health Stat Q. 2007;34:6–25.PubMed Wheller L, Baker A, Griffiths C, Rooney C. Tends in avoidable mortality in England and Wales, 1993–2005. Health Stat Q. 2007;34:6–25.PubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Bailey DBJ, Bruer JT, Symons FJ, Lichtman JW, editors. Critical thinling about critical periods. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes publishing co.; 2001. Bailey DBJ, Bruer JT, Symons FJ, Lichtman JW, editors. Critical thinling about critical periods. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes publishing co.; 2001.
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Kelly-Irving M, Mabile L, Grosclaude P, Lang T, Delpierre C. The embodiment of adverse childhood experiences and cancer development: potential biological mechanisms and pathways across the life course. Int J Public Health. 2013;58(1):3–11. doi:10.1007/s00038-012-0370-0.PubMedCrossRef Kelly-Irving M, Mabile L, Grosclaude P, Lang T, Delpierre C. The embodiment of adverse childhood experiences and cancer development: potential biological mechanisms and pathways across the life course. Int J Public Health. 2013;58(1):3–11. doi:10.​1007/​s00038-012-0370-0.PubMedCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Power C, Elliott J. Cohort profile: 1958 British Birth Cohort (National Child Development Study). Int J Epidemiol. 2006;35(1):34–41.PubMedCrossRef Power C, Elliott J. Cohort profile: 1958 British Birth Cohort (National Child Development Study). Int J Epidemiol. 2006;35(1):34–41.PubMedCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, et al. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The adverse childhood experiences (ACE) study. Am J Prev Med. 1998;14(4):245–58.PubMedCrossRef Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, et al. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The adverse childhood experiences (ACE) study. Am J Prev Med. 1998;14(4):245–58.PubMedCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat House of Commons Science and Technology Committee. Alcohol guidelines. London; 2012. House of Commons Science and Technology Committee. Alcohol guidelines. London; 2012.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Kudielka BM, Buske-Kirschbaum A, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. HPA axis responses to laboratory psychosocial stress in healthy elderly adults, younger adults, and children: impact of age and gender. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29(1):83–98. doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00146-4.PubMedCrossRef Kudielka BM, Buske-Kirschbaum A, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. HPA axis responses to laboratory psychosocial stress in healthy elderly adults, younger adults, and children: impact of age and gender. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29(1):83–98. doi:10.​1016/​S0306-4530(02)00146-4.PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Little R, Rubin DB. Statistical analysis with missing data. New York: Wiley; 1987. Little R, Rubin DB. Statistical analysis with missing data. New York: Wiley; 1987.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Coley N, Gardette V, Cantet C, Gillette-Guyonnet, S, Nourhashemi F, Vellas B, Andrieu S. How should we deal with missing data in clinical trials involving Alzheimer’s disease patients? Curr Alzheimer Res. 2011;8(4):421–33. doi:10.2174/156720511795745339. Coley N, Gardette V, Cantet C, Gillette-Guyonnet, S, Nourhashemi F, Vellas B, Andrieu S. How should we deal with missing data in clinical trials involving Alzheimer’s disease patients? Curr Alzheimer Res. 2011;8(4):421–33. doi:10.​2174/​1567205117957453​39.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuh D, Hardy R, Langenberg C, Richards M, Wadsworth M. Mortality in adults aged 26–54 years related to socioeconomic conditions in childhood and adulthood: post was birth cohort study. Br Med J. 2002;325:1076–80.CrossRef Kuh D, Hardy R, Langenberg C, Richards M, Wadsworth M. Mortality in adults aged 26–54 years related to socioeconomic conditions in childhood and adulthood: post was birth cohort study. Br Med J. 2002;325:1076–80.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Henderson M, Hotopf M, Shah I, Hayes RD, Kuh D. Psychiatric disorder in early adulthood and risk of premature mortality in the 1946 British birth cohort. BMC Psychiatr. 2011;11:37. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-11-37.CrossRef Henderson M, Hotopf M, Shah I, Hayes RD, Kuh D. Psychiatric disorder in early adulthood and risk of premature mortality in the 1946 British birth cohort. BMC Psychiatr. 2011;11:37. doi:10.​1186/​1471-244X-11-37.CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Hertzman C. The biological embedding of early experience and its effects on health in adulthood. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;896:85–95.PubMedCrossRef Hertzman C. The biological embedding of early experience and its effects on health in adulthood. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;896:85–95.PubMedCrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(6):434–45. doi:10.1038/nrn2639.PubMedCrossRef Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(6):434–45. doi:10.​1038/​nrn2639.PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Danese A, Moffitt TE, Harrington H, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and adult risk factors for age-related disease depression, inflammation, and clustering of metabolic risk markers. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(12):1135–43.PubMedCrossRef Danese A, Moffitt TE, Harrington H, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and adult risk factors for age-related disease depression, inflammation, and clustering of metabolic risk markers. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(12):1135–43.PubMedCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Dube SR, Felitti VJ, Dong M, Giles WH, Anda RF. The impact of adverse childhood experiences on health problems: evidence from four birth cohorts dating back to 1900. Prev Med. 2003;37(3):268–77.PubMedCrossRef Dube SR, Felitti VJ, Dong M, Giles WH, Anda RF. The impact of adverse childhood experiences on health problems: evidence from four birth cohorts dating back to 1900. Prev Med. 2003;37(3):268–77.PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Hardt J, Rutter M. Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences: review of the evidence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45(2):260–73.PubMedCrossRef Hardt J, Rutter M. Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences: review of the evidence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45(2):260–73.PubMedCrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Miller GE, Chen E, Parker KJ. Psychological stress in childhood and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging: moving toward a model of behavioral and biological mechanisms. Psychol Bull. 2011;137(6):959–97. doi:10.1037/a0024768.PubMedCrossRef Miller GE, Chen E, Parker KJ. Psychological stress in childhood and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging: moving toward a model of behavioral and biological mechanisms. Psychol Bull. 2011;137(6):959–97. doi:10.​1037/​a0024768.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Adverse childhood experiences and premature all-cause mortality
verfasst von
Michelle Kelly-Irving
Benoit Lepage
Dominique Dedieu
Mel Bartley
David Blane
Pascale Grosclaude
Thierry Lang
Cyrille Delpierre
Publikationsdatum
01.09.2013
Verlag
Springer Netherlands
Erschienen in
European Journal of Epidemiology / Ausgabe 9/2013
Print ISSN: 0393-2990
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-7284
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10654-013-9832-9

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 9/2013

European Journal of Epidemiology 9/2013 Zur Ausgabe