Skip to main content
Erschienen in: The Cerebellum 1/2012

Open Access 01.03.2012

fMRI Activities in the Emotional Cerebellum: A Preference for Negative Stimuli and Goal-Directed Behavior

verfasst von: Caroline K. L. Schraa-Tam, Willem J. R. Rietdijk, Willem J. M. I. Verbeke, Roeland C. Dietvorst, Wouter E. van den Berg, Richard P. Bagozzi, Chris I. De Zeeuw

Erschienen in: The Cerebellum | Ausgabe 1/2012

download
DOWNLOAD
print
DRUCKEN
insite
SUCHEN

Abstract

Several studies indicate that the cerebellum might play a role in experiencing and/or controlling emphatic emotions, but it remains to be determined whether there is a distinction between positive and negative emotions, and, if so, which specific parts of the cerebellum are involved in these types of emotions. Here, we visualized activations of the cerebellum and extracerebellar regions using high-field fMRI, while we asked participants to observe and imitate images with pictures of human faces expressing different emotional states or with moving geometric shapes as control. The state of the emotions could be positive (happiness and surprise), negative (anger and disgust), or neutral. The positive emotional faces only evoked mild activations of crus 2 in the cerebellum, whereas the negative emotional faces evoked prominent activations in lobules VI and VIIa in its hemispheres and lobules VIII and IX in the vermis. The cerebellar activations associated with negative emotions occurred concomitantly with activations of mirror neuron domains such as the insula and amygdala. These data suggest that the potential role of the cerebellum in control of emotions may be particularly relevant for goal-directed behavior that is required for observing and reacting to another person’s (negative) expressions.

Introduction

Emotions can be subdivided into categories according to the effects they evoke [1, 2]. Positive emotions such as happiness or surprise may have broadening effects in that they may encourage novel, varied, and explanatory thoughts and actions [3, 4], while negative emotions such as anger or disgust might be more primitive and evoke fear-related responses such as defensive behavior or autonomic arousal [5]. When confronted with confederates’ expressions of emotions, people empathize with these people’s feelings. One acts as if one experiences the same feelings as his ally through an appreciation of similarity [68]. Possible explanations are either the perception–action model of empathy or the perceptual symbol systems framework [9, 10]. The first model implies that perception of an individual’s state automatically activates the observer’s representation of that state [9], while the second implies that feelings are represented in simulators—distributed networks of conjunctive neurons that activate in response to confrontation with another person’s emotion, enabling the observer to re-enact the emotion of the observed person in the form of simulations [10]. Concretely experiencing one’s own emotions or empathizing with another’s emotions involves an activation of a mirror neuron network (insular cortex, medial areas of prefrontal cortex and amygdala) [1114]. The representations of an emotion (whether the emotion is one’s own or another’s) also automatically prime or generate associated autonomic and somatic responses unless inhibited [9]. Controlling such responses might require involvement of the cerebellum; for example, the cerebellum might help to convert the initial emotional responses into goal-oriented behaviors or contextually appropriate social behaviors [15]. Evidence obtained with positron emission tomography (PET) suggests that the cerebellum indeed plays a role in controlling emotions, but it is still unclear whether and to what extent the cerebellum is involved in both positive and negative emotions, during different experimental (observation and imitation) tasks [16, 17]. Evidence obtained with low-resolution and low-field functional imaging techniques (PET, 1.5T MRI scanner) also supports the possibility that the cerebellum contributes to the control of emotions, but in these studies, precise localization at the cerebellar sub-regional level remains to be established [16, 1820] (Table 1).
Table 1
Functional imaging studies showing cerebellar activities associated with negative emotions
Author and title
Modality
Paradigm
Potential problems/explanation
Lane RD et al. 1997, [16]
PET
Clips showing dynamic facial expressions (happy and sad) vs silent nature clip
Cerebellum activities were found (and only mentioned) in the right lateral cerebellum and in the vermis area with 12 subjects thresholded at p < 0.0005 uncorrected for multiple comparison
Neuroanatomical correlates of happiness, sadness, and disgust
Liotti et al. 2001 [19]
PET
Subjects were asked to generate states of sadness, anxiety, or a neutral, relaxed state comparable to states experienced earlier
Cerebellum activities were found in the right vermis area with 8 subjects with p < 0.001 uncorrected for multiple comparison
Differential limbic-cortical correlates of sadness and anxiety in healthy subjects: Implications for affective disorders
Subjects need to recall memory from its experience, and the paradigm itself also involves cognitive regulation and not perceiving emotion. Therefore the paradigm triggers top-down representations of emotions experienced earlier in life (also involving memory and episodic information), which is different from detecting emotion in a bottom–up fashion by showing emotion-laden dynamic facial expressions
Garrett et al. 2006 [18]
fMRI (1.5T)
IAPS (international affective picture system, static stimuli depicting negative evoking scenery)
Cerebellum activities were found in the right crus 1 area with nine subjects with p < 0.003 uncorrected for multiple comparison
Separating subjective emotion from the perception of emotion-inducing stimuli: an fMRI study
-Pressing button to rate emotion during the experiment.
The paradigm does involve pressing a button; therefore, activations could have been the results of the task, although the contrasts are designed to eliminate activations associated with pressing the button
Van der Gaag et al. 2007 [20]
fMRI (3T)
Dynamic facial expressions vs neutral faces with moving facial muscles but without emotion (e.g. blowing up the cheeks)
Cerebellum activities were found in the right cerebellum area crus 1 when the disgust faces were compared to neutral faces and in the left cerebellum area crus 1 when the fear faces were compared to the neutral faces. Although the cerebellum activities should not be caused by the perception of motion, since the neutral condition also involved motion (though without emotional valence), only one area of cerebellum activation was found in this study with a p value of p < 0.005 uncorrected and using only 10 subjects for this specific analysis. Furthermore, van der Gaag did not really give an explanation why the cerebellum is involved.
Facial expressions: What the mirror neuron system can and cannot tell us
Bastiaanse et al. 2011 [24]
Age-Related Increase in Inferior Frontal Gyrus Activity and Social Functioning in Autism Spectrum Disorder
Our stimuli are similar to the studies of van der Gaag and Batiaanse et al. But our study made use of a blocked design with negative and positive emotions, while the van der Gaag study implemented an event-related design. Our design is likely to be more sensitive in isolating activations associated with negative emotions than the van der Gaag study, because we made use of blocked versus event-related design and because we combined disgust and anger together, as we did with happiness and surprise. Our results have multiple cerebellum areas of activation including the cerebellum area crus 1, 2, lobula VI, VIII, and IX and vermis VIII and IX. All areas are thresholded at p < 0.05 with FDR correction for multiple comparisons and with 20 subjects. Our stimuli consist of dynamic facial expressions vs moving geometric shapes; hence, after subtraction, the activity associated with perception of motion should be excluded and what is left should be from the results of the emotional content of the stimuli.
Here, we tackled these questions by presenting short video clips of emotion-laden faces to human subjects, while examining the activities of their cerebellum and cortical brain regions using high-field functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) (a 3T MRI scanner). Such stimuli are known to evoke activities in several different neural networks involved in emotion, such as the mirror neuron system and the limbic system [1114]. Since the mirror neuron system can be considered one of the main systems for controlling goal-directed behavior [15], it provides further impetus to question whether the cerebellum shows preferences for responding to positive or negative emotions, whether these responses occur concomitantly with those in the mirror neuron system, and, if so, which parts of the cerebellum are involved during these concomitant activations.

Materials and Methods

Subjects
Written informed consent was obtained from each participant prior to the study, which was approved by the Institutional Review Board. Twenty healthy volunteers (11 men, 9 women) participated in the mirror neurons (MN) experiment. Since there is convincing evidence that the mirror mechanism is involved in imitation as an immediate replica from the observed motor act [21, 22], a control experiment—the imitation task (IT)—was also performed.
Stimuli, Experimental Design, and Procedures
The MN experimental stimuli consisted of full-face, full-color, 3-s video clips of five males and five females displaying various emotional states (anger, disgust, happiness, surprise, and neutrality) [20]. The control stimuli were clips of moving geometric shapes. Thus the four experimental conditions included specific clips of a single category: (1) positive emotional faces: happy and surprised, (2) negative emotional faces: angry and disgust, (3) neutral faces, and (4) moving geometric shapes. Each condition was presented 12 times in pseudo-randomized (counterbalanced between subjects) blocks lasting 12 s and comprising three clips. Clips were separated by a 1-s inter-stimulus interval (ISI), and conditions were separated by a 2 s ISI. The design allowed us to investigate three impacts: (a) that of positive faces versus moving geometric shapes; (b) that of negative faces versus moving geometric shapes; and (c) that of neutral faces versus moving geometric shapes. This design is similar to that employed frequently [23, 24]. A scheme is presented in the Appendix section to make the procedure more clear.
The IT experimental stimuli consisted of a mixture of happy, surprised, angry, and disgust emotional faces. The two main conditions that were tested are “observation” and “observation-execution” (i.e., imitation). In the observation condition, subjects were shown with a red cross for 1 s and then followed by one of the four emotional faces for 3 s and this pattern repeated six times with every time randomly with another emotional faces. Subjects were instructed to observe only. In the imitation condition, subjects were shown with a green cross instead and followed by again one of the emotional faces for 3 s and this pattern repeated six times with every time randomly with another emotional faces. However, subjects were instructed to imitate the observed emotional faces. All emotional faces were having the same amount of occurrence. The observation task and the imitation task were alternated five times each. A scheme is presented in the Appendix section to make the procedure more clear.
The imitation task experiment is considered as the control experiment and was performed in pseudo-randomly during the same scanning session with the mirror neuron experiment. Thus half of the study subject group participated first with the IT experiment and half of the study subject group participated first with the MN experiment. The control experiment (IT) is needed to double check that the MN system from our tested subjects do “exist”, and they allow us to show that the activation areas in both experimental tasks are in line with the current literature [25].
fMRI Acquisition
All imaging was performed on a 3T MRI scanner (General Electric, Milwaukee, USA) using a dedicated eight-channel head coil. For the anatomical image, a 3D high-resolution inversion recovery fast spoiled gradient recalled echo sequence (echo time (TE)/repetition time (TR)/inversion time = 2.1/10.4/300 ms, flip angle = 18°, matrix = 416 × 256, field of view (FOV) = 25 cm, slice thickness 1.6 mm with 50% overlap) was required. For functional imaging, a single-shot gradient-echo echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence in transverse orientation was used, allowing sufficient blood oxygenation level dependent contrast. The imaging volume covered the entire brain (TR/TE 3,000/30 ms; 64 × 96 matrix with a rectangular field-of-view of 22 cm, 2.5-mm slice thickness, 39 contiguous slices; voxel size of 3.5 × 3.0 × 2.5 mm3). Acquisition time was 10:45 min with a time series of 210 imaging volumes (including 15 s of dummy scans that were discarded) for the MN experiment. Acquisition time was 4:00 min with a time series of 80 imaging volumes (including 15 s of dummy scans that were discarded) for the IT experiment. The experiment was performed in near darkness with all lights turned off except the video projector light. Visual stimuli were shown by means of back projection with a video projector onto a translucent screen in front of the scanner. Participants viewed this screen with a mirror system on top of the head coil. The total field-of-view extended 21° horizontally and 17° vertically. Stimuli were presented by the stimulation software package, Presentation (Neurobehavioral Systems).
Functional Image Analysis
The functional imaging data were analyzed using statistical parametric mapping software (SPM 5, distributed by the Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, University College London, UK) implemented in MATLAB (Version 6.5, Mathworks, Sherborn, MA, USA). Motion correction and co-registration were done according to the methodology provided by SPM5. Brain volumes were normalized to the standard space defined by the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template. The normalized data had a resolution of 2 × 2 × 2 mm and were spatially smoothed with a 3D isotropic Gaussian kernel, with a full width half maximum of 8 mm. Statistical parametric maps were calculated for each subject. Movement parameters resulting from the realignment pre-processing were included as regressors of no interest in order to further reduce motion artifacts. The model was estimated with a high-pass filter with a cut-off period of 128 s. For each participant, contrast maps were calculated between each condition. The individual contrast maps were used for second-level random effects (group) analysis in the regression analyses. One sample t tests were performed for each comparison (e.g., negative faces versus moving geometric shapes for the MN experiment, and imitation versus observation for the IT experiment). All tests were thresholded at p < 0.05 with false discovery rate (FDR) correction for multiple comparisons and with a minimum cluster size of 5 voxels. Anatomic labeling of the observed areas of activation in SPM was done using the macroscopic anatomic parcellation procedure of the MNI MRI single-subject brain [26].

Results

The quantitative results of the random effects group analyses comparing the impacts of the emotionally laden (positive and negative) faces with that of the moving geometric shapes are shown in Tables 2 and 3, respectively.
Table 2
Positive emotional faces vs moving geometric shapes: areas of activation in the mirror neurons experiment with cluster size, T values of the local maximum, Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates, the anatomical areas within a cluster, the percentage of the cluster size, and the functional area
Positive emotional faces vs moving geometric objects
  
Cluster size
T value
MNI coordinate (mm)
Anatomic area
Side
Functional area
  
x
y
z
   
6
4.8
−16
−82
−42
Cerebellum crus 2
L
Cerebellum
23
6.09
−56
−8
−8
Superior temporal gyrus
L
MN system
65
6.57
52
−8
−8
Superior temporal gyrus
R
MN system
8
4.39
−42
24
−2
Inferior orbital frontal gyrus
L
MN system
12
4.39
−50
28
8
Inferior triangular frontal gyrus
L
MN system
14
5.43
32
10
56
Middle frontal gyrus
R
MN system
265
6.09
−58
−8
−20
Middle temporal gyrus
L
 
338
6.57
54
−10
−20
Middle temporal gyrus
R
 
7
5.52
−34
8
−32
Superior temporal pole
L
 
13
5.52
−40
6
−16
Middle temporal pole
L
 
7
4.39
46
12
−36
Middle temporal pole
R
 
51
9.89
−6
−80
8
Calcarine
L
 
42
7.65
8
−80
8
Calcarine
R
 
11
9.89
−8
−84
−6
Lingual
L
 
67
12.87
−46
−70
4
Inferior occipital gyrus
L
 
25
7.65
38
−72
4
Inferior occipital gyrus
R
 
50
9.89
−39
−100
42
Middle occipital gyrus
L
 
All areas were thresholded at p < 0.05 with FDR correction for multiple comparisons and with a minimum cluster size of 5 voxels
L left hemisphere, R right hemisphere
Table 3
Negative emotional faces vs moving geometric shapes: areas of activation in the mirror neurons experiment with cluster size, T values of the local maximum, Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates, the anatomical areas within a cluster, the percentage of the cluster size and the functional area
Negative emotional faces vs moving geometric shapes
  
Cluster size
T value
MNI coordinate (mm)
Anatomic area
Side
Functional area
  
x
y
z
   
135
5.49
−42
−66
−34
Cerebellum crus 1
L
Cerebellum
147
6.71
42
−66
−32
Cerebellum crus 1
R
Cerebellum
257
12.87
−16
−82
−42
Cerebellum crus 2
L
Cerebellum
84
4.13
18
−82
−40
Cerebellum crus 2
R
Cerebellum
15
5.49
−14
−70
−22
Cerebellum VI
L
Cerebellum
53
6.71
18
−68
−24
Cerebellum VI
R
Cerebellum
14
5.02
−24
−60
−56
Cerebellum VIII
L
Cerebellum
92
5.02
−6
−54
−50
Cerebellum IX
L
Cerebellum
31
5.02
8
−56
−48
Cerebellum IX
R
Cerebellum
80
5.02
2
−58
−38
Vermis IX
 
Cerebellum
11
5.02
2
−64
−42
Vermis VIII
 
Cerebellum
60
5.6
−24
−2
−22
Amygdala
L
MN system
76
6.24
26
0
−22
Amygdala
R
MN system
280
6.64
−40
−22
−2
Insula
L
MN system
85
6.24
42
−20
−6
Insula
R
MN system
41
4.11
−4
−24
62
Precentral gyrus
L
MN system
265
6.24
4
−24
62
Precentral gyrus
R
MN system
548
5.24
−12
−14
66
Supplementary motor area
L
MN system
402
5.24
14
−16
70
Supplementary motor area
R
MN system
104
7.79
−56
−8
−8
Superior temporal gyrus
L
MN system
393
7.91
52
−8
−8
Superior temporal gyrus
R
MN system
971
6.64
−50
28
8
Inferior triangular frontal gyrus
L
MN system
571
6.24
−8
−24
64
Inferior triangular frontal gyrus
R
MN system
31
4.11
−44
8
56
Middle frontal gyrus
L
MN system
250
6.24
32
10
56
Middle frontal gyrus
R
MN system
27
7.79
−56
−56
38
Angular
L
 
9
3.85
48
−52
38
Angular
R
 
12
7.91
28
−14
−20
Hippocampus
R
 
67
6.24
−26
−16
−20
Hippocampus
L
 
23
4.38
18
−2
−4
Pallidum
R
 
20
5.6
−26
−36
−16
Parahippocampal
L
 
65
6.24
22
−36
−14
Parahippocampal
R
 
72
3.79
−22
8
−4
Putamen
L
 
185
4.38
22
10
−4
Putamen
R
 
15
7.91
62
−46
30
Supramarginal
R
 
1,346
7.79
−58
−8
−20
Middle temporal gyrus
L
 
885
7.91
54
−10
−20
Middle temporal gyrus
R
 
12
5.49
−44
−4
−28
Inferior temporal gyrus
L
 
23
6.71
42
4
−38
Inferior temporal gyrus
R
 
128
6.64
−34
8
−32
Superior temporal pole
L
 
7
6.24
36
−4
−32
Superior temporal pole
R
 
19
6.64
−40
6
−16
Middle temporal pole
L
 
53
4.35
46
12
−36
Middle temporal pole
R
 
124
12.87
−6
−80
8
Calcarine
L
 
110
6.45
8
−80
8
Calcarine
R
 
41
5.49
−18
−80
−12
Fusiform
L
 
106
6.71
24
−80
−12
Fusiform
R
 
82
12.87
−8
−84
−6
Lingual
L
 
25
6.45
8
−82
−6
Lingual
R
 
11
12.87
−30
−70
42
Superior occipital gyrus
L
 
14
6.45
30
−70
38
Superior occipital gyrus
R
 
191
12.87
−38
−100
−23
Middle occipital gyrus
L
 
10
6.45
20
−104
0
Middle occipital gyrus
R
 
146
12.87
−46
−70
4
Inferior occipital gyrus
L
 
98
6.45
38
−72
4
Inferior occipital gyrus
R
 
All areas were thresholded at p < 0.05 with FDR correction for multiple comparisons and with a minimum cluster size of 5 voxels
L left hemisphere, R right hemisphere
1.
Positive faces versus moving geometric shapes: When positive faces were contrasted with moving geometric shapes, the analysis revealed only one cerebellar area in which the activity was significantly increased, i.e., the left crus 2 area. The activation size of that region was restricted to 6 voxels only, which is not significantly different (one sample t test with p = 0.5 corrected for multiple comparison with false discovery rate, FDR) from that observed in the same region following the presentation of neutral stimuli (see below). In contrast, the cerebral cortex showed a much more widespread distribution of prominently elevated activations (Table 2): These included bilateral activations in the superior and middle temporal gyri as well as in the middle temporal pole and the inferior occipital gyri; a unilateral activation in the left superior and middle temporal pole as well as in the left middle occipital gyrus; and a unilateral activation in the right precentral gyrus. These data suggest that involvement of the cerebellum in processing positive emotionally laden facial stimuli is very limited.
 
2.
Negative faces versus moving geometric shapes: The analysis of the results obtained with the negative faces revealed a much more prominent role for the cerebellum (Fig. 1 and Table 3). Following these stimuli, we found significantly elevated, bilateral activations in lobules VI and IX as well as crus 1 and crus 2 in the hemispheres, and in lobules VIII and IX in the vermis. In hemispheric lobule VIII, we only observed unilateral activation on the left side. With regard to concomitant activations in the cerebral cortex, we observed that many of the areas that were involved in processing positive stimuli were also involved in processing negative stimuli (Tables 2 and 3); these included bilateral activations of the precentral gyri; the inferior, middle, and superior temporal gyri; the middle and superior temporal poles; the inferior, middle, and superior occipital gyri; and the fusiform gyri. In addition, the negative stimuli induced cerebral activations of areas that were not activated by the positive stimuli. Most prominently, these included bilateral activations of the amygdala, insula, and inferior and middle frontal gyri. Since these three latter regions are all involved in the so-called mirror neuron system [27], it is interesting to note that areas adjacent to this putative mirror neuron system in the temporal and parietal gyri were also activated (for more details see Table 3). Thus, the cerebral cortex in general responded with prominent activations to both positive and negative stimuli, whereas both the cerebellum and the presumptive mirror neuron system showed a preference for negative stimuli.
 
3.
Neutral faces versus moving geometric shapes: Contrasting neutral faces with moving geometric shapes revealed limited cerebellar activity. Like the activity following stimulation with positively emotionally laden faces, only the left crus 2 area was activated; and the size of this activation was also very minimal, covering 4 voxels only. The cerebral cortex and the subcortical regions including the mirror neuron system were also hardly activated by neutral stimuli; we observed only bilateral activation of the occipital gyri (Table 4).
Table 4
Neutral faces vs moving geometric shapes: areas of activation in the mirror neurons experiment with cluster size, T values of the local maximum, Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates, the anatomical areas within a cluster, the percentage of the cluster size and the functional area
Neutral faces vs moving geometric shapes
  
Cluster size
T value
MNI coordinate (mm)
Anatomic area
Side
Functional area
  
x
y
z
   
4
5.81
−16
−82
−42
Cerebellum crus 2
L
Cerebellum
15
8.34
−6
−80
8
Calcarine
L
 
23
6.64
8
−80
8
Calcarine
R
 
29
8.34
−46
−70
4
Inferior occipital gyrus
L
 
6
6.64
38
−72
4
Inferior occipital gyrus
R
 
All areas were thresholded at p < 0.05 with FDR correction for multiple comparisons and with a minimum cluster size of 5 voxels
L left hemisphere, R right hemisphere
 
The restricted involvement of the cerebellum during the viewing of positive and neutral faces and the relatively elaborate activations of the cerebellum in response to negative emotions suggest that the cerebellum plays a salient role during the processing of negative emotions.
The quantitative results of the random effects group analyses comparing the impacts of the emotionally laden (positive, negative, and neutral) faces with that of the natural faces are supplied in the appendix section for reference only (Appendix Tables 5 and 6).
1.
Imitation versus Observation: The quantitative results of the random effects group analyses comparing the impacts of the imitation task with the observation task from the imitation experiment (the control experiment) are supplied in the appendix section (Appendix Table 7). The results of the control experiment show that similar activations are observed under the observation task (described under 1, 2, 3 above), however one can observe more significant, bilateral activation when imitating the emotional (either positive or negative) stimuli. Furthermore, bilateral activations in the crus 1, cerebellum IV-I and VI are shown comparing imitation and observation.
 

Discussion

Our main findings are that the cerebellum may be more prominently involved in control of negative emotions than positive emotions, and that this activity may be related to that of the mirror neuron system (Tables 2 and 3). As will be discussed below, these data are in line with the possibility that the cerebellum is involved in predictive and pro-active control of cognitive functions.
The cerebellum of subjects showed prominent activations in lobules VI and VII in the hemispheres and in lobules VIII and IX in the vermis following exposure to pictures with negative emotions. Several regions of the mirror neuron system including the amygdala, insula, and frontal cortex were concomitantly activated. These regions have been associated with a negative bodily state or arousal that may trigger actions aimed at overcoming this negative state [20]. As negative emotional facial expressions signal threat, activation of the cerebellum may be related to activation of motor plans associated with action preparedness such as during fight–flight behavior, [28] and hence promote processing of the confederate emotion.
As emotions activate a representation [9] or a simulation network [10], negative emotions in particular activate an adaptive network allowing people to develop quick and flexible responses involving an efficient synchronization of a large network of brain regions [17]. Negative emotions are known to be processed more quickly than positive emotions [29]. At an abstract level the observations in this study show that people more vigorously activate a large network of brain regions to a confederate who is in need than to one who experiences welfare [30]. As mirror neurons are involved in managing goal-directed behavior, [15] which may require cerebellum involvement, we can further speculate that negative emotions trigger behaviors which urge people to evoke compassionate behaviors such as altruism [30].
The control experiment performed in this study showed that imitating the emotional stimuli, more areas were significantly activated compared to the observation experiment. However, similar areas in the cerebellum and other cortical areas are significantly activated under both experimental tasks (observation and imitation). These result support current literature concerning activations of the MN system [24]. Among others, Fogassi et al. [31] showed that it is not only the imitation of actions that triggers the MN system, but also the observation that leads to activation in these areas (for more recent study see [25 and or 24]).
Our data are in line with those of Schmahmann and Sherman [2, 32, 33], who identified the Cerebellar Cognitive Affective syndrome in patients with lesions in cerebellar lobules VI and VII crus 1 and crus 2. They observed that patients with either congenital or acquired damage in these regions suffer from a wide spectrum of emotional and behavioral deficits [32, 33]. These included for example flattening of affect or disinhibition characterized by over-familiarity or impulsive actions. Such impairments may well affect the ability to communicate and empathize with perspectives of other persons [7]. Interestingly, Schmahmann and colleagues also found specific correlations in patients with damage in lobule IX [32, 34], one of the other regions that was prominently activated following exposure to pictures with negative emotions in our study. These patients often have problems to form bonds similar to patients with obsessive-compulsive disorders and panic disorders [34].
Several other studies also found similar results. Leslie and colleagues have reported significant cerebellar activation during both positive and negative subjective experiences with fMRI [35]. However, their studies combined both positive and negative emotions together for analysis and only found activation bilaterally in crus 1. Damasio performed a similar whole brain study using PET and reported activation bilaterally at the mesial cerebellum for negative emotions and unilaterally left at the mesial cerebellum for positive emotions [36]. In our study, we were probably able to report activation in the cerebellum at higher spatial resolution than those by Leslie [35] and Damasio [36], because we employed high-field (3T) MRI. Moreover, different from their studies, in our study we used dynamically moving objects instead of the neutral faces as the baseline condition. We did so to circumvent the potential bias induced by the motion that can be induced during exposure to faces with emotions (positive and negative).
Taken together, the current data show that the posterior cerebellum and the mirror neuron system are conceitedly activated when negative emotions laden faces are presented, which raises the possibility that this part of the cerebellum is particularly relevant for empathic responses towards confederates.

Conflict of Interest

There is no conflict of interest.

Open Access

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.
Open AccessThis is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License (https://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by-nc/​2.​0), which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Neurologie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes, den Premium-Inhalten der neurologischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Neurologie-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Anhänge

Appendix

Table 5
Positive emotional faces vs neutral faces: areas of activation in the mirror neurons experiment with cluster size, T values of the local maximum, Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates, the anatomical areas within a cluster, the percentage of the cluster size and the functional area
Positive emotional faces vs neutral faces
  
Cluster size
T value
MNI coordinate (mm)
Anatomic area
Side
Functional area
  
x
y
z
   
47
6.6
−42
−66
−34
Cerebellum crus 1
L
Cerebellum
46
7.84
42
−72
−32
Cerebellum crus 1
R
Cerebellum
89
6.6
−14
−70
−22
Cerebellum VI
L
Cerebellum
145
7.84
18
−68
−24
Cerebellum VI
R
Cerebellum
9
3.77
−24
−2
−22
Amygdala
L
MN system
55
4.17
26
0
−22
Amygdala
R
MN system
39
4.28
−40
−22
−2
Insula
L
MN system
158
5.67
4
−24
62
Precentral gyrus
R
MN system
9
5.67
−14
−42
66
Postcentral gyrus
R
MN system
31
3.88
14
−16
70
Supplementary motor area
R
MN system
160
6.6
−56
−8
−8
Superior temporal gyrus
L
MN system
613
7.84
52
−8
−8
Superior temporal gyrus
R
MN system
48
5.67
32
10
56
Middle frontal gyrus
R
MN system
18
4.72
−26
−16
−20
Hippocampus
L
 
88
4.17
28
−14
−20
Hippocampus
R
 
45
4.72
−26
−36
−16
Parahippocampal gyrus
L
 
19
4.17
22
−36
−14
Parahippocampal gyrus
R
 
25
6.6
−54
−48
30
Supramarginal
L
 
1,077
6.6
−58
−8
−20
Middle temporal gyrus
L
 
54
4.92
54
−10
−20
Middle temporal gyrus
R
 
46
6.6
−44
−4
−28
Inferior temporal gyrus
L
 
444
7.84
42
4
−38
Inferior temporal gyrus
R
 
23
3.77
−34
8
−32
Superior temporal pole
L
 
7
4.17
36
−4
−32
Superior temporal pole
R
 
28
4.28
−40
6
−16
Middle temporal pole
L
 
125
4.92
46
12
−36
Middle temporal pole
R
 
35
7.84
8
−80
8
Calcarine
R
 
13
7.84
0
−80
26
Cuneus
R
 
304
6.6
−18
−80
−12
Fusiform
L
 
377
7.84
24
−80
−12
Fusiform
R
 
13
7.84
8
−82
−6
Lingual
R
 
65
7.84
30
−70
38
Superior Occipital gyrus
R
 
743
6.6
−38
−100
−23
Middle occipital gyrus
L
 
388
7.84
20
−104
0
Middle occipital gyrus
R
 
275
6.6
−46
−70
4
Inferior occipital gyrus
L
 
441
7.84
38
−72
4
Inferior occipital gyrus
R
 
10
3.89
−12
60
34
Superior frontal gyrus
L
 
Table 6
Negative emotional faces vs neutral faces: areas of activation in the mirror neurons experiment with cluster size, T values of the local maximum, Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates, the anatomical areas within a cluster, the percentage of the cluster size and the functional area
Negative emotional faces vs neutral faces
  
Cluster size
T value
MNI coordinate (mm)
Anatomic area
Side
Functional area
  
x
y
z
   
706
16.74
42
−66
−32
Cerebellum crus 1
R
Cerebellum
362
16.74
18
−82
−40
Cerebellum crus 2
R
Cerebellum
267
16.74
−16
−82
−42
Cerebellum crus 2
L
Cerebellum
643
16.74
−42
−66
−34
Cerebellum crus 1
L
Cerebellum
18
16.74
−6
−46
−18
Cerebellum IV-I
L
Cerebellum
86
16.74
8
−46
−18
Cerebellum IV-I
R
Cerebellum
72
16.74
−6
−54
−50
Cerebellum IX
L
Cerebellum
27
16.74
8
−56
−48
Cerebellum IX
R
Cerebellum
820
16.74
−14
−70
−22
Cerebellum VI
L
Cerebellum
792
16.74
18
−68
−24
Cerebellum VI
R
Cerebellum
63
16.74
−34
−64
−54
Cerebellum VIIb
L
Cerebellum
45
16.74
20
−54
−56
Cerebellum VIIb
R
Cerebellum
95
16.74
−24
−60
−56
Cerebellum VIII
L
Cerebellum
9
16.74
28
−60
−56
Cerebellum VIII
R
Cerebellum
41
16.74
−24
−38
−46
Cerebellum X
L
Cerebellum
68
16.74
2
−58
−38
Vermis IX
 
Cerebellum
6
2.97
2
−70
−22
Vermis VI
 
Cerebellum
32
16.74
2
−64
−42
Vermis VIII
 
Cerebellum
127
16.74
−24
−2
−22
Amygdala
L
MN system
122
16.74
26
0
−22
Amygdala
R
MN system
394
16.74
−40
−22
−2
Insula
L
MN system
63
16.74
42
−20
−6
Insula
R
MN system
539
16.74
4
−24
62
Precentral gyrus
L
MN system
538
4.67
−12
−14
66
Supplementary motor area
L
MN system
1,530
16.74
52
−8
−8
Superior temporal gyrus
R
MN system
684
16.74
−56
−8
−8
Superior temporal gyrus
L
MN system
625
16.74
−42
22
2
Inferior opercular frontal gyrus
L
MN system
657
16.74
−52
16
22
Inferior opercular frontal gyrus
R
MN system
910
16.74
−42
24
−2
Inferior orbital frontal gyrus
L
MN system
530
16.74
36
26
−2
Inferior orbital frontal gyrus
R
MN system
1,300
16.74
−50
28
8
Inferior triangular frontal gyrus
L
MN system
643
16.74
−8
−24
64
Inferior triangular frontal gyrus
R
MN system
36
16.74
−44
8
56
Middle frontal gyrus
L
MN system
226
16.74
32
10
56
Middle frontal gyrus
R
MN system
All areas were thresholded at p < 0.05 with FDR correction for multiple comparisons and with a minimum cluster size of 5 voxels
L left hemisphere, R right hemisphere
Table 7
Imitation task vs observation task: areas of activation in the imitation task experiment with cluster size, T values of the local maximum, Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates, the anatomical areas within a cluster, the percentage of the cluster size and the functional area
Imitation Task vs Observation Task
  
Cluster Size
T value
MNI coordinate (mm)
Anatomic Area
Side
Functional area
  
x
y
z
   
163
10.00
−42
−66
−34
Cerebellum crus 1
L
Cerebellum
255
10.00
42
−66
−32
Cerebellum crus 1
R
Cerebellum
5
5.21
18
−82
−40
Cerebellum crus 2
R
Cerebellum
16
10.00
−6
−46
−18
Cerebellum IV-I
L
Cerebellum/motor system
90
10.00
8
−46
−18
Cerebellum IV-I
R
Cerebellum/motor system
225
10.00
−14
−70
−22
Cerebellum VI
L
Cerebellum/motor system
122
10.00
18
−68
−24
Cerebellum VI
R
Cerebellum/motor system
19
5.21
20
−54
−56
Cerebellum VIIb
R
Cerebellum
50
5.21
28
−60
−56
Cerebellum VIII
R
Cerebellum
15
10.00
2
−70
−22
Vermis VI
 
Cerebellum
116
10.00
−40
−22
−2
Insula
L
MN system
26
4.45
42
−20
−6
Insula
R
MN system
687
10.00
−4
−24
62
Precentral gyrus
L
MN system
47
10.67
4
−24
62
Precentral gyrus
R
MN system
327
10.00
−12
−14
66
Supplementary motor area
L
Motor system
847
10.00
−14
−42
68
Postcentral gyrus
L
MN system
123
10.67
−14
−42
66
Postcentral gyrus
R
MN system
19
10.00
12
4
74
Paracentral lobule
L
MN system
7
10.00
−54
−8
36
Paracentral lobule
R
MN system
163
10.00
−56
−8
−8
Superior temporal gyrus
L
MN system
11
4.57
52
−8
−8
Superior temporal gyrus
R
MN system
49
10.00
−42
22
2
Inferior operculus frontal gyrus
L
MN system/imitation
74
10.00
−52
16
22
Inferior operculus frontal gyrus
R
MN system/Imitation
15
10.00
−50
28
8
Inferior triangular frontal gyrus
L
MN system/Imitation
9
3.53
−44
8
56
Middle frontal gyrus
L
MN system
13
10.00
32
10
56
Middle frontal gyrus
R
MN system
99
10.00
−12
60
34
Superior frontal gyrus
L
MN system
46
5.56
−24
−2
−2
Pallidum
L
 
44
5.26
18
−2
−4
Pallidum
R
 
6
4.03
−26
−16
−20
Hippocampus
L
 
6
3.72
28
−14
−20
Hippocampus
R
 
9
5.07
22
−36
−14
Parahippocampal
R
 
176
10.00
−10
−58
38
Precuneus
L
 
134
10.00
8
−52
38
Precuneus
R
 
16
5.56
−22
8
−4
Putamen
L
 
32
5.05
22
10
−4
Putamen
R
 
282
10.00
−54
−48
30
Supramarginal
L
 
8
4.81
14
−24
6
Thalamus
R
 
17
5.97
−44
−4
−28
Inferior temporal gyrus
L
 
44
10.00
42
4
−38
Inferior temporal gyrus
R
 
11
10.00
−56
−8
−20
Middle temporal gyrus
L
 
32
10.00
54
−10
−20
Middle temporal gyrus
R
 
9
4.23
−40
6
−16
Middle temporal pole
L
 
37
5.07
46
12
−36
Middle temporal pole
R
 
56
10.00
36
−4
−32
Superior temporal pole
R
 
100
10.00
−8
−84
−6
Lingual
L
 
36
10.00
−18
−80
−12
Fusiform gyrus
L
 
47
10.00
24
−80
−12
Fusiform gyrus
R
 
38
5.97
−46
−70
4
Inferior occipital gyrus
L
 
24
10.00
38
−72
4
Inferior occipital gyrus
R
 
126
10.00
−38
−100
−23
Middle occipital gyrus
L
 
274
10.00
20
−104
0
Middle occipital gyrus
R
 
229
10.00
−30
−70
42
Superior occipital gyrus
L
 
200
10.00
30
−70
38
Superior occipital gyrus
R
 
All areas were thresholded at p < 0.05 with FDR correction for multiple comparisons and with a minimum cluster size of 5 voxels
L left hemisphere, R right hemisphere
Table 8
Scheme for the mirror neuron experiment
Experimental condition
Positive emotion
inter-stimulus interval (ISI)
Positive emotion
ISI
Positive emotion
ISI
Moving geometric shape
ISI
Moving geometric shape
ISI
Moving geometric shape
ISI
Time (s)
3
1
3
1
3
1
3
1
3
1
3
1
Experimental condition
Negative emotion
inter-stimulus interval (ISI)
Negative emotion
ISI
Negative emotion
ISI
Neutral face
ISI
Neutral face
ISI
Neutral face
ISI
Time (s)
3
1
3
1
3
1
3
1
3
1
3
1
12 times
The MN experimental stimuli consisted of full-face, full-color, 3-s video clips of five males and five females displaying various emotional states (anger, disgust, happiness, surprise, and neutrality). The control stimuli were clips of moving geometric shapes. Thus the four experimental conditions included specific clips of a single category: (1) positive emotional faces: happy and surprised, (2) negative emotional faces: angry and disgust, (3) neutral faces, and (4) moving geometric shapes. Each condition was presented 12 times in pseudo-randomized (counterbalanced between subjects) blocks lasting 12 s and comprising three clips. Clips were separated by a 1-s inter-stimulus interval (ISI), and conditions were separated by a 2-s ISI.
Table 9
Scheme for the imitation task experiment
Experimental condition
Observation task (show with a red cross)
Emotion
Observation task (show with a red cross)
Emotion
Observation task (show with a red cross)
Emotion
Observation task (show with a red cross)
Emotion
Observation task (show with a red cross)
Emotion
Time (s)
1
3
1
3
1
3
1
3
1
3
Experimental condition
Observation task (show with a red cross)
Emotion
Imitation task (show with a green cross)
Emotion
Imitation task (show with a green cross)
Emotion
Imitation task (show with a green cross)
Emotion
Imitation task (show with a green cross)
Emotion
Time (s)
1
3
1
3
1
3
1
3
1
3
Experimental condition
Imitation task (show with a green cross)
Emotion
Imitation task (show with a green cross)
Emotion
…..
X5 times
    
Time (s)
1
3
1
3
      
The IT experimental stimuli consisted of a mixture of happy, surprised, angry, and disgust emotional faces. The two main conditions that were tested are “observation” and “observation-execution” (i.e., imitation). In the observation condition, subjects were shown with a red cross for 1 s and then followed by one of the four emotional faces for 3 s and this pattern repeated six times with every time randomly with another emotional faces. Subjects were instructed to observe only. In the imitation condition, subjects were shown with a green cross instead and followed by again one of the emotional faces for 3 s and this pattern repeated six times with every time randomly with another emotional faces. However, subjects were instructed to imitate the observed emotional faces. All emotional faces were having the same amount of occurrence. The observation task and the imitation task were alternated five times each.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Ivry RB, Spencer RM. The neural representation of time. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14(2):225–32.PubMedCrossRef Ivry RB, Spencer RM. The neural representation of time. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14(2):225–32.PubMedCrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Schmahmann JD, Sherman JC. The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Brain. 1998;121(Pt 4):561–79.PubMedCrossRef Schmahmann JD, Sherman JC. The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Brain. 1998;121(Pt 4):561–79.PubMedCrossRef
3.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Fredrickson BL. The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2004;359(1449):1367–78.PubMedCrossRef Fredrickson BL. The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2004;359(1449):1367–78.PubMedCrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Oberman LM, Pineda JA, Ramachandran VS. The human mirror neuron system: A link between action observation and social skills. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007;2(1):62–6.PubMedCrossRef Oberman LM, Pineda JA, Ramachandran VS. The human mirror neuron system: A link between action observation and social skills. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007;2(1):62–6.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Oberman LM, Ramachandran VS. The simulating social mind: the role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum disorders. Psychol Bull. 2007;133(2):310–27.PubMedCrossRef Oberman LM, Ramachandran VS. The simulating social mind: the role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum disorders. Psychol Bull. 2007;133(2):310–27.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Oberman LM, Winkielman P, Ramachandran VS. Face to face: blocking facial mimicry can selectively impair recognition of emotional expressions. Soc Neurosci. 2007;2(3–4):167–78.PubMedCrossRef Oberman LM, Winkielman P, Ramachandran VS. Face to face: blocking facial mimicry can selectively impair recognition of emotional expressions. Soc Neurosci. 2007;2(3–4):167–78.PubMedCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Preston SD, de Waal FB. Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behav Brain Sci. 2002;25(1):1–20. discussion 20–71.PubMed Preston SD, de Waal FB. Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behav Brain Sci. 2002;25(1):1–20. discussion 20–71.PubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Barsalou LW. Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain Sci. 1999;22(4):577–609. discussion 610–60.PubMed Barsalou LW. Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain Sci. 1999;22(4):577–609. discussion 610–60.PubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Carr L et al. Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for limitation to limbic areas. PNAS. 2003;100(April):5497–502.PubMedCrossRef Carr L et al. Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for limitation to limbic areas. PNAS. 2003;100(April):5497–502.PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Iacoboni M, Dapretto M. The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7(12):942–51.PubMedCrossRef Iacoboni M, Dapretto M. The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7(12):942–51.PubMedCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Lamm C, Batson CD, Decety J. The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19(1):42–58.PubMedCrossRef Lamm C, Batson CD, Decety J. The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19(1):42–58.PubMedCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Wager TD, Lindquist M, Kaplan L. Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging data: current and future directions. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007;2(2):150–8.PubMedCrossRef Wager TD, Lindquist M, Kaplan L. Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging data: current and future directions. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007;2(2):150–8.PubMedCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Buccino G et al. Neural circuits underlying imitation learning of hand actions: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron. 2004;42(2):323–34.PubMedCrossRef Buccino G et al. Neural circuits underlying imitation learning of hand actions: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron. 2004;42(2):323–34.PubMedCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Lane RD et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of happiness, sadness, and disgust. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154(7):926–33.PubMed Lane RD et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of happiness, sadness, and disgust. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154(7):926–33.PubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Turner BM et al. The cerebellum and emotional experience. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(6):1331–41.PubMedCrossRef Turner BM et al. The cerebellum and emotional experience. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(6):1331–41.PubMedCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Garrett AS, Maddock RJ. Separating subjective emotion from the perception of emotion-inducing stimuli: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2006;33(1):263–74.PubMedCrossRef Garrett AS, Maddock RJ. Separating subjective emotion from the perception of emotion-inducing stimuli: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2006;33(1):263–74.PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Liotti M, Mayberg HS. The role of functional neuroimaging in the neuropsychology of depression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2001;23(1):121–36.PubMedCrossRef Liotti M, Mayberg HS. The role of functional neuroimaging in the neuropsychology of depression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2001;23(1):121–36.PubMedCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat van der Gaag C, Minderaa RB, Keysers C. Facial expressions: what the mirror neuron system can and cannot tell us. Soc Neurosci. 2007;2(3–4):179–222.PubMedCrossRef van der Gaag C, Minderaa RB, Keysers C. Facial expressions: what the mirror neuron system can and cannot tell us. Soc Neurosci. 2007;2(3–4):179–222.PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Rizzolatti, G. and M. Fabbri-Destro, Mirror neurons: from discovery to autism. Exp Brain Res, 2009. Rizzolatti, G. and M. Fabbri-Destro, Mirror neurons: from discovery to autism. Exp Brain Res, 2009.
22.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Wicker B et al. Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron. 2003;40(3):655–64.PubMedCrossRef Wicker B et al. Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron. 2003;40(3):655–64.PubMedCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Bastiaanse JA et al. Age-Related Increase in Inferior Frontal Gyrus Activity and Social Functioning in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Biol Psych. 2011;69(9):832.CrossRef Bastiaanse JA et al. Age-Related Increase in Inferior Frontal Gyrus Activity and Social Functioning in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Biol Psych. 2011;69(9):832.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Gallese V., et al. Action representation and the inferior parietal lobule. In Attention & Performance XIX.CommonMechanisms inPerception and Action, ed.WPrinz, B Hommel, (2002) pp. 247–66. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press Gallese V., et al. Action representation and the inferior parietal lobule. In Attention & Performance XIX.CommonMechanisms inPerception and Action, ed.WPrinz, B Hommel, (2002) pp. 247–66. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Tzourio-Mazoyer N et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage. 2002;15(1):273–89.PubMedCrossRef Tzourio-Mazoyer N et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage. 2002;15(1):273–89.PubMedCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Carr L et al. Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(9):5497–502.PubMedCrossRef Carr L et al. Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(9):5497–502.PubMedCrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Han S et al. Neural processing of threat cues in social environments. Hum Brain Mapp. 2008;29(8):945–57.PubMedCrossRef Han S et al. Neural processing of threat cues in social environments. Hum Brain Mapp. 2008;29(8):945–57.PubMedCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat de Waal FBW. Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of Empathy. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:279–300.PubMedCrossRef de Waal FBW. Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of Empathy. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:279–300.PubMedCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Fogassi L et al. Neurons responding to the sight of goal directed hand/arm actions in the parietal area PF (7b) of the macaque monkey. Soc Neurosci. 1998;24:257.5. Fogassi L et al. Neurons responding to the sight of goal directed hand/arm actions in the parietal area PF (7b) of the macaque monkey. Soc Neurosci. 1998;24:257.5.
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Schmahmann JD. The role of the cerebellum in affect and psychosis. J Neurolinguist. 2000;13:189–214.CrossRef Schmahmann JD. The role of the cerebellum in affect and psychosis. J Neurolinguist. 2000;13:189–214.CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Schmahmann JD. Disorders of the cerebellum: ataxia, dysmetria of thought, and the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2004;16(3):367–78.PubMedCrossRef Schmahmann JD. Disorders of the cerebellum: ataxia, dysmetria of thought, and the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2004;16(3):367–78.PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Schmahmann JD, Weilburg JB, Sherman JC. The neuropsychiatry of the cerebellum - insights from the clinic. Cerebellum. 2007;6(3):254–67.PubMedCrossRef Schmahmann JD, Weilburg JB, Sherman JC. The neuropsychiatry of the cerebellum - insights from the clinic. Cerebellum. 2007;6(3):254–67.PubMedCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Leslie KR, Johnson-Frey SH, Grafton ST. Functional imaging of face and hand imitation: towards a motor theory of empathy. NeuroImage. 2004;21(2):601–7.PubMedCrossRef Leslie KR, Johnson-Frey SH, Grafton ST. Functional imaging of face and hand imitation: towards a motor theory of empathy. NeuroImage. 2004;21(2):601–7.PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Damasio AR et al. Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3(10):1049–56.PubMedCrossRef Damasio AR et al. Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3(10):1049–56.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
fMRI Activities in the Emotional Cerebellum: A Preference for Negative Stimuli and Goal-Directed Behavior
verfasst von
Caroline K. L. Schraa-Tam
Willem J. R. Rietdijk
Willem J. M. I. Verbeke
Roeland C. Dietvorst
Wouter E. van den Berg
Richard P. Bagozzi
Chris I. De Zeeuw
Publikationsdatum
01.03.2012
Verlag
Springer-Verlag
Erschienen in
The Cerebellum / Ausgabe 1/2012
Print ISSN: 1473-4222
Elektronische ISSN: 1473-4230
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-011-0301-2

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2012

The Cerebellum 1/2012 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Schützt Olivenöl vor dem Tod durch Demenz?

10.05.2024 Morbus Alzheimer Nachrichten

Konsumieren Menschen täglich 7 Gramm Olivenöl, ist ihr Risiko, an einer Demenz zu sterben, um mehr als ein Viertel reduziert – und dies weitgehend unabhängig von ihrer sonstigen Ernährung. Dafür sprechen Auswertungen zweier großer US-Studien.

Bluttest erkennt Parkinson schon zehn Jahre vor der Diagnose

10.05.2024 Parkinson-Krankheit Nachrichten

Ein Bluttest kann abnorm aggregiertes Alpha-Synuclein bei einigen Menschen schon zehn Jahre vor Beginn der motorischen Parkinsonsymptome nachweisen. Mit einem solchen Test lassen sich möglicherweise Prodromalstadien erfassen und die Betroffenen früher behandeln.

Darf man die Behandlung eines Neonazis ablehnen?

08.05.2024 Gesellschaft Nachrichten

In einer Leseranfrage in der Zeitschrift Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology möchte ein anonymer Dermatologe bzw. eine anonyme Dermatologin wissen, ob er oder sie einen Patienten behandeln muss, der eine rassistische Tätowierung trägt.

Wartezeit nicht kürzer, aber Arbeit flexibler

Psychotherapie Medizin aktuell

Fünf Jahren nach der Neugestaltung der Psychotherapie-Richtlinie wurden jetzt die Effekte der vorgenommenen Änderungen ausgewertet. Das Hauptziel der Novellierung war eine kürzere Wartezeit auf Therapieplätze. Dieses Ziel wurde nicht erreicht, es gab jedoch positive Auswirkungen auf andere Bereiche.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.