Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Public Health 1/2014

Open Access 01.12.2014 | Research article

Does physical activity prevent cognitive decline and dementia?: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies

verfasst von: Sarah J Blondell, Rachel Hammersley-Mather, J Lennert Veerman

Erschienen in: BMC Public Health | Ausgabe 1/2014

Abstract

Background

By 2050, it has been estimated that approximately one-fifth of the population will be made up of older adults (aged ≥60 years). Old age often comes with cognitive decline and dementia. Physical activity may prevent cognitive decline and dementia.

Methods

We reviewed and synthesised prospective studies into physical activity and cognitive decline, and physical activity and dementia, published until January 2014. Forty-seven cohorts, derived from two previous systematic reviews and an updated database search, were used in the meta-analyses. Included participants were aged ≥40 years, in good health and/or randomly selected from the community. Studies were assessed for methodological quality.

Results

Twenty-one cohorts on physical activity and cognitive decline and twenty-six cohorts on physical activity and dementia were included. Meta-analysis, using the quality-effects model, suggests that participants with higher levels of physical activity, when compared to those with lower levels, are at reduced risk of cognitive decline, RR 0.65, 95% CI 0.55-0.76, and dementia, RR 0.86, 95% CI 0.76-0.97. Sensitivity analyses revealed a more conservative estimate of the impact of physical activity on cognitive decline and dementia for high quality studies, studies reporting effect sizes as ORs, greater number of adjustments (≥10), and longer follow-up time (≥10 years). When one heavily weighted study was excluded, physical activity was associated with an 18% reduction in the risk of dementia (RR 0.82; 0.73-0.91).

Conclusions

Longitudinal observational studies show an association between higher levels of physical activity and a reduced risk of cognitive decline and dementia. A case can be made for a causal interpretation. Future research should use objective measures of physical activity, adjust for the full range of confounders and have adequate follow-up length. Ideally, randomised controlled trials will be conducted. Regardless of any effect on cognition, physical activity should be encouraged, as it has been shown to be beneficial on numerous levels.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1471-2458-14-510) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

SJB participated in the design of the study, extracted data, performed the statistical analysis, drafted the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript. RHM participated in the data extraction and read and approved the final manuscript. JLV conceived the study, and participated in its design and coordination, and read and approved the final manuscript.
Abkürzungen
AD
Alzheimer’s Disease
CI
Confidence Interval
CIND
Cognitive Impairment No Dementia
DSM
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
HR
Hazard Ratio
ICD
International Classification of Disease
MCI
Mild Cognitive Impairment
MMSE
Mini-Mental State Exam
OR
Odds Ratio
RCT
Randomised Control Trial
RR
Relative Risk
SD
Standard Deviation
VaD
Vascular Dementia.

Background

Cognitive decline

By 2050, it has been estimated that the world will number 2 billion older adults, aged ≥60 years, or approximately one-fifth (22%) of the population [1]. Such a demographic profile will have significant implications on a multitude of dimensions, including social, economic and health [1]. Individuals aged ≥65 years are over twice as likely as a 20 year old to be afflicted with ≥ 1 chronic disease, 84 per cent compared to 38 per cent respectively, and other physical and mental disturbances [2]. One particular issue with potentially significant implications for all of the aforementioned factors, is cognitive decline. Research suggests that cognitive decline is associated with the aging process, particularly from age ≥50 years [3]. Whilst some cognitive decline, stemming from reduced brain size and plasticity, is normally occurring in most people, not all cognitive decline is considered ‘normal’ [4, 5]. The following will outline, in order of severity, ‘abnormal’ cognitive decline.

Cognitive impairment no dementia (CIND)

CIND refers to the subjective and objective cognitive decline, which is neither normal nor extreme, experienced by individuals, and which is not sufficient to warrant a diagnosis of dementia [6]. This form of cognitive decline is not debilitating but, nevertheless, causes worry to the sufferer [6]. The criteria for CIND diagnoses are not yet universally agreed upon, but generally relate to a decline in cognitive domains, such as memory, executive function or praxis, that is deemed worse than that of individuals in the same age bracket, of the same sex and educational level [7]. A CIND diagnosis may be made regardless of known underlying mechanisms, such as delirium, substance abuse and psychiatric illness [8]. Approximately one-fifth of Americans aged ≥ 71 years are thought to suffer from CIND [9]. CIND may be a stand-alone illness or may be a precursor to dementia [9]. A significant proportion, namely half to two-thirds, of those individuals fitting the CIND classification may be further classified as having MCI [10].

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI)

MCI is an ever- evolving term; however, it is most widely used to categorise older adults who present with subjective and objective memory impairment that cannot be explained by an underlying neurological or psychiatric ailment, and whose other cognitive functions are generally intact with no impediment to their everyday activities and life [10]. MCI is considered a precursor to dementia and AD.
Neuroimaging of people with MCI has shown early structural changes that are in keeping with what would be expected of AD sufferers [10]. Annually, 10 to 15 per cent of individuals with MCI progress to dementia, as compared to 1 to 2 per cent of healthy adults, and this figure is thought to be on the rise [6]. It is important to note from this that clinically-recognised abnormal cognitive decline represents only an increased risk for dementia, and does not mean that dementia is inevitable; in fact, people may revert back to a previous unimpaired state [11]. The identification of cognitive impairment before full-blown/clinically diagnosed dementia may, however, provide the opportunity for possible preventative measures.

Dementia

Dementia, like CIND and MCI, is an illness primarily seen and diagnosed in older adulthood; some 90–98 per cent of cases are aged ≥65 years [1]. Dementia is a syndrome that is typified by memory impairment, as well as ≥1 other cognitive deficit [12]. Dementia may be diagnosed if these deficits (1) are incapacitating for the individual, i.e. he/she is no longer able to function in daily tasks; (2) represent a decline from previous functioning; and (3) are not attributable exclusively to a delirium, i.e. a short-onset disturbance in cognition [12]. A number of categories of dementia exist and represent a number of different underlying factors, including dementia with Lewy bodies, vascular dementia(VaD) and Alzheimer’s disease (AD), with the latter two sub-types being the most common [13, 14].
In 2010, the number of people with dementia was reportedly 35.6 million, with AD and VaD accounting for 80 per cent of all clinically diagnosed dementia cases [1, 14]. There are currently no curative treatment options for sufferers of dementia [15]. Given that, at its extremes, dementia may cause a sufferer to forget key biographical information, including his/her own name and family members, it is not surprising that people with dementia often require some significant degree of care [12]. In 2010, it was estimated that the world-wide economic cost of dementia was US$604 billion, the vast majority of which was not provided by the formal sector, but rather by unpaid informal carers, i.e. spouse, child [1, 5]. Dementia, therefore, is a huge burden for sufferers, their carers, families, friends and the wider community.
Given the high prevalence of dementia, limited treatment options, the costs of the disease to individuals, families and society, and the increased risk of a subset of those with CIND to develop dementia, it is important to explore potential risk and protective factors for both cognitive decline and dementia.

Risk and protective factors for cognitive decline and dementia

Given the apparent connectedness between cognitive decline and dementia, it seems fitting that they would share many of the same risk and protective factors. Both cognitive decline and dementia share common risk factors. Some of these are amenable to change, others not. Non-modifiable factors associated with both cognitive decline and dementia include age, sex and genetics (particularly APOE genes) [11, 16]. Whilst it is important to be aware of these, from a preventive point of view, the focus needs to be on measures that are modifiable. Cardiovascular risk factors, including hypertension, diabetes, the metabolic syndrome, obesity and smoking are considered as risk factors for both cognitive decline and dementia [1, 11, 15]. There is suggestive evidence that lifestyle factors may contribute to, or be protective of, cognitive decline and dementia, one such example being physical activity [7].

Physical activity as a protective factor for cognitive decline and dementia

Physical activity is defined as the movement of skeletal muscles, resulting in energy expenditure exceeding the resting state [17]. Physical activity, which encompasses exercise, is different from physical fitness [18]. One can be physically active without necessarily having high aerobic fitness [19]. Physical activity is noted as being beneficial across many domains, including cardiovascular disease, cancer and depression [5].
Much research has been undertaken to assess whether physical activity reduces cognitive decline and prevents dementia. Results from prospective studies have shown mixed results [20, 21]. Aarsland et al. 2010, for example, noted that of five studies looking into physical activity and cognitive decline, two reported a significant relationship for both sexes, two reported a significant relationship for females only, and one showed no association [16]. A similar picture emerges for dementia - Aarsland et al. 2010 found that, of 12 studies examining physical activity and dementia, over half did not find statistically significant effects [16]. However, despite individual variation between studies, systematic reviews and meta-analyses of longitudinal studies do lend support to the association between physical activity, cognitive decline and dementia.
Sofi et al. 2011 [20] undertook a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies exploring physical activity and risk of cognitive decline in non-demented individuals. Cognitive decline, or cognitive impairment, was defined as decline in tests of cognitive function at follow-up (no minimum threshold was specified). Participants with high and low-to-moderate levels of physical activity were compared to those who were sedentary. Included as part of the meta-analyses were 12 prospective studies (15 cohorts) published up until January 2010 [2233]. All studies, bar one, had participants aged ≥65 years. Both low-to-moderate level (HR 0.65, 95% CI 0.57- 0.75; p = <0.001) and high level (HR 0.62, 95% CI 0.54-0.70; p = <0.001) physical activity were shown to be protective of cognitive decline.
Hamer & Chida 2009 [34] explored the relationship between physical activity and neurodegenerative diseases in healthy adults, at baseline, in meta-analyses of prospective cohort studies. The outcome measure was a diagnosis of dementia or cognitive impairment at follow-up. Included as part of the meta-analyses were nine studies on dementia and AD (10 cohorts), published up until 2007 [23, 3542]. The highest level of physical activity, when compared with the lowest level, gave a relative risk (RR) of 0.72 (95% CI 0.60 - 0.86, p = <0.001) for dementia and 0.55 (95% CI 0.36 - 0.84, p = <0.006) for AD.
The following review will summarise results from prospective studies into physical activity and cognitive decline, and physical activity and dementia up to January 2014. The present review is undertaken in view of additional original research in the field, including a number of studies that include objective measures of physical activity. This review also utilises a quality assessment tool to improve the validity of results [43]. Examined in this review are individuals aged ≥40 years who were in good health and/or randomly selected from the community.

Methods

This paper uses the preferred reporting items for a systematic review and meta-analysis (PRISMA) to ensure accuracy and comprehensiveness [44]. A review protocol was written prior to undertaking the searches. The paper has been registered with PROSPERO, registration number CRD42014008722.

Search strategy

This review builds on the work of the two most recently published systematic reviews on physical activity and cognitive decline and dementia. Sofi et al. 2011 [20] explored the relationship between physical activity and cognitive decline and Hamer & Chida 2009 [34] looked at the association between physical activity and dementia. An updated database search retrieved papers published after these reviews. Two modes, therefore, as outlined below, were used to search for relevant literature.

Previous systematic reviews

Previous systematic reviews on physical activity preventing cognitive decline or dementia were included in the current review. The reference lists of the Sofi et al. 2011 [20] and Hamer & Chida 2009 [34] articles were examined, and relevant articles sought, providing evidence up until 2010 and 2007, respectively.
Searches were conducted to identify any relevant studies published since 2007 (the cut-off date for the earliest literature searches from the previous reviews). The PubMed and PsycInfo databases were searched for studies published between 1 January 2007 and 31 December 2013. Terms searched were ((“physical activity” OR “exercise”) and (“cognitive decline” OR “dementia” OR “cognitive impairment” OR “Alzheimer’s disease” OR “cognition”)). The search was further limited, using filters available in the databases, to journal articles with subjects aged ≥ 40 years.

Study selection

Study selection was limited to those (1) utilising a prospective design; (2) utilising a population-based sample; (3) with a definition of what constitutes cognitive decline or dementia, and a description of the methods used to assess these; (4) with data on baseline physical activity; (5) original research articles from dates after identified/included reviews; and (6) that reported estimates of association between physical activity subgroups and cognitive decline or dementia.

Data extraction

Key data from the relevant studies were independently extracted and tabulated by two investigators with a master-level degree in health sciences (SJB, RHM). Inclusion of studies was based firstly, on title; secondly, on abstract; or thirdly, on full text. The PRISMA 2009 flow diagram was used as a template for reporting study inclusion [44]. Discussion was undertaken post-extraction by the investigators to resolve any differences of opinion. Where agreement could not be reached, a third investigator with a PhD (JLV), was consulted. Additional file 1: Table S1 presents the extracted information.

Quality assessment

Included studies were assessed by the two independent reviewers, as described above, for methodological quality with the use of a tool adapted from Singh et al. 2012 [45]. Additional file 2: Table S2 contains the ten items that were used to assess study quality. Each of the items was weighted equally, with a score of one representing a ‘yes’, and zero a ‘no’. Where a paper cited other papers (for further details) the additional papers were sought; however, if the secondary papers did not contain the required information, no additional papers were sought. In such instances the criterion in question was given a score of ‘0’, for not being stated. If a paper used a standardised measure, and did not report the reliability or validity, a cursory search of Google Scholar (using the measure name and ‘validity’ or ‘reliability’) was undertaken. If the required information could not be readily accessed (in the first page of search results), then a ‘0’ was given. A score (out of ten) was allocated to papers by each reviewer, and any points of difference were discussed until an agreement could be met. Where an agreement could not be met, a third investigator, as above, was consulted. In line with Singh et al. 2012, studies with a score equating to ≥70 per cent were considered as “high quality”, and those <70 per cent were deemed “low quality” studies [45].

Statistical analysis

MS Excel with add-in MetaXL [freely available at http://​www.​epigear.​com/​index_​files/​metaxl.​html] was utilised to synthesise the effect sizes from individual studies, with the most fully-adjusted included in the meta-analyses. All effect sizes were reported in their original format, and outputs given as relative risks (RRs). Analyses were carried out on the highest level of physical activity compared to the lowest level of physical activity for cognitive decline and dementia, separately, as this was how most studies reported their results. Heterogeneity was examined in multiple ways. Firstly, it was assessed via the I2 statistic, whereby an I2 statistic of 25 per cent was considered low; 50 per cent, moderate; and 75 per cent, high [46]. Secondly, sensitivity analyses were conducted to address possible sources of heterogeneity. Potential publication bias was assessed with the use of funnel plots.

Results

Search outputs

Previous reviews

Twelve prospective studies (15 cohorts) [2233] were obtained from the Sofi et al. 2011 [20] systematic meta-analysis on physical activity and risk of cognitive decline, see Figure 1. Nine studies on dementia and AD (10 cohorts) [23, 3542] were derived from the systematic review by Hamer & Chida 2009 [34] on physical activity and neurodegenerative diseases (including dementia).
The searches yielded 1916 titles. Of those, 94 potentially relevant papers where identified and full text articles retrieved; see Figure 1. Twenty studies were not original research, seventeen papers had inappropriate study design, no definition of cognitive decline/dementia was given in twenty-five studies, eleven studies reported on outcomes not of interest, and five articles identified were reported in the previous reviews. Sixteen studies met the inclusion criteria and are included in the review [21, 4761]. One small study reported only ORs for the combined condition MCI/AD (Active vs inactive OR: 0.93; 95% CI 0.45-1.90) [62]. Inclusion of this study did not change the results in either analysis, and was excluded from the analysis/not reported (“outcome not of interest”).

Study characteristics

Cognitive decline

Assessed were 17 prospective studies (21 cohorts), 12 studies from the Sofi et al. review paper [20] and 5 additional studies identified in the database search [4751], on physical activity and cognitive decline, see Additional file 1: Table S1. The majority of the cohorts were mixed (n = 9); followed by male only (n = 7) and female only (n = 5). Studies were conducted world-wide, including China, Italy, Netherlands, Australia, Canada, Singapore, Germany, Japan, UK and USA. The sample sizes ranged from 27 to 12303. Follow-up time ranged from 1 to 21 years. Cognitive decline was assessed primarily with the MMSE, 8 cohorts, and the modified MMSE, 5 cohorts. Assessment of physical activity was undertaken via the use of questionnaires, with the exception of one study that used doubly-labelled water and indirect calorimetry [49]. All studies adjusted for confounders, the number of factors ranging from 1 to 18. Sixteen cohorts controlled for at least age and education, two controlled for age and National Reading Test score, two controlled for age and education only, respectively, and one controlled for neither.

Dementia

Twenty-one studies on physical activity and dementia (26 cohorts) were examined in relation to physical activity and dementia. Nine studies were from the previous review paper [34] and twelve additional studies were identified in the database search [21, 50, 5261] (Additional file 1: Table S1). The majority of the cohorts were mixed sex (n = 18); however, some studies reported data for males only (n = 7) and females only (n = 1). Studies represented countries globally, including Finland, Italy, UK, Australia, Korea, Iceland, Canada, France, Nigeria, the Netherlands, Japan, Hawaii and USA. The sample sizes ranged from 469 to 4945. Follow-up time ranged from 1 to 26 years. Dementia was primarily assessed with the use of a version of the DSM. With the exception of one study that utilised data from an Actigraph [52], physical activity was assessed with the use of questionnaires. Age and education were almost universally adjusted for in all cohorts, age and education (n = 20), age and National Reading Test score (n = 2), age (n = 2), education (n = 1), and neither (n = 1). Other co-variates adjusted for across the studies were broadly classified as demographics, health indicators, gene type and lifestyle factors.

Quality assessment

A total of eight studies of physical activity and cognitive decline were defined as being of high quality and thirteen studies were deemed as low quality. Fifteen studies on physical activity and dementia were high quality, and the remaining eleven were low quality, see Additional file 3: Table S3.

Meta-analysis

In light of the heterogeneous nature of the studies included in both analyses (See Additional file 1: Table S1), RRs are derived from a quality effects model. The quality effects model is used when studies differ in relation to study design [43].

Cognitive decline

Results suggest that participants with higher levels of physical activity, when compared to those with lower levels, are protected against cognitive decline (RR 0.65, 95% CI 0.55-0.76), as shown in Figure 2. There was moderate heterogeneity between the studies (I2 = 52%).

Dementia

Results suggest that higher levels of physical activity, versus lower levels of physical activity, are associated with a 14% reduction in the risk of dementia (RR 0.86, 95% CI 0.76-0.97), see Figure 3. Heterogeneity between the studies was high (I2 = 66%).

Sensitivity analyses

Sensitivity analyses were performed in view of the non-homogenous nature of studies in both domains. Studies were grouped by low quality or high quality, ORs or RRs, follow-up time <10 or ≥ 10 years, and number of adjustments <10 or ≥ 10 (see Additional file 4: Table S4). For both cognitive decline and dementia, studies reporting ORs, RR 0.67 and RR 0.89, studies of high quality, RR 0.73 and RR 0.87, with a greater number of adjustments (≥10), RR 0.68 and RR 0.86, and longer follow-up time (≥10 years), RR 0.89 and RR 0.86, respectively, provided more conservative findings. An additional sensitivity analysis was run for the dementia studies by excluding one small study [37] that was weighted heavily in the quality effects model due to its small confidence intervals. Exclusion of this study (Wilson et al. 2002 [37]) strengthened the negative association of physical activity with dementia, RR 0.82 (0.73 - 0.91).

Publication bias

Publication bias was assessed via funnel plots, see Figures 4 and 5. The funnel plot for cognitive decline does not suggest publication bias, but publication bias may have influenced the findings for dementia, where the heavily-weighted study by Wilson (2002) found no effect of physical activity [37].

Discussion

The aim of this article was to review, update and quantify the association between physical activity, cognitive decline and dementia. The current review shows significant negative associations of physical activity with cognitive decline and dementia, with overall effects of RR 0.65, 95% CI 0.55-0.76 and RR 0.86, 95% CI 0.76-0.97, respectively. The addition of more recent studies to the previous reviews marginally diminished the relationship between physical activity and cognitive decline, and physical activity and dementia. Publication bias may have influenced the results for dementia. Excluding the study by Wilson et al. [37], put the effect of physical activity on dementia as stronger than those reported in the overall findings. This analysis brought results for dementia more in line with those witnessed for cognitive decline. Unlike most authors, Wilson et al. [37] analysed hours of physical activity per week and dementia, not high versus low physical activity. This resulted in (1) more efficient use of the data, and thus (2) more power, (3) a lesser effect, but smaller confidence intervals and, therefore, a weight of approximately 35 per cent in the quality-effects analysis. While this analysis clearly differs from the others, our pre-defined selection criteria, however, provided no grounds to remove the study (despite it being a small study that was weighted heavily in the analysis). On the whole the results do, however, lend support to the notion of a causal relationship between physical activity, cognitive decline and dementia, according to the established criteria for causal inference.

Bradford Hill criteria

Austin Bradford Hill first established guidelines for causality, which is today often considered the leading method for identifying a causal relationship [63]. It is important to note that Bradford Hill himself did not present these factors as needing to be strictly adhered to, but rather as aids for interpreting associations [64]. The Bradford Hill criteria are (1) strength of association; (2) consistency; (3) specificity; (4) temporality; (5) biological gradient; (6) plausibility; and (7) coherence [65]. While many of these factors are not infallible, the proposed considerations are still useful [65]. In the following paragraphs, we will discuss each in turn.

Association

This study confirms that there is an association between physical activity and cognitive decline and dementia. In terms of strength, Webb & Bain 2011 [66] suggest that RR 0.20 should be considered strong, and RR 0.50 considered moderately strong. In line with this, then, the associations established by this review may be considered as low-to-moderate, although the magnitude may in fact be inaccurate due to several factors, for example publication bias.
Publication bias refers to the under-reporting of results that fail to find significant positive associations [46]. The funnel plot for cognitive decline does not suggest publication bias. However, publication bias may have influenced the findings for dementia, whereby a large number of smaller studies show a larger-than-average effect. Misleading effect sizes may stem also from inadequate control for confounders.
A number of variables have been identified as possible confounders in the relationship between physical activity, cognitive decline and dementia. Scarmeas et al. 2009 [60] identify and control for a comprehensive list of variables in their study; however, a number of other studies included in the current review failed to adequately adjust their data for known confounders. Potential confounders to be controlled for include age, sex, educational level, alcohol consumption, smoking status, depression score, stress, anxiety and cardiovascular disease [4, 16]. Incomplete adjustment for confounders may inflate the association between physical activity, cognitive decline and dementia and, therefore, control for confounders should be a priority for researchers. One dementia study [42] included in the meta-analysis did not adjust for age or education, both key/plausible confounders; however, exclusion of this study did not alter results significantly (RR: 0.86, 0.76-0.98). Sensitivity analysis into number of adjustments, for example, revealed that those studies which adjusted for a greater number of confounders (≥10), as compared to a lesser number (<10), found a smaller protective effect of physical activity on cognitive decline, RR 0.68 (0.51 – 0.91), and dementia, RR 0.86 (0.77 – 0.96). Inadequate control for confounders may also have implications for consistency.

Consistency

Consistency relates to the reliability of outcomes across different settings, populations, study designs, etc. [64]. Consistency, therefore, inherently relies on sound methodological underpinnings. In the present study, inconsistent protective effects may be traced to the many methodological issues in both domains, as well as to limits in the power of individual studies and, perhaps, real differences in effects across age and other characteristics of the population under study.
Plassman et al. 2010 [7], in their systematic review of observational studies on factors associated with risk for, and possible prevention of, cognitive decline in later life, found cognitive decline to be classified in numerous ways, for example categorical or continuous. However, one test is used predominantly in the literature, the MMSE; in the current review, eight studies utilised the MMSE to assess cognitive decline. The MMSE, a brief 30-item cognitive test, has been the most widely used test to determine an individual’s cognitive health, in terms of possible cognitive impairment or dementia. Scores on the MMSE of <24 points are considered to be indicative of dementia, and >3 point score declines are in line with mild impairment [20]. However, the use of this test is increasingly being challenged, as it is considered to have insufficient scope, and, therefore sensitivity, to identify mild forms of cognitive decline [67]. A modified version of the MMSE, the 3MS, which has an altered scoring system and additional questions, was the second most widely used cognition test across the studies in the review [68]. The 3MS is noted as having slightly greater validity and reliability than the MMSE [69]. The screening/assessment of cognitive decline using tests designed to screen for dementia is, however, noted as potentially biasing persons with cognitive decline toward sub-clinical dementia [8]. The utilisation of psychometric tests not specifically designed for assessment of dementia may be more appropriate, and may limit the aforementioned bias.
So, too, it has been noted that physical activity is a somewhat ill-defined term [7]. Physical activity can be described in terms of leisure activity only, all activity, “sweat index”, etc. [4, 62]. These physical activity indicators are then most often assessed via self-reported questionnaire; however, such a subjective method may introduce bias, for example recall and social desirability bias [4]. The physical activity questionnaires utilised in the papers included in the review often failed to provide sufficient detail on activity type, frequency, duration, intensity, and, therefore, limited insights can be drawn of clinical significance. Further, the majority of studies did not report information on the validity or reliability of the measures. Even when physical activity questionnaires are more in depth, however, Middleton et al. 2011 [49] note that older adults may not adequately assess their range of activities, particularly low-intensity physical activity. Inaccuracies in measurement of physical activity, as outlined above, may lead to regression dilution bias, whereby imprecise measurement of the exposure variable may bias a result toward null [70]. There is, therefore, a great need to use objective measures such as accelerometry [4]. Only two studies in the present review utilised an objective measure of physical activity. Buchman [52] used actigraphy in 716 older adults, with a follow-up time of 3.5 years, and found total daily physical activity to be significantly protective for AD (HR 0.53, 95% CI 0.29-0.95). Likewise, Middleton et al. 2011 [49], in their study of 197 subjects over a follow-up time of 5 to 8 years, found a highly protective effect of physical activity on cognitive decline, as measured via doubly-labelled water and indirect calorimetry (high vs low OR: 0.09, 95% CI 0.01-0.79).

Specificity

Specificity, the notion that one agent causes one disease, is not widely apparent in the physical activity, cognitive decline and dementia domains [65]. As outlined above, numerous factors may contribute to the outcomes of interest. However, the lessened risk for cognitive decline experienced by physically active individuals with the APOE 4 gene, offers strength to the notion of causality, in regard to specificity of susceptibility [66, 71]. It must be noted, however, that specificity is not central to establishing a causal relationship, unlike temporality [66].

Temporality

Reverse causality needs to be considered in the association between physical activity, cognitive decline and dementia. Could cognitive decline and dementia lead to a lack of physical activity, rather than the reverse? It is noted, for example, that individuals with pre-clinical AD may be less inclined to undertake physical activity [1]. To overcome such potential issues around causality, longer follow-up time may be beneficial. The current review had studies with follow-up times ranging from 1 to 21 years for cognitive decline, and 1 to 26 years for dementia. Sensitivity analyses for longer (≥10 year) and shorter (<10 year) follow-up time showed that studies with longer follow-up time found weaker protective effect of physical activity for cognitive decline, RR 0.89 (0.62 – 1.27), and dementia, RR 0.86 (0.68 – 1.11). The recent study by Morgan et al. 2012 [50], with a follow-up time of 16 years, did not lend support to the protective effect of physical activity on cognitive decline or dementia. However, Chang et al. 2010 [53], with the longest follow-up time of all studies, 26 years, found a significant association of physical activity on cognitive decline. Likewise, for dementia, Rovio et al. 2005 [41] found physical activity to be protective over an extended follow-up period (active vs sedentary OR: 0.47, 95% CI 0.25-0.90). The inconsistencies witnessed in establishing temporality are also mirrored in relation to biological gradient.

Biological gradient

Dose–response relationships have not been widely reported in the literature. Laurin et al., 2001 [23], for example, witnessed a dose–response relationship in relation to physical activity and cognitive decline in females, but found no dose–response relationship for physical activity and cognitive decline in males, or for either sex in relation to physical activity and dementia. In their meta-analyses, Sofi et al. 2011 [20] found no evidence of a dose–response relationship between physical activity and cognitive decline. Likewise, Hamer & Chida 2009 [34] did not find a linear-dose response relationship across studies, although such results are not unexpected. A curvilinear relationship, with diminishing returns, seems the most likely association [72]; however, more research in this area is needed. Despite the insufficient evidence for a dose–response relationship, however, there are mechanisms by which the effects of physical activity on cognitive decline and dementia are biologically plausible.

Plausibility

A number of hypotheses have been proposed to account for the likelihood of physical activity impacting on cognitive decline and dementia. One relates to enlargement of the cognitive reserve, which results from increased brain perfusion stimulated by physical activity; another purports vascular origins, whereby cognitive decline and dementia risk are reduced due to the protective effects of exercise on cardiovascular disease, a risk factor for dementia and, more specifically, atherosclerosis; and another suggests exercise limits stress, which in turn diminishes risk for dementia [73]. These hypotheses are not only plausible, but have often proven accurate in studies [73].

Coherence

Results from a myriad of sources, namely animal, brain plasticity, brain-imaging, epidemiological, experimental and neuropathological studies, suggest that the above hypotheses, namely cognitive reserve, vascular origins, and stress, have merit [74]. Studies utilising neuroimaging techniques have, for example, found physically active older persons to have greater brain volume than less active older individuals (2–2.5 per cent increase, per physical activity quintile) [74]. In a study of older rats, van Praag et al. 2005 [75] found that those that exercised on a running wheel showed a reversal in declines of neurogenesis (decline in neurogenesis has been implicated in cognitive decline). The proposed causal relationship between physical activity, cognitive decline and dementia is, therefore, in keeping with what is known from research into cognitive functioning more generally. The similarities observed in the RRs for cognitive decline and dementia, in the current review, offer credence to shared underlying mechanisms (which is not surprising, given that the definition of dementia centres on cognitive decline).

Strengths

Unlike many other studies in the field, the search strategy for the current review was not limited by language barriers, which may have given a more complete picture of the available literature. The search was also independently undertaken by two investigators to ensure thorough and accurate reporting. Evidence included in the review is up to date, and includes the first studies utilising objective measures in this field. Extensive sensitivity analyses were undertaken to adequately address the heterogeneous nature of the studies.

Limitations

A number of limitations of this study are worth noting. The quality assessment tool, and scoring method, may be considered somewhat arbitrary. It is important to note that the same score could be attained by two very different studies each missing different things. However, there was no basis to differentially weigh study quality aspects. Further limitations are in the available evidence base. The studies used a non-standardised set of methods, which makes it difficult to accurately synthesise the findings. The tendency of authors to compare between groups with high and low (or no) physical activity limits the utility of results. This method, of utilising only data from either end of the spectrum, is a less efficient use of the data compared to analysing hours of physical activity per week, for example. Effect sizes were reported in numerous formats; all outputs, however, were given in relative risks (RRs). This is not ideal; however, there was no means of accurately converting adjusted odds ratios (ORs) to RRs, for example. This approach, of taking ORs to equal RRs, and RRs to equal hazard ratios (HRs), is widely used in the literature, e.g. [16, 34].

Conclusions

The current study provides support for the association between physical activity and cognitive decline and dementia. Although methodological limitations of the current evidence base preclude the drawing of definitive conclusions, a case can be made for a causal interpretation of this association. The implications are statistically and clinically significant. It has been estimated, for example, that 3 million AD cases could be averted globally, with a 10–25 per cent shift in modifiable risk factors, including physical activity [76]. Future research should use objective measures of physical activity, adjust for the full range of known, or likely, confounders, and have adequate follow-up length. Ideally, randomised-control trials would be conducted; however, implementing constant stimulation of physical activity over a long duration may prove difficult. In the meantime, given that physical activity has been shown to be beneficial across multiple health domains, genders and ages, physical activity should be encouraged regardless of its relationship with cognitive decline and dementia [73].

Authors’ information

SJB has a BBeSc, GDipIH, and MPH. RHM has a BIntSt, GradDipIntlDev, MA (Research) and MHID (Health and International Development). JLV has a MD, MPH and PhD.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Dr Jan Barendregt for his input regarding the meta-analyses. Dr Mark Griffin for his help relating to the statistical analysis. Thanks also go to Josephine Blondell for editing the manuscript.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made.
The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.
The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (https://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

SJB participated in the design of the study, extracted data, performed the statistical analysis, drafted the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript. RHM participated in the data extraction and read and approved the final manuscript. JLV conceived the study, and participated in its design and coordination, and read and approved the final manuscript.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat World Health Organization: Dementia: A Public Health Priority. 2012, whqlibdoc.who.int/publications/2012/9789241564458_eng.pdf World Health Organization: Dementia: A Public Health Priority. 2012, whqlibdoc.who.int/publications/2012/9789241564458_eng.pdf
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Mortazavi SS, Mohammad K, Ardebili HE, Beni RD, Mahmoodi M, Keshteli AH: Mental disorder prevention and physical activity in Iranian elderly. Int J Prev Med. 2012, 3 (Suppl 1): S64-S72.PubMedPubMedCentral Mortazavi SS, Mohammad K, Ardebili HE, Beni RD, Mahmoodi M, Keshteli AH: Mental disorder prevention and physical activity in Iranian elderly. Int J Prev Med. 2012, 3 (Suppl 1): S64-S72.PubMedPubMedCentral
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Angevaren M, Aufdemkampe G, Verhaar HJJ, Aleman A, Vanhees L: Physical activity and enhanced fitness improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment, a Cochrane Systematic Review. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2008, 14: S44-CrossRef Angevaren M, Aufdemkampe G, Verhaar HJJ, Aleman A, Vanhees L: Physical activity and enhanced fitness improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment, a Cochrane Systematic Review. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2008, 14: S44-CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Miller DI, Taler V, Davidson PSR, Messier C: Measuring the impact of exercise on cognitive aging: methodological issues. Neurobiol Aging. 2012, 33 (3): 622.e629-622.e643.CrossRef Miller DI, Taler V, Davidson PSR, Messier C: Measuring the impact of exercise on cognitive aging: methodological issues. Neurobiol Aging. 2012, 33 (3): 622.e629-622.e643.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Ahlskog JE, Geda YE, Graff-Radford NR, Petersen RC: Physical exercise as a preventive or disease-modifying treatment of dementia and brain aging. Mayo Clin Proc. 2011, 86 (9): 876-884. 10.4065/mcp.2011.0252.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ahlskog JE, Geda YE, Graff-Radford NR, Petersen RC: Physical exercise as a preventive or disease-modifying treatment of dementia and brain aging. Mayo Clin Proc. 2011, 86 (9): 876-884. 10.4065/mcp.2011.0252.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosenberg PB, Johnston D, Lykestos CG: A clinical appraoch to mild cognitive impairment. Am J Psychiatr. 2006, 163 (11): 1884-1890. 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.11.1884.CrossRefPubMed Rosenberg PB, Johnston D, Lykestos CG: A clinical appraoch to mild cognitive impairment. Am J Psychiatr. 2006, 163 (11): 1884-1890. 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.11.1884.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Plassman BL, Williams JW, Burke JR, Holsinger T, Benjamin S: Systematic review: factors associated with risk for and possible prevention of cognitive decline in later life. Ann Intern Med. 2010, 153 (3): 182-193. 10.7326/0003-4819-153-3-201008030-00258.CrossRefPubMed Plassman BL, Williams JW, Burke JR, Holsinger T, Benjamin S: Systematic review: factors associated with risk for and possible prevention of cognitive decline in later life. Ann Intern Med. 2010, 153 (3): 182-193. 10.7326/0003-4819-153-3-201008030-00258.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Ritchie K, Touchon J: Mild cognitive impairment: conceptual basis and current nosological status. Lancet. 2000, 355 (9199): 225-228. 10.1016/S0140-6736(99)06155-3.CrossRefPubMed Ritchie K, Touchon J: Mild cognitive impairment: conceptual basis and current nosological status. Lancet. 2000, 355 (9199): 225-228. 10.1016/S0140-6736(99)06155-3.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Lautenschlager NT, Cox K, Cyarto EV: The influence of exercise on brain aging and dementia. Biochim Biophys Acta (BBA) - Mol Basis Dis. 2012, 1822 (3): 474-481. 10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.07.010.CrossRef Lautenschlager NT, Cox K, Cyarto EV: The influence of exercise on brain aging and dementia. Biochim Biophys Acta (BBA) - Mol Basis Dis. 2012, 1822 (3): 474-481. 10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.07.010.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Chertkow H, Nasreddine Z, Joanette Y, Drolet V, Kirk J, Massoud F, Belleville S, Bergman H: Mild cognitive impairment and cognitive impairment, no dementia: Part A, concept and diagnosis. Alzheimers Dement. 2007, 3 (4): 266-282. 10.1016/j.jalz.2007.07.013.CrossRefPubMed Chertkow H, Nasreddine Z, Joanette Y, Drolet V, Kirk J, Massoud F, Belleville S, Bergman H: Mild cognitive impairment and cognitive impairment, no dementia: Part A, concept and diagnosis. Alzheimers Dement. 2007, 3 (4): 266-282. 10.1016/j.jalz.2007.07.013.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Molinuevo JL, Valls-Pedret C, Rami L: From mild cognitive impairment to prodromal Alzheimer disease: A nosological evolution. Eur Geriatr Med. 2010, 1 (3): 146-154. 10.1016/j.eurger.2010.05.003.CrossRef Molinuevo JL, Valls-Pedret C, Rami L: From mild cognitive impairment to prodromal Alzheimer disease: A nosological evolution. Eur Geriatr Med. 2010, 1 (3): 146-154. 10.1016/j.eurger.2010.05.003.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV-TR®. 2000, Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Pub American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV-TR®. 2000, Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Pub
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Sosa-Ortiz AL, Acosta-Castillo I, Prince MJ: Epidemiology of dementias and Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Medical Research. 2012, 43 (8): 600-608. 10.1016/j.arcmed.2012.11.003.CrossRefPubMed Sosa-Ortiz AL, Acosta-Castillo I, Prince MJ: Epidemiology of dementias and Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Medical Research. 2012, 43 (8): 600-608. 10.1016/j.arcmed.2012.11.003.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Lin F, Friedman E, Quinn J, Chen DG, Mapstone M: Effect of leisure activities on inflammation and cognitive function in an aging sample. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2012, 54 (3): e398-e404. 10.1016/j.archger.2012.02.002.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lin F, Friedman E, Quinn J, Chen DG, Mapstone M: Effect of leisure activities on inflammation and cognitive function in an aging sample. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2012, 54 (3): e398-e404. 10.1016/j.archger.2012.02.002.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Cyarto EV, Lautenschlager NT, Desmond PM, Ames D, Szoeke C, Salvado O, Sharman MJ, Ellis KA, Phal PM, Masters CM, Rowe CC, Martins RN, Cox KL: Protocol for a randomized controlled trial evaluating the effect of physical activity on delaying the progression of white matter changes on MRI in older adults with memory complaints and mild cognitive impairment: The AIBL Active trial. BMC psychiatry. 2012, 12 (1): 167-10.1186/1471-244X-12-167.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Cyarto EV, Lautenschlager NT, Desmond PM, Ames D, Szoeke C, Salvado O, Sharman MJ, Ellis KA, Phal PM, Masters CM, Rowe CC, Martins RN, Cox KL: Protocol for a randomized controlled trial evaluating the effect of physical activity on delaying the progression of white matter changes on MRI in older adults with memory complaints and mild cognitive impairment: The AIBL Active trial. BMC psychiatry. 2012, 12 (1): 167-10.1186/1471-244X-12-167.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Aarsland D, Sardahaee FS, Anderssen S, Ballard C, the Alzheimer’s Society Systematic Review: Is physical activity a potential preventive factor for vascular dementia? A systematic review. Aging Ment Health. 2010, 14 (4): 386-395. 10.1080/13607860903586136.CrossRefPubMed Aarsland D, Sardahaee FS, Anderssen S, Ballard C, the Alzheimer’s Society Systematic Review: Is physical activity a potential preventive factor for vascular dementia? A systematic review. Aging Ment Health. 2010, 14 (4): 386-395. 10.1080/13607860903586136.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Fuchs R: Physical Activity and Health. International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. Edited by: Neil JS, Paul BB. 2001, Oxford: Pergamon, 11411-11415.CrossRef Fuchs R: Physical Activity and Health. International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. Edited by: Neil JS, Paul BB. 2001, Oxford: Pergamon, 11411-11415.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Tseng CN, Gau BS, Lou MF: The effectiveness of exercise on improving cognitive function in older people: a systematic review. J Nurs Res. 2011, 19 (2): 119-131. 10.1097/JNR.0b013e3182198837.CrossRefPubMed Tseng CN, Gau BS, Lou MF: The effectiveness of exercise on improving cognitive function in older people: a systematic review. J Nurs Res. 2011, 19 (2): 119-131. 10.1097/JNR.0b013e3182198837.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Benedict C, Brooks SJ, Kullberg J, Nordenskjold R, Burgos J, Le Greves M, Kilander L, Larsson EM, Johansson L, Ahlstrom H, Lind L, Schioth HB: Association between physical activity and brain health in older adults. Neurobiol Aging. 2013, 34 (1): 83-90. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.04.013.CrossRefPubMed Benedict C, Brooks SJ, Kullberg J, Nordenskjold R, Burgos J, Le Greves M, Kilander L, Larsson EM, Johansson L, Ahlstrom H, Lind L, Schioth HB: Association between physical activity and brain health in older adults. Neurobiol Aging. 2013, 34 (1): 83-90. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.04.013.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Sofi F, Valecchi D, Bacci D, Abbate R, Gensini GF, Casini A, Macchi C: Physical activity and risk of cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. J Intern Med. 2011, 269 (1): 107-117. 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2010.02281.x.CrossRefPubMed Sofi F, Valecchi D, Bacci D, Abbate R, Gensini GF, Casini A, Macchi C: Physical activity and risk of cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. J Intern Med. 2011, 269 (1): 107-117. 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2010.02281.x.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Bowen ME: A prospective examination of the relationship between physical activity and dementia risk in later life. Am J Health Promot. 2012, 26 (6): 333-340. 10.4278/ajhp.110311-QUAN-115.CrossRefPubMed Bowen ME: A prospective examination of the relationship between physical activity and dementia risk in later life. Am J Health Promot. 2012, 26 (6): 333-340. 10.4278/ajhp.110311-QUAN-115.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Ho SC, Woo J, Sham A, Chan SG, Yu AL: A 3-year follow-up study of social, lifestyle and health predictors of cognitive impairment in a Chinese older cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2001, 30 (6): 1389-1396. 10.1093/ije/30.6.1389.CrossRefPubMed Ho SC, Woo J, Sham A, Chan SG, Yu AL: A 3-year follow-up study of social, lifestyle and health predictors of cognitive impairment in a Chinese older cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2001, 30 (6): 1389-1396. 10.1093/ije/30.6.1389.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Laurin D, Verreault R, Lindsay J, MacPherson K, Rockwood K: Physical activity and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in elderly persons. Arch Neurol. 2001, 58 (3): 498-504.CrossRefPubMed Laurin D, Verreault R, Lindsay J, MacPherson K, Rockwood K: Physical activity and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in elderly persons. Arch Neurol. 2001, 58 (3): 498-504.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Schuit AJ, Feskens EJ, Launer LJ, Kromhout D: Physical activity and cognitive decline, the role of the apolipoprotein e4 allele. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2001, 33 (5): 772-777.CrossRefPubMed Schuit AJ, Feskens EJ, Launer LJ, Kromhout D: Physical activity and cognitive decline, the role of the apolipoprotein e4 allele. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2001, 33 (5): 772-777.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Yaffe K, Barnes D, Nevitt M, Lui LY, Covinsky K: A prospective study of physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly women: women who walk. Arch Intern Med. 2001, 161 (14): 1703-1708. 10.1001/archinte.161.14.1703.CrossRefPubMed Yaffe K, Barnes D, Nevitt M, Lui LY, Covinsky K: A prospective study of physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly women: women who walk. Arch Intern Med. 2001, 161 (14): 1703-1708. 10.1001/archinte.161.14.1703.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Pignatti F, Rozzini R, Trabucchi M: Physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly persons. Arch Intern Med. 2002, 162 (3): 361-362. 10.1001/archinte.162.3.361.CrossRefPubMed Pignatti F, Rozzini R, Trabucchi M: Physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly persons. Arch Intern Med. 2002, 162 (3): 361-362. 10.1001/archinte.162.3.361.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Lytle ME, Vander Bilt J, Pandav RS, Dodge HH, Ganguli M: Exercise level and cognitive decline: the MoVIES project. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2004, 18 (2): 57-64. 10.1097/01.wad.0000126614.87955.79.CrossRefPubMed Lytle ME, Vander Bilt J, Pandav RS, Dodge HH, Ganguli M: Exercise level and cognitive decline: the MoVIES project. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2004, 18 (2): 57-64. 10.1097/01.wad.0000126614.87955.79.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Flicker L, Almeida OP, Acres J, Le MT, Tuohy RJ, Jamrozik K, Hankey G, Norman P: Predictors of impaired cognitive function in men over the age of 80 years: results from the Health in Men Study. Age Ageing. 2005, 34 (1): 77-80. 10.1093/ageing/afh235.CrossRefPubMed Flicker L, Almeida OP, Acres J, Le MT, Tuohy RJ, Jamrozik K, Hankey G, Norman P: Predictors of impaired cognitive function in men over the age of 80 years: results from the Health in Men Study. Age Ageing. 2005, 34 (1): 77-80. 10.1093/ageing/afh235.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Singh-Manoux A, Hillsdon M, Brunner E, Marmot M: Effects of physical activity on cognitive functioning in middle age: evidence from the Whitehall II prospective cohort study. Am J Public Health. 2005, 95 (12): 2252-2258. 10.2105/AJPH.2004.055574.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Singh-Manoux A, Hillsdon M, Brunner E, Marmot M: Effects of physical activity on cognitive functioning in middle age: evidence from the Whitehall II prospective cohort study. Am J Public Health. 2005, 95 (12): 2252-2258. 10.2105/AJPH.2004.055574.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Sumic A, Michael YL, Carlson NE, Howieson DB, Kaye JA: Physical activity and the risk of dementia in oldest old. J Aging Health. 2007, 19 (2): 242-259. 10.1177/0898264307299299.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Sumic A, Michael YL, Carlson NE, Howieson DB, Kaye JA: Physical activity and the risk of dementia in oldest old. J Aging Health. 2007, 19 (2): 242-259. 10.1177/0898264307299299.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Middleton L, Kirkland S, Rockwood K: Prevention of CIND by physical activity: different impact on VCI-ND compared with MCI. J Neurol Sci. 2008, 269 (1–2): 80-84.CrossRefPubMed Middleton L, Kirkland S, Rockwood K: Prevention of CIND by physical activity: different impact on VCI-ND compared with MCI. J Neurol Sci. 2008, 269 (1–2): 80-84.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Niti M, Yap KB, Kua EH, Tan CH, Ng TP: Physical, social and productive leisure activities, cognitive decline and interaction with APOE-epsilon 4 genotype in Chinese older adults. Int Psychogeriatr. 2008, 20 (2): 237-251.CrossRefPubMed Niti M, Yap KB, Kua EH, Tan CH, Ng TP: Physical, social and productive leisure activities, cognitive decline and interaction with APOE-epsilon 4 genotype in Chinese older adults. Int Psychogeriatr. 2008, 20 (2): 237-251.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Etgen T, Sander D, Huntgeburth U, Poppert H, Forstl H, Bickel H: Physical activity and incident cognitive impairment in elderly persons: the INVADE study. Arch Intern Med. 2010, 170 (2): 186-193. 10.1001/archinternmed.2009.498.CrossRefPubMed Etgen T, Sander D, Huntgeburth U, Poppert H, Forstl H, Bickel H: Physical activity and incident cognitive impairment in elderly persons: the INVADE study. Arch Intern Med. 2010, 170 (2): 186-193. 10.1001/archinternmed.2009.498.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Hamer M, Chida Y: Physical activity and risk of neurodegenerative disease: a systematic review of prospective evidence. Psychol Med. 2009, 39 (1): 3-11. 10.1017/S0033291708003681.CrossRefPubMed Hamer M, Chida Y: Physical activity and risk of neurodegenerative disease: a systematic review of prospective evidence. Psychol Med. 2009, 39 (1): 3-11. 10.1017/S0033291708003681.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Yoshitake T, Kiyohara Y, Kato I, Ohmura T, Iwamoto H, Nakayama K, Ohmori S, Nomiyama K, Kawano H, Ueda K, Sueishi K, Tsuneyoshi M, Fujishima M: Incidence and risk factors of vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in a defined elderly Japanese population: the Hisayama Study. Neurology. 1995, 45 (6): 1161-1168. 10.1212/WNL.45.6.1161.CrossRefPubMed Yoshitake T, Kiyohara Y, Kato I, Ohmura T, Iwamoto H, Nakayama K, Ohmori S, Nomiyama K, Kawano H, Ueda K, Sueishi K, Tsuneyoshi M, Fujishima M: Incidence and risk factors of vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in a defined elderly Japanese population: the Hisayama Study. Neurology. 1995, 45 (6): 1161-1168. 10.1212/WNL.45.6.1161.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Fabrigoule C, Letenneur L, Dartigues JF, Zarrouk M, Commenges D, Barberger-Gateau P: Social and leisure activities and risk of dementia: a prospective longitudinal study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1995, 43 (5): 485-490.CrossRefPubMed Fabrigoule C, Letenneur L, Dartigues JF, Zarrouk M, Commenges D, Barberger-Gateau P: Social and leisure activities and risk of dementia: a prospective longitudinal study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1995, 43 (5): 485-490.CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Bienias JL, Aggarwal NT, Mendes De Leon CF, Morris MC, Schneider JA, Evans DA: Cognitive activity and incident AD in a population-based sample of older persons. Neurology. 2002, 59 (12): 1910-1914. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000036905.59156.A1.CrossRefPubMed Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Bienias JL, Aggarwal NT, Mendes De Leon CF, Morris MC, Schneider JA, Evans DA: Cognitive activity and incident AD in a population-based sample of older persons. Neurology. 2002, 59 (12): 1910-1914. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000036905.59156.A1.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, Hall CB, Derby CA, Kuslansky G, Ambrose AF, Sliwinski M, Buschke H: Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N Engl J Med. 2003, 348 (25): 2508-2516. 10.1056/NEJMoa022252.CrossRefPubMed Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, Hall CB, Derby CA, Kuslansky G, Ambrose AF, Sliwinski M, Buschke H: Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N Engl J Med. 2003, 348 (25): 2508-2516. 10.1056/NEJMoa022252.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Abbott RD, White LR, Ross GW, Masaki KH, Curb JD, Petrovitch H: Walking and dementia in physically capable elderly men. JAMA. 2004, 292 (12): 1447-1453. 10.1001/jama.292.12.1447.CrossRefPubMed Abbott RD, White LR, Ross GW, Masaki KH, Curb JD, Petrovitch H: Walking and dementia in physically capable elderly men. JAMA. 2004, 292 (12): 1447-1453. 10.1001/jama.292.12.1447.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Podewils LJ, Guallar E, Kuller LH, Fried LP, Lopez OL, Carlson M, Lyketsos CG: Physical activity, APOE genotype, and dementia risk: findings from the Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2005, 161 (7): 639-651. 10.1093/aje/kwi092.CrossRefPubMed Podewils LJ, Guallar E, Kuller LH, Fried LP, Lopez OL, Carlson M, Lyketsos CG: Physical activity, APOE genotype, and dementia risk: findings from the Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2005, 161 (7): 639-651. 10.1093/aje/kwi092.CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Rovio S, Kareholt I, Helkala E, Viitanen M, Winblad B, Tuomilehto J, Soininen H, Nissinen A, Kivipelto M: Leisure-time physical activity at midlife and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2005, 4: 705-711. 10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70198-8.CrossRefPubMed Rovio S, Kareholt I, Helkala E, Viitanen M, Winblad B, Tuomilehto J, Soininen H, Nissinen A, Kivipelto M: Leisure-time physical activity at midlife and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2005, 4: 705-711. 10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70198-8.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Larson EB, Wang L, Bowen J, McCormick W, Teri L, Crane P, Kukull W: Exercise Is Associated with Reduced Risk for Incident Dementia among Persons 65 Years of Age and Older. Ann Intern Med. 2006, 144: 73-81. 10.7326/0003-4819-144-2-200601170-00004.CrossRefPubMed Larson EB, Wang L, Bowen J, McCormick W, Teri L, Crane P, Kukull W: Exercise Is Associated with Reduced Risk for Incident Dementia among Persons 65 Years of Age and Older. Ann Intern Med. 2006, 144: 73-81. 10.7326/0003-4819-144-2-200601170-00004.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Doi SA, Thalib L: A quality-effects model for meta-analysis. Epidemiology. 2008, 19 (1): 94-100. 10.1097/EDE.0b013e31815c24e7.CrossRefPubMed Doi SA, Thalib L: A quality-effects model for meta-analysis. Epidemiology. 2008, 19 (1): 94-100. 10.1097/EDE.0b013e31815c24e7.CrossRefPubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG: Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Ann Intern Med. 2009, 151 (4): 264-269. 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135.CrossRefPubMed Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG: Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Ann Intern Med. 2009, 151 (4): 264-269. 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Singh A, Uijtdewilligen L, Twisk JW, Van Mechelen W, Chinapaw MJ: Physical activity and performance at school: a systematic review of the literature including a methodological quality assessment. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012, 166 (1): 49-55. 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.716.CrossRefPubMed Singh A, Uijtdewilligen L, Twisk JW, Van Mechelen W, Chinapaw MJ: Physical activity and performance at school: a systematic review of the literature including a methodological quality assessment. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012, 166 (1): 49-55. 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.716.CrossRefPubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Livingston JD, Boyd JE: Correlates and consequences of internalized stigma for people living with mental illness: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Soc Sci Med. 2010, 71 (12): 2150-2161. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.09.030.CrossRefPubMed Livingston JD, Boyd JE: Correlates and consequences of internalized stigma for people living with mental illness: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Soc Sci Med. 2010, 71 (12): 2150-2161. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.09.030.CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Iwasa H, Yoshida Y, Kai I, Suzuki T, Kim H, Yoshida H: Leisure activities and cognitive function in elderly community-dwelling individuals in Japan: A 5-year prospective cohort study. J Psychosom Res. 2012, 72 (2): 159-164. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2011.10.002.CrossRefPubMed Iwasa H, Yoshida Y, Kai I, Suzuki T, Kim H, Yoshida H: Leisure activities and cognitive function in elderly community-dwelling individuals in Japan: A 5-year prospective cohort study. J Psychosom Res. 2012, 72 (2): 159-164. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2011.10.002.CrossRefPubMed
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Lee S, Yuki A, Nishita Y, Tange C, Kim H, Kozakai R, Ando F, Shimokata H: Research relationship between light-intensity physical activity and cognitive function in a community-dwelling elderly population-an 8-year longitudinal study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2013, 61 (3): 452-453. 10.1111/jgs.12119.CrossRefPubMed Lee S, Yuki A, Nishita Y, Tange C, Kim H, Kozakai R, Ando F, Shimokata H: Research relationship between light-intensity physical activity and cognitive function in a community-dwelling elderly population-an 8-year longitudinal study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2013, 61 (3): 452-453. 10.1111/jgs.12119.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Middleton LE, Manini TM, Simonsick EM, Harris TB, Barnes DE, Tylavsky F, Brach JS, Everhart JE, Yaffe K: Activity energy expenditure and incident cognitive impairment in older adults. Arch Intern Med. 2011, 171 (14): 1251-1257. 10.1001/archinternmed.2011.277.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Middleton LE, Manini TM, Simonsick EM, Harris TB, Barnes DE, Tylavsky F, Brach JS, Everhart JE, Yaffe K: Activity energy expenditure and incident cognitive impairment in older adults. Arch Intern Med. 2011, 171 (14): 1251-1257. 10.1001/archinternmed.2011.277.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Morgan GS, Gallacher J, Bayer A, Fish M, Ebrahim S, Ben-Shlomo Y: Physical activity in middle-age and dementia in later life: findings from a prospective cohort of men in Caerphilly, South Wales and a meta-analysis. J Alzheimers Dis. 2012, 31 (3): 569-580.PubMed Morgan GS, Gallacher J, Bayer A, Fish M, Ebrahim S, Ben-Shlomo Y: Physical activity in middle-age and dementia in later life: findings from a prospective cohort of men in Caerphilly, South Wales and a meta-analysis. J Alzheimers Dis. 2012, 31 (3): 569-580.PubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Verghese J, Cuiling W, Katz MJ, Sanders A, Lipton RB: Leisure activities and risk of vascular cognitive impairment in older adults. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2009, 22 (2): 110-118. 10.1177/0891988709332938.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Verghese J, Cuiling W, Katz MJ, Sanders A, Lipton RB: Leisure activities and risk of vascular cognitive impairment in older adults. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2009, 22 (2): 110-118. 10.1177/0891988709332938.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Buchman AS, Boyle PA, Yu L, Shah RC, Wilson RS, Bennett DA: Total daily physical activity and the risk of AD and cognitive decline in older adults. Neurology. 2012, 78 (17): 1323-1329. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182535d35.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Buchman AS, Boyle PA, Yu L, Shah RC, Wilson RS, Bennett DA: Total daily physical activity and the risk of AD and cognitive decline in older adults. Neurology. 2012, 78 (17): 1323-1329. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182535d35.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Chang M, Jonsson PV, Snaedal J, Bjornsson S, Saczynski JS, Aspelund T, Eiriksdottir G, Jonsdottir MK, Lopez OL, Harris TB, Gudnason V, Launer LJ: The effect of midlife physical activity on cognitive function among older adults: AGES–Reykjavik Study. J Gerontol A: Biol Med Sci. 2010, 65 (12): 1369-1374.CrossRef Chang M, Jonsson PV, Snaedal J, Bjornsson S, Saczynski JS, Aspelund T, Eiriksdottir G, Jonsdottir MK, Lopez OL, Harris TB, Gudnason V, Launer LJ: The effect of midlife physical activity on cognitive function among older adults: AGES–Reykjavik Study. J Gerontol A: Biol Med Sci. 2010, 65 (12): 1369-1374.CrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat De Bruijn RF, Schrijvers EM, De Groot KA, Witteman JC, Hofman A, Franco OH, Koudstaal PJ, Ikram MA: The association between physical activity and dementia in an elderly population: the Rotterdam Study. Eur J Epidemiol. 2013, 28 (3): 277-283. 10.1007/s10654-013-9773-3.CrossRefPubMed De Bruijn RF, Schrijvers EM, De Groot KA, Witteman JC, Hofman A, Franco OH, Koudstaal PJ, Ikram MA: The association between physical activity and dementia in an elderly population: the Rotterdam Study. Eur J Epidemiol. 2013, 28 (3): 277-283. 10.1007/s10654-013-9773-3.CrossRefPubMed
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Gureje O, Ogunniyi A, Kola L, Abiona T: Incidence of and risk factors for dementia in the Ibadan Study of Aging. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011, 59 (5): 869-874. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03374.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Gureje O, Ogunniyi A, Kola L, Abiona T: Incidence of and risk factors for dementia in the Ibadan Study of Aging. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011, 59 (5): 869-874. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03374.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim JM, Stewart R, Bae KY, Kim SW, Yang SJ, Park KH, Shin IS, Yoon JS: Role of BDNF val66met polymorphism on the association between physical activity and incident dementia. Neurobiol Aging. 2011, 32 (3): 551 e555-512.CrossRef Kim JM, Stewart R, Bae KY, Kim SW, Yang SJ, Park KH, Shin IS, Yoon JS: Role of BDNF val66met polymorphism on the association between physical activity and incident dementia. Neurobiol Aging. 2011, 32 (3): 551 e555-512.CrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat McCallum J, Simons LA, Simons J, Friedlander Y: Delaying dementia and nursing home placement: the Dubbo study of elderly Australians over a 14-year follow-up. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007, 1114: 121-129. 10.1196/annals.1396.049.CrossRefPubMed McCallum J, Simons LA, Simons J, Friedlander Y: Delaying dementia and nursing home placement: the Dubbo study of elderly Australians over a 14-year follow-up. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007, 1114: 121-129. 10.1196/annals.1396.049.CrossRefPubMed
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Ravaglia G, Forti P, Lucicesare A, Pisacane N, Rietti E, Bianchin M, Dalmonte E: Physical activity and dementia risk in the elderly: findings from a prospective Italian study. Neurology. 2008, 70 (19 Pt 2): 1786-1794.CrossRefPubMed Ravaglia G, Forti P, Lucicesare A, Pisacane N, Rietti E, Bianchin M, Dalmonte E: Physical activity and dementia risk in the elderly: findings from a prospective Italian study. Neurology. 2008, 70 (19 Pt 2): 1786-1794.CrossRefPubMed
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Rovio S, Kareholt I, Viitanen M, Winblad B, Tuomilehto J, Soininen H, Nissinen A, Kivipelto M: Work-related physical activity and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007, 22 (9): 874-882. 10.1002/gps.1755.CrossRefPubMed Rovio S, Kareholt I, Viitanen M, Winblad B, Tuomilehto J, Soininen H, Nissinen A, Kivipelto M: Work-related physical activity and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007, 22 (9): 874-882. 10.1002/gps.1755.CrossRefPubMed
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Scarmeas N, Luchsinger JA, Schupf N, Brickman AM, Cosentino S, Tang MX, Stern Y: Physical activity, diet, and risk of Alzheimer disease. JAMA. 2009, 302 (6): 627-637. 10.1001/jama.2009.1144.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Scarmeas N, Luchsinger JA, Schupf N, Brickman AM, Cosentino S, Tang MX, Stern Y: Physical activity, diet, and risk of Alzheimer disease. JAMA. 2009, 302 (6): 627-637. 10.1001/jama.2009.1144.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Taaffe DR, Irie F, Masaki KH, Abbott RD, Petrovitch H, Ross GW, White LR: Physical activity, physical function, and incident dementia in elderly men: the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. J Gerontol A: Biol Med Sci. 2008, 63 (5): 529-535. 10.1093/gerona/63.5.529.CrossRef Taaffe DR, Irie F, Masaki KH, Abbott RD, Petrovitch H, Ross GW, White LR: Physical activity, physical function, and incident dementia in elderly men: the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. J Gerontol A: Biol Med Sci. 2008, 63 (5): 529-535. 10.1093/gerona/63.5.529.CrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Sattler C, Erikson KI, Torro P, Shroder J: Physical fitness as a protective factor for cognitive impairment in a prospective population-based study in Germany. J Alzheimers Dis. 2011, 26: 709-718.PubMed Sattler C, Erikson KI, Torro P, Shroder J: Physical fitness as a protective factor for cognitive impairment in a prospective population-based study in Germany. J Alzheimers Dis. 2011, 26: 709-718.PubMed
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Ward AC: The role of causal criteria in causal inferences: Bradford Hill’s “aspects of association”. Epidemiol Perspect Innov. 2009, 6: 2-10.1186/1742-5573-6-2.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ward AC: The role of causal criteria in causal inferences: Bradford Hill’s “aspects of association”. Epidemiol Perspect Innov. 2009, 6: 2-10.1186/1742-5573-6-2.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Rothman KJ, Greenland S, Lash TL: Modern Epidemiology. 2008, Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Rothman KJ, Greenland S, Lash TL: Modern Epidemiology. 2008, Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Hofler M: The Bradford Hill considerations on causality: a counterfactual perspective. Emerg Themes Epidemiol. 2005, 2: 11-10.1186/1742-7622-2-11.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hofler M: The Bradford Hill considerations on causality: a counterfactual perspective. Emerg Themes Epidemiol. 2005, 2: 11-10.1186/1742-7622-2-11.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Webb P, Bain C: Essential Epidemiology: An Introduction for Students and Health Professionals. 2010, Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRef Webb P, Bain C: Essential Epidemiology: An Introduction for Students and Health Professionals. 2010, Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Jacova C, Kertesz A, Blair M, Fisk JD, Feldman HH: Neuropsychological testing and assessment for dementia. Alzheimers Dement. 2007, 3 (4): 299-317. 10.1016/j.jalz.2007.07.011.CrossRefPubMed Jacova C, Kertesz A, Blair M, Fisk JD, Feldman HH: Neuropsychological testing and assessment for dementia. Alzheimers Dement. 2007, 3 (4): 299-317. 10.1016/j.jalz.2007.07.011.CrossRefPubMed
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Tombaugh TN: Test-retest reliable coefficients and 5-year change scores for the MMSE and 3MS. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2005, 20 (4): 485-503. 10.1016/j.acn.2004.11.004.CrossRefPubMed Tombaugh TN: Test-retest reliable coefficients and 5-year change scores for the MMSE and 3MS. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2005, 20 (4): 485-503. 10.1016/j.acn.2004.11.004.CrossRefPubMed
69.
Zurück zum Zitat McDowell I, Kristjansson B, Hill GB, Hébert R: Community screening for dementia: The Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE) and modified Mini-Mental State Exam (3MS) compared. J Clin Epidemiol. 1997, 50 (4): 377-383. 10.1016/S0895-4356(97)00060-7.CrossRefPubMed McDowell I, Kristjansson B, Hill GB, Hébert R: Community screening for dementia: The Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE) and modified Mini-Mental State Exam (3MS) compared. J Clin Epidemiol. 1997, 50 (4): 377-383. 10.1016/S0895-4356(97)00060-7.CrossRefPubMed
70.
Zurück zum Zitat MacMahon S, Peto R, Cutler J, Collins R, Sorlie P, Neaton J, Abbott R, Godwin J, Dyer A, Stamler J: Blood pressure, stroke, and coronary heart disease. Part 1, Prolonged differences in blood pressure: prospective observational studies corrected for the regression dilution bias. Lancet. 1990, 335 (8692): 765-774. 10.1016/0140-6736(90)90878-9.CrossRefPubMed MacMahon S, Peto R, Cutler J, Collins R, Sorlie P, Neaton J, Abbott R, Godwin J, Dyer A, Stamler J: Blood pressure, stroke, and coronary heart disease. Part 1, Prolonged differences in blood pressure: prospective observational studies corrected for the regression dilution bias. Lancet. 1990, 335 (8692): 765-774. 10.1016/0140-6736(90)90878-9.CrossRefPubMed
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Woodard JL, Nielson KA, Sugarman MA, Smith JC, Seidenberg M, Durgerian S, Butts A, Hantke N, Lancaster M, Matthews MA: Lifestyle and genetic contributions to cognitive decline and hippocampal integrity in healthy aging. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2012, 9 (4): 436-10.2174/156720512800492477.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Woodard JL, Nielson KA, Sugarman MA, Smith JC, Seidenberg M, Durgerian S, Butts A, Hantke N, Lancaster M, Matthews MA: Lifestyle and genetic contributions to cognitive decline and hippocampal integrity in healthy aging. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2012, 9 (4): 436-10.2174/156720512800492477.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Pate RR, Pratt M, Blair SN, Haskell WL, Macera CA, Bouchard C, Buchner D, Ettinger W, Heath GW, King AC, Kriska A, Leon AS, Marcus BH, Morris J, Paffenbarger RS, Patrick K, Pollock ML, Rippe JM, Sallis J, Wilmore JH: Physical activity and public health. A recommendation from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and the American College of Sports Medicine. JAMA. 1995, 273 (5): 402-407. 10.1001/jama.1995.03520290054029.CrossRefPubMed Pate RR, Pratt M, Blair SN, Haskell WL, Macera CA, Bouchard C, Buchner D, Ettinger W, Heath GW, King AC, Kriska A, Leon AS, Marcus BH, Morris J, Paffenbarger RS, Patrick K, Pollock ML, Rippe JM, Sallis J, Wilmore JH: Physical activity and public health. A recommendation from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and the American College of Sports Medicine. JAMA. 1995, 273 (5): 402-407. 10.1001/jama.1995.03520290054029.CrossRefPubMed
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Fratiglioni L, Paillard-Borg S, Winblad B: An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2004, 3 (6): 343-353. 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00767-7.CrossRefPubMed Fratiglioni L, Paillard-Borg S, Winblad B: An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2004, 3 (6): 343-353. 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00767-7.CrossRefPubMed
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Ho AJ, Raji CA, Becker JT, Lopez OL, Kuller LH, Hua X, Dinov ID, Stein JL, Rosano C, Toga AW, Thompson PM: The effects of physical activity, education, and body mass index on the aging brain. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011, 32 (9): 1371-1382. 10.1002/hbm.21113.CrossRefPubMed Ho AJ, Raji CA, Becker JT, Lopez OL, Kuller LH, Hua X, Dinov ID, Stein JL, Rosano C, Toga AW, Thompson PM: The effects of physical activity, education, and body mass index on the aging brain. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011, 32 (9): 1371-1382. 10.1002/hbm.21113.CrossRefPubMed
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Praag H, Shubert T, Zhao C, Gage FH: Exercise enhances learning and hippocampal neurogenesis in aged mice. J Neurosci. 2005, 25 (38): 8680-8685. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1731-05.2005.CrossRefPubMed Van Praag H, Shubert T, Zhao C, Gage FH: Exercise enhances learning and hippocampal neurogenesis in aged mice. J Neurosci. 2005, 25 (38): 8680-8685. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1731-05.2005.CrossRefPubMed
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Erickson KI, Weinstein AM, Lopez OL: Physical activity, brain plasticity, and Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Med Res. 2012, 43 (8): 615-621. 10.1016/j.arcmed.2012.09.008.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Erickson KI, Weinstein AM, Lopez OL: Physical activity, brain plasticity, and Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Med Res. 2012, 43 (8): 615-621. 10.1016/j.arcmed.2012.09.008.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
Does physical activity prevent cognitive decline and dementia?: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies
verfasst von
Sarah J Blondell
Rachel Hammersley-Mather
J Lennert Veerman
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2014
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Public Health / Ausgabe 1/2014
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2458
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-14-510

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2014

BMC Public Health 1/2014 Zur Ausgabe