Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BioPsychoSocial Medicine 1/2016

Open Access 01.12.2016 | Review

Links among emotional awareness, somatic awareness and autonomic homeostatic processing

Erschienen in: BioPsychoSocial Medicine | Ausgabe 1/2016

Abstract

Emotional awareness and somatic interoceptive awareness are essential processes for human psychosomatic health. A typical trait of lacking emotional awareness related to psychosomatic symptoms is alexithymia. In contrast, alexisomia refers to the trait of lacking somatic awareness. Links between emotional and somatic awareness and homeostatic processing are also significant for the psychosomatic health. The purpose of the present paper is to review the links among emotional awareness, somatic interoceptive awareness and autonomic homeostatic processing. On the basis of the collected evidence, the following arguments were presented1: (1) The main subcortical neural substrates for these processes are limbic-related systems, which are also responsible for autonomic functions for optimization of homeostatic efficiency. (2) Considerable studies have shown that autonomic activity and/or reactivity to stress correlate with both emotional and interoceptive awareness. A hypothesis was advocated about the links between the two types of awareness and autonomic function: Autonomic dysfunction, especially high sympathetic tone at baseline and/or attenuated reactivity or variability to stress, appears to be involved in disturbance of emotional and interoceptive awareness. (3) Several studies suggest that a link or a cooperative relationship exists between emotional and somatic awareness, and that somatic awareness is the more fundamental of the two types of awareness. Emotional awareness, somatic awareness and autonomic homeostatic processing generally occur in parallel or concurrently. However, some complex features of pathologies include coexistence of reduced interoceptive awareness and somatosensory amplification. The autonomic homeostatic process is fundamentally involved in emotional and somatic awareness. Investigation of these types of awareness with both neuroimaging evaluations and estimation of peripheral autonomic function are required as next steps for exploration of the relationship between awareness and human somatic states including somatic symptoms as well as general psychosomatic health.
Hinweise

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

KK and MF collected the research and edited the paper. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Abkürzungen
FSS
functional somatic syndrome
ANS
autonomic nervous system
HPA (axis)
hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (axis)
(d) ACC
(dorsal) anterior cingulate cortex
TAS-20
20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale
EEG
electroencephalogram

Background

Emotional awareness and somatic awareness are essential processes for human psychosomatic health, because disturbance of these types of awareness leads to unhealthy conditions through obstruction of homeostatic processing. Emotional/somatic awareness is the state in which individuals have access to their own emotional/somatic condition [1]. A typical trait of lacking emotional awareness related to psychosomatic symptoms is called alexithymia. In contrast, somatic awareness is physiologically based on interoception, which is defined as the homeostatic afferent neural system that represents the physiological condition of the body in humans [24]. Alexisomia is a term that, in contrast with alexithymia, refers to a trait of lacking somatic awareness [5]. The mechanisms that link these traits to unhealthy conditions include implicit emotional processing [68], disconnection between neocortical and subcortical systems [9, 10], and homeostatic inadequacy by blunt interoception [11, 12] as will be described later. In the present paper, we focus on the relationships among emotional awareness, somatic awareness, and autonomic homeostatic processing.
Patients with psychosomatic disorders often present with difficulties in awareness and expression of their emotions, or alexithymia trait [13]. The alexithymia construct was conceptualized by Nemiah, Freyberger, and Sifneos [14, 15] as a trait characterized by difficulty in identifying feelings, difficulty in describing feelings, externally oriented thinking, and a limited capacity of imagination. The alexithymia trait is involved in generation and expansion of somatic symptoms and related not only to psychosomatic disorders but also to several physical illnesses [16, 17], functional somatic disorders such as functional gastrointestinal disorders [18] or so-called “medically unexplained symptoms” [19], chronic pain [20, 21] and certain types of illness behavior [22]. Better understanding of alexithymic features for patients with psychosomatic or functional somatic disorders, in both general medical settings and specialized clinical settings, is required.
Links between the emotional process and the homeostatic physiological process are important for understanding the relationship of alexithymia to somatic symptoms or disorders. Lane conceptualized the explicit and implicit emotional processes and demonstrated that alexithymia is one of the constructs related to implicit emotional process [68]. Explicit emotional processing in this context means that negative emotional states such as depression, anxiety and hostility are associated with the unhealthy state or disease, while implicit emotional processing means interruption in the awareness and expression of negative emotion, which leads to unhealthy physiological conditions, typically psychosomatic disorders.
They also advocated the level of emotional awareness model [6], in which greater awareness corresponds to differentiated emotion and less awareness corresponds to undifferentiated emotion or somatic sensation. According to the concept and the model, verbal expressions are accessible for well-differentiated emotions, while undifferentiated emotions tend to lead to physiological dysfunctions or somatic symptoms through implicit emotional processing.
MacLean hypothesized for the first time that interference in connections between the limbic system and the neocortical system is the basic source of psychosomatic problems [23, 24]. Emotional awareness, in this context, is an emotional processing into awareness enabled by connection between the neocortical and subcortical systems. Alexithymia is therefore assumed to be one of the disconnection syndromes [7, 9, 10]. Disconnection between the higher-level neocortical emotional processes and subcortical emotion-generating processes contributes to dysfunctions in the autonomic nervous system (ANS) and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis [23], and hence leads to homeostatic dysregulation and finally to disease.

Neural substrates for emotional/somatic awareness and autonomic homeostatic processing

Neuroimaging approaches have demonstrated several neural substrates for the implicit emotional and somatic processes and their links to homeostatic regulation. The amygdala is a core limbic structure, and many studies have demonstrated its functions in emotional processing [2528] and emotion-related memory [29, 30]. The cingulate cortex is a paralimbic structure, and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) was initially thought to have general affective and emotional functions [31, 32]. It is now recognized to have two subdivisions: the dorsal and ventral portions of the ACC. Although the dorsal ACC (dACC) has generally been thought to have primarily cognitive functions [33, 34], the dACC actually plays considerable roles in emotional processing [35, 36]. Moreover, several bodies of evidence using emotional awareness scales [37] for example suggest the dACC is involved in emotional awareness or expression of emotion [38, 39]. Whereas, the ventral ACC mainly connects to the amygdala, hypothalamus and insula [33, 40], it has outflow to autonomic nervous and endocrine systems, and plays a role in regulation of emotional responses with respect to limbic systems [39, 41, 42]. Overall, the amygdala is associated primarily with unconscious or implicit emotional processing, while the cingulate cortex is associated predominantly with conscious processing of emotion [43, 44].
On the other hand, the insula is a cortical area closely connected with the limbic system, which receives somatosensory signals [45]. The insula is particularly involved in interoception or interoceptive awareness [2, 3, 46]. Interoceptive stimuli include thirst, dyspnea, sensual touch, coolness, warmth, heartbeat, and so on [4]. Interoceptive representations in neural substrates are primarily via the anterior insula [4, 46], which has a fundamental role in all subjective feelings from the body [4], while the ACC is responsible for autonomic alteration along with interoception [47]. The basic purpose of interoceptive awareness is to optimize homeostatic efficiency [11]. Disturbed interoceptive awareness therefore could lead to an unhealthy condition through homeostatic insufficiency. The relationship between interoceptive and emotional awareness has been studied and several lines of evidence to be described later have suggested a cooperative relationship between the two.
Most of these neural substrates for implicit emotional processing and interoceptive awareness are also known to be responsible for the ANS and HPA axis functions, which are major homeostatic regulatory systems or allostatic systems [48, 49]. Allostasis is the process of achieving stability through physiological or behavioral change; the set point of the stability is not static but dynamic in allostatic systems [48]. Alteration of the set point could lead to disease; this constitutes one explanation for the psychosomatic process. Hence, the concept is important for psychosomatic health.
Damasio advocated a neural model, in which the neural distinctions among primary emotion, secondary emotion, and feeling are demonstrated [50]. Primary emotions are the result of an implicit subconscious emotional process that occurs as a first response to a situation, and are closely related to adaptive and survival processes; in contrast, secondary emotions are the outcome of a process that occurs after the primary emotions, and represent social and higher-level processes with the experience of consciously processed emotion. The primary emotional response is considered the phylogenetically older behavioral and a physiological expression of the emotional response [7]. Meanwhile, interoception is a homeostatic afferent pathway and sensory aspects of homeostasis that represent the physiological condition of the body [2]. Thus, the implicit emotional and interoceptive processes have a basal relationship with the homeostatic process through ANS and HPA axis.
Considering these theories and arguments together, both emotional awareness and somatic interoceptive awareness are essential processes for human psychosomatic health. The homeostatic physiological process—that is, autonomic and HPA functions, or its response to environment or stress—is intimately linked to emotional and interoceptive awareness; therefore, this process is one of the most important clues to investigate both types of awareness. The ANS is key for achieving the appropriate modification of physiological parameters, although the hormonal system is activated simultaneously [51]. Additionally, the autonomic indexes are easy to measure and easy to apply in psychophysiological approaches such as biofeedback to psychosomatic patients. We have therefore directed attention to autonomic response to stress as a homeostatic marker. Here, the purpose of the present paper is to review evidence of the links among emotional awareness, somatic interoceptive awareness and autonomic homeostatic processing.

Emotional awareness and autonomic homeostatic processing

Many studies have addressed autonomic characteristics in response to stress tasks or various stimuli in alexithymic individuals. The findings vary by experimental condition and have some inconsistencies [52]. Some studies indicate a hyperarousal model of alexithymia, in which higher autonomic responses are observed in alexithymic individuals [53, 54], whereas considerable studies support a hypoarousal model of alexithymia, in which attenuated autonomic reactivity inhibits the perception of emotional signals [5557]. These inconsistencies are probably due in part to differences in stimuli (e.g., tasks involving mental arithmetic, affective picture viewing, or social speech stress) and differences in physiological measurements (e.g., heart rate or other cardiovascular marker, electrodermal activity or skin conductance level, or muscle tension).
Although results have diverged in some areas, two findings are mostly consistent. (1) Autonomic tone at baseline, as measured by electrodermal activities such as skin conductance, is higher in alexithymic than in non-alexithymic individuals, whereas responses to stress (measured as change scores from baseline) are relatively low [53, 54, 5860]. (2) In contrast, baseline autonomic states, measured by heart rates, are unrelated to alexithymic levels, while the responses to stress differ according to stimulus types [58, 59, 61, 62].
Electrodermal activities are known to be an index of autonomic tone related to emotional arousal that is mainly controlled by the sympathetic system [63], while cardiac activities are regulated by both sympathetic and vagal functions. Electrodermal activities are also known to follow the “law of initial value,” which means that high baseline levels limit the amount of change that can be produced by stimuli [64]. Another study using cortisol levels suggested increased basal HPA activity in alexithymic subjects [65]. The HPA axis and sympathetic-adrenal-medullary systems are major stress-responsive systems that work in alliance and react concurrently in most cases with stressful situations [66]. On the basis of these reviews, it is plausible that heightened resting sympathetic tone and/or attenuated reactivity to stress, or variability, are involved in the disturbance of proper perception of emotional inputs in a majority of alexithymic individuals (Fig. 1).
Additionally, several studies [65, 67] have reported varying autonomic or HPA reactivity results with different subscales of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) [68, 69]. For example, Pollatos et al. demonstrated that only the alexithymia facet “difficulty in describing feelings” was associated with smaller electrodermal responses [67]. Bermond and Vorst presented evidence that the alexithymia trait has both affective and cognitive dimensions that are orthogonal to one another, and proposed different physiological features in these dimensional categories [70, 71]. Therefore, such heterogeneity of alexithymia construct should also be considered as a source of the variety in outcomes of the studies described earlier.
Our previous studies in patients with psychosomatic or functional somatic symptoms, who generally have been thought to have difficulties in emotional and/or somatic awareness, showed overall hyporeactivity to a mental arithmetic stress task in several physiological measurements including skin conductance level, skin temperature, and blood volume pulse amplitude [72, 73]. A subsequent study with a larger sample identified two clusters of psychophysiological stress response patterns: a majority cluster whose members have low stress response patterns and another cluster with high or even response patterns [74]. Another study suggested that functional somatic syndrome patients with difficulty in identifying feelings had higher sympathetic tone at pre-stress baseline, measured by salivary amylase [75, 76]. Moreover, the alexithymic trait and resting sympathetic tone correlated positively in healthy controls [75].
Considering these findings and aforementioned reviews together, several dysfunctional states in ANS are involved in the disturbance of emotional awareness; especially, high sympathetic tone at baseline and/or impaired autonomic variability affect emotional awareness in a number of alexithymic individuals (Fig. 1). Whereas, the alexithymia construct is heterogeneous, and one or more other mechanisms could also affect emotional awareness in other types of alexithymia.

Emotional awareness and feedback model through vagal function

Several studies have indicated that the function of the medial prefrontal cortex, which has a strong connection to limbic structures such as the amygdala, is correlated positively with autonomic vagal activity measured by the vagal component of heart rate variability [7780]. The ACC, which is anatomically adjacent to the medial prefrontal cortex, has also been shown to have a positive correlation with vagal function [81].
The medial prefrontal cortex and the ACC have a role of conscious processing of emotion as described previously [7, 8]. Conscious processing or conscious awareness of emotion is generally accepted clinically as an important process for emotional self-regulation [82]. Lane et al. argued that conscious processing of emotion requires the transmission of subcortical affective information to the cerebral cortex − medial prefrontal cortex and ACC in this context [80]. The processing also requires top-down feedback from cortical to subcortical function [43, 80]. They considered the positive correlation of the cortical activity with vagal function described previously as evidences of top-down feedback [7, 80].
Based on the existing evidence and its interpretations, the conscious processing of emotion is assumed to have a negative feedback loop through vagal function [6, 83]; that is, when emotional experience is consciously processed into awareness, vagal tone is accelerated, after which emotional arousal is regulated [7]. In other words, the medial prefrontal cortex and ACC have a tonic inhibitory effect through vagal activity when these areas are activated in connection with conscious emotional experience. Vagal tone has been considered to have a function of regulating emotional responses [84], or selecting an optimal response and inhibiting less optimal ones [80] (Fig. 2).
Research suggests that the neural pathway for the feedback process involves cortical areas, from the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and especially the medial prefrontal cortex; through limbic system structures such as the cingulate cortex (especially the ACC), amygdala and hypothalamus; to the ANS [7, 47, 83, 85] (Fig. 2). The insula is also involved in this pathway in addition to being a representative center for interoception as described before. The anterior insula and ACC have a close functional relationship and the two areas conjointly bear an important role in emotional processing [86].
If conscious processing is disturbed, vagal tone would not be accelerated, and the resting sympathetic tone would be easily activated. This negative feedback model suggests a mechanism of the relationship between the autonomic function and emotional awareness, and supports the hypothesis shown in Fig. 1.

Interoceptive awareness and autonomic homeostatic processing

Autonomic activity or reactivity to stress is involved in interoceptive awareness as well as emotional awareness. Some studies have suggested that individual difference in sympathetic cardiac activity affect interoceptive sensitivity to heartbeats [87, 88]. Several subsequent studies with heartbeat perception tasks [89, 90] have indicated that cardiac interoceptive awareness is affected by autonomic reactivity to some tasks [9193]. These studies suggested that interoceptive awareness is positively related to the autonomic response to such tasks.
Herbert in particular established that cardiac awareness was associated with greater sympathetic reactivity during mental stress and greater vagal reactivity during emotional picture viewing [93]. Both sympathetic and vagal activities could contribute to cardiac awareness [93]. Autonomic function or reactivity therefore affects not only emotional awareness but also somatic interoceptive awareness.

Interoceptive awareness and emotional awareness

James first described the idea that emotional experience necessarily included perception of a somatic response [94, 95], and Lange proposed a similar concept [96]. Since then, historical debates on the relationship between emotional experience and somatic change have continued and have influenced theories of interaction between emotions and the body.
Considerable studies with a heartbeat perception task as an interoceptive index have demonstrated that subjective emotional experience relates positively to interoceptive sensitivity [46, 97, 98]. Consistent with these findings, evidence exists that alexithymic individuals have less interoceptive awareness [99]. Several studies using electroencephalogram (EEG) recordings also suggest interoception correlates positively with emotion processing, measured as P300 event-related potentials, for example [100102].
Studies using heartbeat-evoked brain potential, which is observed by EEG promptly after the R-wave of the electrocardiogram over the somatosensory cortex and the frontal/prefrontal cortex, have also examined cortical processing of signals from cardiovascular activities [103, 104]. The heartbeat-evoked potential could be a plausible neurophysiological marker of cardiac interoceptive awareness [105] and has a potential significance for exploration of the relationship between cardiac interoception and cortical awareness processes. Heartbeat-evoked potentials have been linked to empathy [106], suggesting a correlation between cardiac awareness and emotional processing.
The anterior insula, a core substrate for interoception, is also activated by emotional processing [107, 108]. Neuroimaging studies suggested that the common activated areas for interoceptive awareness and emotional experience were the right anterior insular cortex and ventromedial prefrontal cortex [109, 110]. These findings suggest the close neurophysiological correlation of emotional and somatic interoceptive awareness. Additionally, difficulties in emotional awareness, i.e., alexithymia, are in most cases clinically associated with difficulties in somatic awareness, or alexisomia [5].
In contrast, several studies have suggested activation of some somatosensory systems in alexithymic individuals. For example, visceral sensitivity to stimulation is associated with alexithymia [111]. Alexithymic persons have exhibited higher activation in the pain matrix area of brain [112]. Women with alexithymia have demonstrated higher activation of sensory and motor cortex, compared with controls [113]. These findings are consistent with the psychosomatic theory in which alexithymic patients tend to express their conflicts of emotion somatically rather than linguistically.
It is seemingly contradictory that emotional and somatic interoceptive awareness positively correlate, while impairment of emotional awareness leads to somatosensory amplification. Moriguchi and Komaki elucidated this argument by demonstrating the limitation of TAS-20 as a self-report measure and the heterogeneity of alexithymia [114]. As mentioned before, Bermond and Vorst hypothesized subtypes of alexithymia by dimensional model: one characterized by attenuation of physiological arousal, and another marked by intact physiological arousal [70]. This typology could partially explain the apparent contradiction.
Another explanation is suggested by the difference between interoceptive awareness and somatosensory amplification [115]. For example, a number of patients with chronic pain, who have less emotional and interoceptive awareness, experience persistence of a specific type of pain, such as myalgia of some part of the body, with somatosensory amplification. Barsky et al., who advocated the concept of somatosensory amplification, demonstrated that patients with hypochondria had high somatosensory amplification but were not good at cardiac awareness [116, 117]. Moreover, Mailloux and Brener directly examined the relationship between somatosensory amplification and cardiac interoceptive awareness [118]. They showed greater somatosensory amplification in poor heartbeat detectors than in good detectors, and suggested that somatosensory amplification is a “cognitive bias” and does not reflect heightened somatic awareness.
Interoceptive awareness and somatosensory amplification are therefore not on the same axis. At its core, somatosensory amplification is a condition in which sensation of a somatic state is higher than the assumed actual state, whereas interoception is an “accurate” sensation of a somatic state; therefore, reduced interoception could mean either a hyper- or hyposensitive condition. Individuals with less interoceptive or emotional awareness may not perceive their own somatic or emotional state properly and may exhibit somatosensory amplification.
According to psychosomatic theory, the amplification comes not only from cognitive bias but also from physiological bias. Kano and Fukudo reported in their recent review that alexithymic individuals showed reduced responses in limbic areas in their cognitive processing, but amplified responses in more primitive processing, e.g., hypersensitivity to visceral pain [119]. Somatosensory amplification in more primitive processes may occur typically in psychosomatic patients who have less emotional and interoceptive awareness.

Discussion

Several key points were presented in this paper1. First, both emotional and somatic interoceptive awareness are important for psychosomatic health. The main subcortical neural substrates for these processes are limbic-related systems: the amygdala, cingulate and insula, which are also responsible for autonomic and HPA axis functions for optimization of homeostatic efficiency. Second, considerable studies show that autonomic activity and/or reactivity to stress correlates with both emotional and interoceptive awareness. The hypothesis that autonomic dysfunction is involved in the disturbance of emotional and interoceptive awareness has been advocated (Fig. 1). Third, several sources of evidence suggest links or a cooperative relationship between emotional and somatic awareness. However, impairment of emotional or somatic awareness often accompanies somatosensory amplifications in a specific area or system, typically in psychosomatic patients.
The first point suggests links between emotional/interoceptive awareness and psychosomatic health through homeostatic or allostatic process. The so-called emotional awareness or somatic awareness approach, which is assumed to accelerate emotional or somatic awareness, could also accelerate the homeostatic process and lead to psychosomatic health. One of the key mechanisms for psychosomatic health is links between limbic-related systems and higher cortical systems. The emotional/somatic awareness approaches are assumed to accelerate these links.

Awareness and autonomic function: Clinical implications

Regarding the second point, in which both emotional and somatic awareness correlate with autonomic function, increased awareness could improve autonomic function and consequently autonomic-related symptoms; or reciprocally, the improvement of autonomic function could facilitate awareness. A number of neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that putting emotional feeling into words, i.e., affect labeling, changed the response to stimuli of the amygdala and other limbic structures [85, 120]. These results suggest that conscious awareness and linguistic processing of emotion effect alterations in autonomic function. Although it is inconclusive whether emotional awareness or linguistic processing actually effects the alteration of limbic function, the linguistic process necessarily involves conscious awareness, and both processes could improve autonomic function. The aforementioned negative feedback model, in which conscious awareness of emotion accelerates vagal function [6, 83] (Fig. 2) supports these arguments.
Interoceptive awareness also could improve autonomic function in a way suggested by the second and third points and arguments in the last two sections. In the context of the negative feedback model, interoceptive awareness probably accelerates vagal function, which leads to the reduction of symptoms. Actually, Schaefer et al. reported that improving interoceptive awareness reduced symptom distress in patients with somatoform disorders or medically unexplained symptoms [121, 122]. Further empirical verification of the relationship between interoceptive awareness and the vagal feedback process is required.
On the basis of this review, the hypothesis presented in Fig. 1, and our previous studies, in which psychosomatic patients had low autonomic responsiveness, we conclude that autonomic responsiveness is involved in one of the most important processes for emotional/somatic awareness upon psychosomatic health. Our previous study also suggested co-occurrence of low variability in subjective feelings of tension and attenuated autonomic response [72]. Low variability in autonomic function and subjective feeling probably contributes to disturbance of emotional and/or interoceptive awareness through less discriminative sensation. Excessive variability also may contribute to this disturbance. We therefore tentatively consider variability in autonomic function as one of the most important contributing factors for emotional/somatic awareness although this hypothesis needs further verification study.
The autonomic dysfunctional condition interacting with the impairment of emotional and/or somatic awareness seen in Fig. 1 leads to unhealthy psychosomatic conditions through insufficient homeostatic or allostatic process along with autonomic dysfunction. The condition should be ameliorated by improvement in emotional/somatic awareness.

Alexithymia and alexisomia

Regarding the third point, either the emotional or somatic form of awareness could facilitate the other. The concept of “response system coherence” [123125], that is, the idea that emotions organize and synchronize different response systems (e.g., behavioral and physiological) [126], can explain the cooperative relationship between emotional and somatic awareness. Sze et al. reported that the coherence between subjective emotion and cardiac awareness was greater in those who had specialized training in promotion of somatic awareness than did controls [126]. Our arguments are consistent with the clinically accepted process that “body-oriented” approaches such as yoga, biofeedback, and certain kinds of body psychotherapies encourage the emotional process of awareness, i.e., improvement of the alexithymic trait.
Ikemi, a founder of psychosomatic medicine in Japan, first described the concept of alexisomia in association with alexithymia in the early 1980s as clinical characteristics of difficulties in awareness or expression of somatic feeling/sensations from an Eastern point of view [5, 127]. Mind and body are considered as one harmonic entity in the Eastern view, but as dualistic entities in the Western view. The concept of alexisomia was developed on the basis of the Eastern conceptualization, but the concept has not yet been explored extensively. Meanwhile, the mechanism of interoceptive awareness has been investigated in the context of neurophysiological studies as described in the present paper. Impairment of interoceptive awareness appears to be similar to alexisomia, although a conclusive decision about whether these states are exactly the same cannot yet be made.
Regarding the relationship between alexisomia and alexithymia, Moriguchi and Komaki addressed this topic with reference to reviews of neuroimaging studies and concluded that somatic awareness is the basis of emotional awareness because bodily states, including autonomic and hormonal status, are fundamentally involved in basic affective states [114]. Damasio advocated the somatic marker hypothesis, wherein emotional processes lead behavior, especially the choice of a proper action or decision, and demonstrated the fundamental role of the physiological state in emotional processing [50, 128].
Therefore, although emotional awareness, somatic awareness and autonomic processing are generally parallel or concurrent processes, the aforementioned arguments suggest that somatic awareness is fundamental for emotional awareness, and that the autonomic homeostatic process has a more fundamental involvement in somatic and emotional awareness. However, psychosomatic patients with alexithymia/alexisomia have complex pathologies, which include coexistence of impairment of interoceptive awareness and somatosensory amplification, as previously described. This complexity might be associated with the multiple pathologies seen in patients not only with psychosomatic disorder but also with chronic pain or functional somatic syndromes.
Another source of complexity is that the process of awareness is involved in various levels of processing such as peripheral/sensory, intermediate/limbic related and central/cognitive systems. Alexisomia includes impairments of not only peripheral processes but also cognitive or even higher-level processes [5]. One of the challenges and difficulties for investigation of mechanisms of alexisomia is that this multilevel construct applies here as well as in alexithymia. Further clinical and physiological investigations that consider such multilevel properties are needed for better understanding of the process of alexisomia and interoceptive awareness.

Self and awareness

Finally, any arguments about awareness, which can be seen as a change in the subjective–objective relationship, must include some mention of the notion of “self”, because subjective feelings necessarily require a self that experiences the feelings. Craig stated that to have awareness is to know that one exists [4] and addressed the neural model for integrative representations of all feelings from the body at any moment as “the sentient self” [11]. Damasio posited that the self is a repeatedly reconstructed biological state, called “neural self” [129]. Thus, the self is inseparable from the somatic and emotional feelings or senses and awareness is a state of linkage between the self and the senses. This theme is extremely profound and beyond the scope of our paper, but clinically, impairment of emotional/somatic awareness likely relates to disestablishment of the self. We may therefore have to consider the need for reestablishment of the self in our approach to patients with difficulties in these types of awareness or alexithymia/alexisomia, especially children or persons with developmental disorders.

Conclusions

This paper reviewed links between emotional awareness, somatic awareness including interoception, and autonomic homeostatic processing, and advocated a hypothesis about the links between the two types of awareness and autonomic function. Autonomic homeostatic processing has a fundamental involvement in both emotional and somatic awareness. Investigation of these types of awareness, using both neuroimaging evaluations and estimation of peripheral autonomic function, are required next steps for exploration of their relationship to somatic symptoms, and to the more general issue of human psychosomatic health.

Endnote

1For readability, the key points of this manuscript are indicated as (1), (2), and (3) in the abstract. The numbers correspond to points labeled as “first,” “second,” and “third” in the Discussion section. The first point also relates to the context in the Background and to the subsection titled “Neural substrates for emotional/somatic awareness and autonomic homeostatic processing.” The second point corresponds to the following subsections in the main text: “Emotional awareness and autonomic homeostatic processing,” “Emotional awareness and feedback model through vagal function,” and “Interoceptive awareness and autonomic homeostatic processing.” The third point corresponds to the subsection titled “Interoceptive awareness and emotional awareness.”

Acknowledgements

This review study was supported in part by Grant from Pfizer Health Research Foundation and the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (C), 22570228, 2010–2012.
We would like to express our appreciation to Prof. Haruhiko Murakawa, Prof. Kiyoshi Hamano and Dr. Makoto Hashizume for their valuable suggestions. We would also like to sincerely thank the members of staff at Kansai Medical University, and members of the Society for Alexisomia Research.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

KK and MF collected the research and edited the paper. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Chalmers DJ. The Conscious Mind. In: Search of a Fundamental Theory. 1st ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1997. Chalmers DJ. The Conscious Mind. In: Search of a Fundamental Theory. 1st ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1997.
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Craig AD. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:655–66.PubMedCrossRef Craig AD. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:655–66.PubMedCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Craig AD. Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003;13:500–5.PubMedCrossRef Craig AD. Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003;13:500–5.PubMedCrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Craig AD. How do you feel--now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:59–70.PubMedCrossRef Craig AD. How do you feel--now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:59–70.PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Ikemi Y, Ikemi A. An oriental point of view in psychosomatic medicine. Psychother Psychosom. 1986;45:118–26.PubMedCrossRef Ikemi Y, Ikemi A. An oriental point of view in psychosomatic medicine. Psychother Psychosom. 1986;45:118–26.PubMedCrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Lane RD, Schwartz GE. Levels of emotional awareness: a cognitive-developmental theory and its application to psychopathology. Am J Psychiatry. 1987;144:133–43.PubMedCrossRef Lane RD, Schwartz GE. Levels of emotional awareness: a cognitive-developmental theory and its application to psychopathology. Am J Psychiatry. 1987;144:133–43.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Lane RD. Neural substrates of implicit and explicit emotional processes: a unifying framework for psychosomatic medicine. Psychosom Med. 2008;70:214–31.PubMedCrossRef Lane RD. Neural substrates of implicit and explicit emotional processes: a unifying framework for psychosomatic medicine. Psychosom Med. 2008;70:214–31.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Lane RD. Theory of emotional awareness and brain processing of emotion. Int Congr Ser. 2006;1287:116–21.CrossRef Lane RD. Theory of emotional awareness and brain processing of emotion. Int Congr Ser. 2006;1287:116–21.CrossRef
10.
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Duschek S, Montoro CI, Reyes Del Paso GA. Diminished interoceptive awareness in fibromyalgia syndrome. Behav Med. 2015;4289:Oct 2:0 [Epub ahead of print]. Duschek S, Montoro CI, Reyes Del Paso GA. Diminished interoceptive awareness in fibromyalgia syndrome. Behav Med. 2015;4289:Oct 2:0 [Epub ahead of print].
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Sifneos PE. The prevalence of “alexithymic” characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychother Psychosom. 1973;22:255–62.PubMedCrossRef Sifneos PE. The prevalence of “alexithymic” characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychother Psychosom. 1973;22:255–62.PubMedCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Nemiah J, Freyberger H. Alexithymia: a view of the psychosomatic process. In: Hill O, editor. Modem trends in psychosomatic medicine, vol. 3. London: Butterworths; 1976. p. 430–9. Nemiah J, Freyberger H. Alexithymia: a view of the psychosomatic process. In: Hill O, editor. Modem trends in psychosomatic medicine, vol. 3. London: Butterworths; 1976. p. 430–9.
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Nemiah J, Sifneos P. Affect and fantasy in patients with psychosomatic disorders. In: Hill O, editor. Modem trends in psychosomatic medicine, vol. 2. London: Butterworths; 1970. p. 26–34. Nemiah J, Sifneos P. Affect and fantasy in patients with psychosomatic disorders. In: Hill O, editor. Modem trends in psychosomatic medicine, vol. 2. London: Butterworths; 1970. p. 26–34.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor GJ, Bagby RM, Parker JDA. Disorders of affect regulation: Alexithymia in medical and psychiatric illness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1997.CrossRef Taylor GJ, Bagby RM, Parker JDA. Disorders of affect regulation: Alexithymia in medical and psychiatric illness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1997.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Grabe HJ, Schwahn C, Barnow S, Spitzer C, John U, Freyberger HJ, et al. Alexithymia, hypertension, and subclinical atherosclerosis in the general population. J Psychosom Res. 2010;68:139–47.PubMedCrossRef Grabe HJ, Schwahn C, Barnow S, Spitzer C, John U, Freyberger HJ, et al. Alexithymia, hypertension, and subclinical atherosclerosis in the general population. J Psychosom Res. 2010;68:139–47.PubMedCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Porcelli P, De Carne M, Todarello O. Prediction of treatment outcome of patients with functional gastrointestinal disorders by the diagnostic criteria for psychosomatic research. Psychother Psychosom. 2004;73:166–73.PubMedCrossRef Porcelli P, De Carne M, Todarello O. Prediction of treatment outcome of patients with functional gastrointestinal disorders by the diagnostic criteria for psychosomatic research. Psychother Psychosom. 2004;73:166–73.PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Kooiman CG. The status of alexithymia as a risk factor in medically unexplained physical symptoms. Compr Psychiatry. 1998;39:152–9.PubMedCrossRef Kooiman CG. The status of alexithymia as a risk factor in medically unexplained physical symptoms. Compr Psychiatry. 1998;39:152–9.PubMedCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Acklin MW, Bernat E. Depression, alexithymia, and pain prone disorder: a Rorschach study. J Pers Assess. 1987;51:462–79.PubMedCrossRef Acklin MW, Bernat E. Depression, alexithymia, and pain prone disorder: a Rorschach study. J Pers Assess. 1987;51:462–79.PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Lumley MA, Radcliffe AM, Macklem DJ, Mosley-Williams A, Leisen JCC, Huffman JL, et al. Alexithymia and pain in three chronic pain samples: comparing Caucasians and African Americans. Pain Med. 2005;6:251–61.PubMedCrossRef Lumley MA, Radcliffe AM, Macklem DJ, Mosley-Williams A, Leisen JCC, Huffman JL, et al. Alexithymia and pain in three chronic pain samples: comparing Caucasians and African Americans. Pain Med. 2005;6:251–61.PubMedCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Lumley MA, Stettner L, Wehmer F. How are alexithymia and physical illness linked? A review and critique of pathways. J Psychosom Res. 1996;41:505–18.PubMedCrossRef Lumley MA, Stettner L, Wehmer F. How are alexithymia and physical illness linked? A review and critique of pathways. J Psychosom Res. 1996;41:505–18.PubMedCrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat MacLean PD. Psychosomatic disease and the visceral brain; recent developments bearing on the Papez theory of emotion. Psychosom Med. 1949;11:338–53.PubMedCrossRef MacLean PD. Psychosomatic disease and the visceral brain; recent developments bearing on the Papez theory of emotion. Psychosom Med. 1949;11:338–53.PubMedCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat MacLean PD. Some psychiatric implications of physiological studies on frontotemporal portion of limbic system (visceral brain). Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1952;4:407–18.PubMedCrossRef MacLean PD. Some psychiatric implications of physiological studies on frontotemporal portion of limbic system (visceral brain). Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1952;4:407–18.PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Weiskrantz L. Behavioral changes associated with ablation of the amygdaloid complex in monkeys. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1956;49:381–91.PubMedCrossRef Weiskrantz L. Behavioral changes associated with ablation of the amygdaloid complex in monkeys. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1956;49:381–91.PubMedCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Aggleton JP. The Amygdala: A Functional Analysis. 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2000. Aggleton JP. The Amygdala: A Functional Analysis. 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2000.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat LaBar KS, LeDoux JE, Spencer DD, Phelps EA. Impaired fear conditioning following unilateral temporal lobectomy in humans. J Neurosci. 1995;15:6846–55.PubMed LaBar KS, LeDoux JE, Spencer DD, Phelps EA. Impaired fear conditioning following unilateral temporal lobectomy in humans. J Neurosci. 1995;15:6846–55.PubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Adolphs R, Rockland C, Damasio AR C. Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science. 1995;269:1115–8.PubMedCrossRef Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Adolphs R, Rockland C, Damasio AR C. Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science. 1995;269:1115–8.PubMedCrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Babinsky R, Calabrese P, Durwen HF, Markowitsch HJ, Brechtelsbauer D, Heuser L, et al. The possible contribution of the amygdala to memory. Behav Neurol. 1993;6:167–70.PubMedCrossRef Babinsky R, Calabrese P, Durwen HF, Markowitsch HJ, Brechtelsbauer D, Heuser L, et al. The possible contribution of the amygdala to memory. Behav Neurol. 1993;6:167–70.PubMedCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Adolphs R, Cahill L, Schul R, Babinsky R. Impaired declarative memory for emotional material following bilateral amygdala damage in humans. Learn Mem. 1997;4:291–300.PubMedCrossRef Adolphs R, Cahill L, Schul R, Babinsky R. Impaired declarative memory for emotional material following bilateral amygdala damage in humans. Learn Mem. 1997;4:291–300.PubMedCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Papez JW. A proposed mechanism of emotion. Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 1937;38:725.CrossRef Papez JW. A proposed mechanism of emotion. Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 1937;38:725.CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Vogt BA, Finch DM, Olson CR. Functional heterogeneity in cingulate cortex: the anterior executive and posterior evaluative regions. Cereb Cortex. 1992;2:435–43.PubMed Vogt BA, Finch DM, Olson CR. Functional heterogeneity in cingulate cortex: the anterior executive and posterior evaluative regions. Cereb Cortex. 1992;2:435–43.PubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Bush G, Luu P, Posner M. Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4:215–22.PubMedCrossRef Bush G, Luu P, Posner M. Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4:215–22.PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Mohanty A, Engels AS, Herrington JD, Heller W, Ringo Ho M-HH, Banich MT, et al. Differential engagement of anterior cingulate cortex subdivisions for cognitive and emotional function. Psychophysiology. 2007;44:343–51.PubMedCrossRef Mohanty A, Engels AS, Herrington JD, Heller W, Ringo Ho M-HH, Banich MT, et al. Differential engagement of anterior cingulate cortex subdivisions for cognitive and emotional function. Psychophysiology. 2007;44:343–51.PubMedCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage. 2002;16:331–48.PubMedCrossRef Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage. 2002;16:331–48.PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Hutcherson CA, Goldin PR, Ochsner KN, Gabrieli JD, Barrett LF, Gross JJ. Attention and emotion: does rating emotion alter neural responses to amusing and sad films? Neuroimage. 2005;27:656–68.PubMedCrossRef Hutcherson CA, Goldin PR, Ochsner KN, Gabrieli JD, Barrett LF, Gross JJ. Attention and emotion: does rating emotion alter neural responses to amusing and sad films? Neuroimage. 2005;27:656–68.PubMedCrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Lane RD, Quinlan DM, Schwartz GE, Walker PA, Zeitlin SB. The levels of emotional awareness scale: a cognitive-developmental measure of emotion. J Pers Assess. 1990;55:124–34.PubMedCrossRef Lane RD, Quinlan DM, Schwartz GE, Walker PA, Zeitlin SB. The levels of emotional awareness scale: a cognitive-developmental measure of emotion. J Pers Assess. 1990;55:124–34.PubMedCrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat McRae K, Reiman EM, Fort CL, Chen K, Lane RD. Association between trait emotional awareness and dorsal anterior cingulate activity during emotion is arousal-dependent. Neuroimage. 2008;41:648–55.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef McRae K, Reiman EM, Fort CL, Chen K, Lane RD. Association between trait emotional awareness and dorsal anterior cingulate activity during emotion is arousal-dependent. Neuroimage. 2008;41:648–55.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Devinsky O, Morrell MJ, Vogt BA. Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain. 1995;118(Pt 1):279–306.PubMedCrossRef Devinsky O, Morrell MJ, Vogt BA. Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain. 1995;118(Pt 1):279–306.PubMedCrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Cabeza R, Nyberg L. Imaging cognition: an empirical review of PET studies with normal subjects. J Cogn Neurosci. 1997;9:1–26.PubMedCrossRef Cabeza R, Nyberg L. Imaging cognition: an empirical review of PET studies with normal subjects. J Cogn Neurosci. 1997;9:1–26.PubMedCrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Drevets WC, Raichle ME. Suppression of regional cerebral blood during emotional versus higher cognitive implications for interactions between emotion and cognition. Cogn Emot. 1998;12:353–85.CrossRef Drevets WC, Raichle ME. Suppression of regional cerebral blood during emotional versus higher cognitive implications for interactions between emotion and cognition. Cogn Emot. 1998;12:353–85.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Williams LM, Liddell BJ, Kemp AH, Bryant RA, Meares RA, Peduto AS, et al. Amygdala-prefrontal dissociation of subliminal and supraliminal fear. Hum Brain Mapp. 2006;27:652–61.PubMedCrossRef Williams LM, Liddell BJ, Kemp AH, Bryant RA, Meares RA, Peduto AS, et al. Amygdala-prefrontal dissociation of subliminal and supraliminal fear. Hum Brain Mapp. 2006;27:652–61.PubMedCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Morris JS, Ohman A, Dolan RJ. Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature. 1998;393:467–70.PubMedCrossRef Morris JS, Ohman A, Dolan RJ. Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature. 1998;393:467–70.PubMedCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Fabri M, Burton H. Ipsilateral cortical connections of primary somatic sensory cortex in rats. J Comp Neurol. 1991;311:405–24.PubMedCrossRef Fabri M, Burton H. Ipsilateral cortical connections of primary somatic sensory cortex in rats. J Comp Neurol. 1991;311:405–24.PubMedCrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Ohman A, Dolan RJ. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:189–95.PubMedCrossRef Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Ohman A, Dolan RJ. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:189–95.PubMedCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Critchley HD. Neural mechanisms of autonomic, affective, and cognitive integration. J Comp Neurol. 2005;493:154–66.PubMedCrossRef Critchley HD. Neural mechanisms of autonomic, affective, and cognitive integration. J Comp Neurol. 2005;493:154–66.PubMedCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat McEwen BS, Stellar E, Bruce S. Stress and the individual. Mechanisms leading to disease. Arch Intern Med. 1993;153:2093–101.PubMedCrossRef McEwen BS, Stellar E, Bruce S. Stress and the individual. Mechanisms leading to disease. Arch Intern Med. 1993;153:2093–101.PubMedCrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Sterling P, Eyer J. Allostasis: a new paradigm to explain arousal pathology. In: Fisher J, Reason J, editors. Handbook of Life Stress Cognition, and Health. New York: Wiley; 1988. p. 629–49. Sterling P, Eyer J. Allostasis: a new paradigm to explain arousal pathology. In: Fisher J, Reason J, editors. Handbook of Life Stress Cognition, and Health. New York: Wiley; 1988. p. 629–49.
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Damasio AR. Descartes’ Error : Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Penguin; 1994. Damasio AR. Descartes’ Error : Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Penguin; 1994.
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Damasio AR. Testing the somatic marker hypothesis. In: Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Penguin; 1994. p. 205–22. Damasio AR. Testing the somatic marker hypothesis. In: Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Penguin; 1994. p. 205–22.
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor GJ, Bagby RM. New trends in alexithymia research. Psychother Psychosom. 2004;73:68–77.PubMedCrossRef Taylor GJ, Bagby RM. New trends in alexithymia research. Psychother Psychosom. 2004;73:68–77.PubMedCrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Infrasca R. Alexithymia, neurovegetative arousal and neuroticism. An experimental study. Psychother Psychosom. 1997;66:276–80.PubMedCrossRef Infrasca R. Alexithymia, neurovegetative arousal and neuroticism. An experimental study. Psychother Psychosom. 1997;66:276–80.PubMedCrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Rabavilas AD. Electrodermal activity in low and high alexithymia neurotic patients. Psychother Psychosom. 1987;47:101–4.PubMedCrossRef Rabavilas AD. Electrodermal activity in low and high alexithymia neurotic patients. Psychother Psychosom. 1987;47:101–4.PubMedCrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Wehmer F, Brejnak C, Lumley M, Stettner L. Alexithymia and physiological reactivity to emotional-provoking visual scenes. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1995;183:351–7.PubMedCrossRef Wehmer F, Brejnak C, Lumley M, Stettner L. Alexithymia and physiological reactivity to emotional-provoking visual scenes. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1995;183:351–7.PubMedCrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Linden W, Lenz JW, Stossel C. Alexithymia, defensiveness and cardiovascular reactivity to stress. J Psychosom Res. 1996;41:575–83.PubMedCrossRef Linden W, Lenz JW, Stossel C. Alexithymia, defensiveness and cardiovascular reactivity to stress. J Psychosom Res. 1996;41:575–83.PubMedCrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Neumann SA, Sollers JJ, Thayer JF, Waldstein SR. Alexithymia predicts attenuated autonomic reactivity, but prolonged recovery to anger recall in young women. Int J Psychophysiol. 2004;53:183–95.PubMedCrossRef Neumann SA, Sollers JJ, Thayer JF, Waldstein SR. Alexithymia predicts attenuated autonomic reactivity, but prolonged recovery to anger recall in young women. Int J Psychophysiol. 2004;53:183–95.PubMedCrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Friedlander L, Lumley MA, Farchione T, Doyal G. Testing the alexithymia hypothesis: physiological and subjective responses during relaxation and stress. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1997;185:233–9.PubMedCrossRef Friedlander L, Lumley MA, Farchione T, Doyal G. Testing the alexithymia hypothesis: physiological and subjective responses during relaxation and stress. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1997;185:233–9.PubMedCrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Roedema TM, Simons RF. Emotion-processing deficit in alexithymia. Psychophysiology. 1999;36:379–87.PubMedCrossRef Roedema TM, Simons RF. Emotion-processing deficit in alexithymia. Psychophysiology. 1999;36:379–87.PubMedCrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Stone LA, Nielson KA. Intact physiological response to arousal with impaired emotional recognition in alexithymia. Psychother Psychosom. 2001;70:92–102.PubMedCrossRef Stone LA, Nielson KA. Intact physiological response to arousal with impaired emotional recognition in alexithymia. Psychother Psychosom. 2001;70:92–102.PubMedCrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Newton TL, Contrada RJ. Alexithymia and repression: contrasting emotion-focused coping styles. Psychosom Med. 1994;56:457–62.PubMedCrossRef Newton TL, Contrada RJ. Alexithymia and repression: contrasting emotion-focused coping styles. Psychosom Med. 1994;56:457–62.PubMedCrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Luminet O, Rimé B, Bagby RM, Taylor G. A multimodal investigation of emotional responding in alexithymia. Cogn Emot. 2004;18:741–66.CrossRef Luminet O, Rimé B, Bagby RM, Taylor G. A multimodal investigation of emotional responding in alexithymia. Cogn Emot. 2004;18:741–66.CrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Dawson ME, Schell AM, Filion DL. The electrodermal system. In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Berntson GG, editors. Handbook of psychophysiology. Thirdth ed. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2007. p. 159–81. Dawson ME, Schell AM, Filion DL. The electrodermal system. In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Berntson GG, editors. Handbook of psychophysiology. Thirdth ed. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2007. p. 159–81.
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Wilder J. Stimulus and response: The law of initial value. Bristol: J. Wright; 1967. Wilder J. Stimulus and response: The law of initial value. Bristol: J. Wright; 1967.
65.
Zurück zum Zitat De Timary P, Roy E, Luminet O, Fillée C, Mikolajczak M. Relationship between alexithymia, alexithymia factors and salivary cortisol in men exposed to a social stress test. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33:1160–4.PubMedCrossRef De Timary P, Roy E, Luminet O, Fillée C, Mikolajczak M. Relationship between alexithymia, alexithymia factors and salivary cortisol in men exposed to a social stress test. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33:1160–4.PubMedCrossRef
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Bauer AM, Quas JA, Boyce WT. Associations between physiological reactivity and children’s behavior: advantages of a multisystem approach. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2002;23:102–13.PubMedCrossRef Bauer AM, Quas JA, Boyce WT. Associations between physiological reactivity and children’s behavior: advantages of a multisystem approach. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2002;23:102–13.PubMedCrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Pollatos O, Werner NS, Duschek S, Schandry R, Matthias E, Traut-Mattausch E, et al. Differential effects of alexithymia subscales on autonomic reactivity and anxiety during social stress. J Psychosom Res. 2011;70:525–33.PubMedCrossRef Pollatos O, Werner NS, Duschek S, Schandry R, Matthias E, Traut-Mattausch E, et al. Differential effects of alexithymia subscales on autonomic reactivity and anxiety during social stress. J Psychosom Res. 2011;70:525–33.PubMedCrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Bagby RM, Parker JDA, Taylor GJ. The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia scale-I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J Psychosom Res. 1994;38:23–32.PubMedCrossRef Bagby RM, Parker JDA, Taylor GJ. The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia scale-I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J Psychosom Res. 1994;38:23–32.PubMedCrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Bagby RM, Taylor GJ, Parker JD. The Twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale-II. Convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity. J Psychosom Res. 1994;38:33–40.PubMedCrossRef Bagby RM, Taylor GJ, Parker JD. The Twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale-II. Convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity. J Psychosom Res. 1994;38:33–40.PubMedCrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Vorst HCM, Bermond B. Validity and reliability of the bermond - Vorst alexithymia questionnaire. Pers Individ Dif. 2001;30:413–34.CrossRef Vorst HCM, Bermond B. Validity and reliability of the bermond - Vorst alexithymia questionnaire. Pers Individ Dif. 2001;30:413–34.CrossRef
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Bermond B, Vorst HCM, Moormann PP. Cognitive neuropsychology of alexithymia: implications for personality typology. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2006;11:332–60.PubMedCrossRef Bermond B, Vorst HCM, Moormann PP. Cognitive neuropsychology of alexithymia: implications for personality typology. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2006;11:332–60.PubMedCrossRef
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Kanbara K, Fukunaga M, Ishino S, Takebayashi N, Nakai Y. Characteristics of psychophysiological stress responses in patients with psychosomatic disorders. Japanese J Psychosom Med. 2005;45:685–95. Kanbara K, Fukunaga M, Ishino S, Takebayashi N, Nakai Y. Characteristics of psychophysiological stress responses in patients with psychosomatic disorders. Japanese J Psychosom Med. 2005;45:685–95.
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Kanbara K, Mitani Y, Fukunaga M, Ishino S, Takebayashi N, Nakai Y. Paradoxical results of psychophysiological stress profile in functional somatic syndrome: Correlation between subjective tension score and objective stress response. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2004;29:255–68.PubMedCrossRef Kanbara K, Mitani Y, Fukunaga M, Ishino S, Takebayashi N, Nakai Y. Paradoxical results of psychophysiological stress profile in functional somatic syndrome: Correlation between subjective tension score and objective stress response. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2004;29:255–68.PubMedCrossRef
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Kanbara K, Fukunaga M, Mutsuura H, Takeuchi H, Kitamura K, Nakai Y. An exploratory study of subgrouping of patients with functional somatic syndrome based on the psychophysiological stress response: its relationship with moods and subjective variables. Psychosom Med. 2007;69:158–65.PubMedCrossRef Kanbara K, Fukunaga M, Mutsuura H, Takeuchi H, Kitamura K, Nakai Y. An exploratory study of subgrouping of patients with functional somatic syndrome based on the psychophysiological stress response: its relationship with moods and subjective variables. Psychosom Med. 2007;69:158–65.PubMedCrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Kiba T, Kanbara K, Yamamoto K, Ban I, Oka Y, Kato F, et al. The relationship of pre-stress salivary α-amylase levels to alexithymia in patients with functional somatic syndrome. Japanese J Psychosom Med. 2013;53:670–81. Kiba T, Kanbara K, Yamamoto K, Ban I, Oka Y, Kato F, et al. The relationship of pre-stress salivary α-amylase levels to alexithymia in patients with functional somatic syndrome. Japanese J Psychosom Med. 2013;53:670–81.
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Kiba T, Kanbara K, Ban I, Kato F, Kawashima S, Saka Y, et al. Saliva Amylase as a measure of sympathetic change elicited by autogenic training in patients with functional somatic syndromes. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2015;40:339–47.PubMedCrossRef Kiba T, Kanbara K, Ban I, Kato F, Kawashima S, Saka Y, et al. Saliva Amylase as a measure of sympathetic change elicited by autogenic training in patients with functional somatic syndromes. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2015;40:339–47.PubMedCrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Drevets WC. Prefrontal cortical-amygdalar metabolism in major depression. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;877:614–7.PubMedCrossRef Drevets WC. Prefrontal cortical-amygdalar metabolism in major depression. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;877:614–7.PubMedCrossRef
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Buchanan SL, Valentine J, Powell DA. Autonomic responses are elicited by electrical stimulation of the medial but not lateral frontal cortex in rabbits. Behav Brain Res. 1985;18:51–62.PubMedCrossRef Buchanan SL, Valentine J, Powell DA. Autonomic responses are elicited by electrical stimulation of the medial but not lateral frontal cortex in rabbits. Behav Brain Res. 1985;18:51–62.PubMedCrossRef
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Gianaros PJ, Van Der Veen FM, Jennings JR. Regional cerebral blood flow correlates with heart period and high-frequency heart period variability during working-memory tasks: Implications for the cortical and subcortical regulation of cardiac autonomic activity. Psychophysiology. 2004;41:521–30.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Gianaros PJ, Van Der Veen FM, Jennings JR. Regional cerebral blood flow correlates with heart period and high-frequency heart period variability during working-memory tasks: Implications for the cortical and subcortical regulation of cardiac autonomic activity. Psychophysiology. 2004;41:521–30.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Lane RD, McRae K, Reiman EM, Chen K, Ahern GL, Thayer JF. Neural correlates of heart rate variability during emotion. Neuroimage. 2009;44:213–22.PubMedCrossRef Lane RD, McRae K, Reiman EM, Chen K, Ahern GL, Thayer JF. Neural correlates of heart rate variability during emotion. Neuroimage. 2009;44:213–22.PubMedCrossRef
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Matthews SC, Paulus MP, Simmons AN, Nelesen RA, Dimsdale JE. Functional subdivisions within anterior cingulate cortex and their relationship to autonomic nervous system function. Neuroimage. 2004;22:1151–6.PubMedCrossRef Matthews SC, Paulus MP, Simmons AN, Nelesen RA, Dimsdale JE. Functional subdivisions within anterior cingulate cortex and their relationship to autonomic nervous system function. Neuroimage. 2004;22:1151–6.PubMedCrossRef
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Barrett LF, Niedenthal PM, Winkielman P. Emotion and Consciousness. New York: Guilford Press; 2005. Barrett LF, Niedenthal PM, Winkielman P. Emotion and Consciousness. New York: Guilford Press; 2005.
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Thayer JF, Lane RD. A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J Affect Disord. 2000;61:201–16.PubMedCrossRef Thayer JF, Lane RD. A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J Affect Disord. 2000;61:201–16.PubMedCrossRef
84.
Zurück zum Zitat Appelhans BM, Luecken LJ. Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Rev Gen Psychol. 2006;10:229–40.CrossRef Appelhans BM, Luecken LJ. Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Rev Gen Psychol. 2006;10:229–40.CrossRef
85.
Zurück zum Zitat Lieberman MD, Eisenberger NI, Crockett MJ, Tom SM, Pfeifer JH, Way BM. Putting feelings into words: affect labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli. Psychol Sci. 2007;18:421–8.PubMedCrossRef Lieberman MD, Eisenberger NI, Crockett MJ, Tom SM, Pfeifer JH, Way BM. Putting feelings into words: affect labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli. Psychol Sci. 2007;18:421–8.PubMedCrossRef
86.
Zurück zum Zitat Medford N, Critchley HD. Conjoint activity of anterior insular and anterior cingulate cortex: awareness and response. Brain Struct Funct. 2010;214:535–49.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Medford N, Critchley HD. Conjoint activity of anterior insular and anterior cingulate cortex: awareness and response. Brain Struct Funct. 2010;214:535–49.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
87.
Zurück zum Zitat Katkin ES. Blood, sweat, and tears: individual differences in autonomic self-perception. Psychophysiology. 1985;22:125–37.PubMedCrossRef Katkin ES. Blood, sweat, and tears: individual differences in autonomic self-perception. Psychophysiology. 1985;22:125–37.PubMedCrossRef
88.
Zurück zum Zitat Eichler S, Katkin ES. The relationship between cardiovascular reactivity and heartbeat detection. Psychophysiology. 1994;31:229–34.PubMedCrossRef Eichler S, Katkin ES. The relationship between cardiovascular reactivity and heartbeat detection. Psychophysiology. 1994;31:229–34.PubMedCrossRef
89.
Zurück zum Zitat Schandry R. Heart beat perception and Emotional Experience. Psychophysiology. 1981;18:483–8.PubMedCrossRef Schandry R. Heart beat perception and Emotional Experience. Psychophysiology. 1981;18:483–8.PubMedCrossRef
90.
Zurück zum Zitat Whitehead WE, Drescher VM, Heiman P, Blackwell B. Relation of heart rate control to heartbeat perception. Biofeedback Self Regul. 1977;2:371–92.CrossRef Whitehead WE, Drescher VM, Heiman P, Blackwell B. Relation of heart rate control to heartbeat perception. Biofeedback Self Regul. 1977;2:371–92.CrossRef
91.
Zurück zum Zitat Pollatos O, Herbert BM, Matthias E, Schandry R. Heart rate response after emotional picture presentation is modulated by interoceptive awareness. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007;63:117–24.PubMedCrossRef Pollatos O, Herbert BM, Matthias E, Schandry R. Heart rate response after emotional picture presentation is modulated by interoceptive awareness. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007;63:117–24.PubMedCrossRef
92.
Zurück zum Zitat Pollatos O, Herbert BM, Kaufmann C, Auer DP, Schandry R. Interoceptive awareness, anxiety and cardiovascular reactivity to isometric exercise. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007;65:167–73.PubMedCrossRef Pollatos O, Herbert BM, Kaufmann C, Auer DP, Schandry R. Interoceptive awareness, anxiety and cardiovascular reactivity to isometric exercise. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007;65:167–73.PubMedCrossRef
93.
Zurück zum Zitat Herbert BM, Pollatos O, Flor H, Enck P, Schandry R. Cardiac awareness and autonomic cardiac reactivity during emotional picture viewing and mental stress. Psychophysiology. 2010;47:342–54.PubMedCrossRef Herbert BM, Pollatos O, Flor H, Enck P, Schandry R. Cardiac awareness and autonomic cardiac reactivity during emotional picture viewing and mental stress. Psychophysiology. 2010;47:342–54.PubMedCrossRef
94.
Zurück zum Zitat James W. The Principles of Psychology. New York: Dover Publications; 1950. James W. The Principles of Psychology. New York: Dover Publications; 1950.
96.
Zurück zum Zitat Lange CG. The mechanism of the emotions. In: Rand B, editor. The classical psychologists : selections illustrating psychology from Anaxagoras to Wundt. Boston: Houghton Mifflin; 1912. p. 672–84. Lange CG. The mechanism of the emotions. In: Rand B, editor. The classical psychologists : selections illustrating psychology from Anaxagoras to Wundt. Boston: Houghton Mifflin; 1912. p. 672–84.
97.
Zurück zum Zitat Ferguson ML, Katkin ES. Visceral perception, anhedonia, and emotion. Biol Psychol. 1996;42:131–45.PubMedCrossRef Ferguson ML, Katkin ES. Visceral perception, anhedonia, and emotion. Biol Psychol. 1996;42:131–45.PubMedCrossRef
98.
Zurück zum Zitat Wiens S, Mezzacappa ES, Katkin ES. Heartbeat detection and the experience of emotions. Cognition & Emotion. 2000;14:417–27. Wiens S, Mezzacappa ES, Katkin ES. Heartbeat detection and the experience of emotions. Cognition & Emotion. 2000;14:417–27.
99.
Zurück zum Zitat Herbert BM, Herbert C, Pollatos O. On the relationship between interoceptive awareness and alexithymia: Is interoceptive awareness related to emotional awareness? J Pers. 2011;79:1149–75.PubMedCrossRef Herbert BM, Herbert C, Pollatos O. On the relationship between interoceptive awareness and alexithymia: Is interoceptive awareness related to emotional awareness? J Pers. 2011;79:1149–75.PubMedCrossRef
100.
Zurück zum Zitat Herbert BM, Pollatos O, Schandry R. Interoceptive sensitivity and emotion processing: an EEG study. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007;65:214–27.PubMedCrossRef Herbert BM, Pollatos O, Schandry R. Interoceptive sensitivity and emotion processing: an EEG study. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007;65:214–27.PubMedCrossRef
101.
Zurück zum Zitat Pollatos O, Kirsch W, Schandry R. On the relationship between interoceptive awareness, emotional experience, and brain processes. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005;25:948–62.PubMedCrossRef Pollatos O, Kirsch W, Schandry R. On the relationship between interoceptive awareness, emotional experience, and brain processes. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005;25:948–62.PubMedCrossRef
102.
Zurück zum Zitat Pollatos O, Gramann K, Schandry R. Neural systems connecting interoceptive awareness and feelings. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28:9–18.PubMedCrossRef Pollatos O, Gramann K, Schandry R. Neural systems connecting interoceptive awareness and feelings. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28:9–18.PubMedCrossRef
103.
Zurück zum Zitat Schandry R, Sparrer B, Weitkunat R. From the heart to the brain: a study of heartbeat contingent scalp potentials. Int J Neurosci. 1986;30:261–75.PubMedCrossRef Schandry R, Sparrer B, Weitkunat R. From the heart to the brain: a study of heartbeat contingent scalp potentials. Int J Neurosci. 1986;30:261–75.PubMedCrossRef
104.
Zurück zum Zitat Montoya P, Schandry R, Müller A. Heartbeat evoked potentials (HEP): topography and influence of cardiac awareness and focus of attention. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1993;88:163–72.PubMedCrossRef Montoya P, Schandry R, Müller A. Heartbeat evoked potentials (HEP): topography and influence of cardiac awareness and focus of attention. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1993;88:163–72.PubMedCrossRef
105.
Zurück zum Zitat Schandry R, Weitkunat R. Enhancement of heartbeat-related brain potentials through cardiac awareness training. Int J Neurosci. 1990;53:243–53.PubMedCrossRef Schandry R, Weitkunat R. Enhancement of heartbeat-related brain potentials through cardiac awareness training. Int J Neurosci. 1990;53:243–53.PubMedCrossRef
106.
Zurück zum Zitat Fukushima H, Terasawa Y, Umeda S. Association between interoception and empathy: evidence from heartbeat-evoked brain potential. Int J Psychophysiol. 2011;79:259–65.PubMedCrossRef Fukushima H, Terasawa Y, Umeda S. Association between interoception and empathy: evidence from heartbeat-evoked brain potential. Int J Psychophysiol. 2011;79:259–65.PubMedCrossRef
107.
Zurück zum Zitat Phillips ML, Young AW, Senior C, Brammer M, Andrew C, Calder AJ, et al. A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature. 1997;389:495–8.PubMedCrossRef Phillips ML, Young AW, Senior C, Brammer M, Andrew C, Calder AJ, et al. A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature. 1997;389:495–8.PubMedCrossRef
108.
Zurück zum Zitat Büchel C, Morris J, Dolan RJ, Friston KJ. Brain systems mediating aversive conditioning: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron. 1998;20:947–57.PubMedCrossRef Büchel C, Morris J, Dolan RJ, Friston KJ. Brain systems mediating aversive conditioning: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron. 1998;20:947–57.PubMedCrossRef
109.
Zurück zum Zitat Terasawa Y, Fukushima H, Umeda S. How does interoceptive awareness interact with the subjective experience of emotion? An fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34:598–612.PubMed Terasawa Y, Fukushima H, Umeda S. How does interoceptive awareness interact with the subjective experience of emotion? An fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34:598–612.PubMed
110.
111.
Zurück zum Zitat Kano M, Hamaguchi T, Itoh M, Yanai K, Fukudo S. Correlation between alexithymia and hypersensitivity to visceral stimulation in human. Pain. 2007;132:252–63.PubMedCrossRef Kano M, Hamaguchi T, Itoh M, Yanai K, Fukudo S. Correlation between alexithymia and hypersensitivity to visceral stimulation in human. Pain. 2007;132:252–63.PubMedCrossRef
112.
Zurück zum Zitat Moriguchi Y, Decety J, Ohnishi T, Maeda M, Mori T, Nemoto K, et al. Empathy and judging other’s pain: an fMRI study of alexithymia. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:2223–34.PubMedCrossRef Moriguchi Y, Decety J, Ohnishi T, Maeda M, Mori T, Nemoto K, et al. Empathy and judging other’s pain: an fMRI study of alexithymia. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:2223–34.PubMedCrossRef
113.
Zurück zum Zitat Karlsson H, Näätänen P, Stenman H. Cortical activation in alexithymia as a response to emotional stimuli. Br J Psychiatry. 2008;192:32–8.PubMedCrossRef Karlsson H, Näätänen P, Stenman H. Cortical activation in alexithymia as a response to emotional stimuli. Br J Psychiatry. 2008;192:32–8.PubMedCrossRef
114.
115.
Zurück zum Zitat Kanbara K, Ban I, Fukunaga M, Nakai Y. The process toward awareness of bodily feeling and biofeedback [in Japanese]. Japanese J biofeedback Res. 2008;35:19–25. Kanbara K, Ban I, Fukunaga M, Nakai Y. The process toward awareness of bodily feeling and biofeedback [in Japanese]. Japanese J biofeedback Res. 2008;35:19–25.
116.
Zurück zum Zitat Barsky AJ, Wyshak G, Klerman GL. The somatosensory amplification scale and its relationship to hypochondriasis. J Psychiatr Res. 1990;24:323–34.PubMedCrossRef Barsky AJ, Wyshak G, Klerman GL. The somatosensory amplification scale and its relationship to hypochondriasis. J Psychiatr Res. 1990;24:323–34.PubMedCrossRef
117.
Zurück zum Zitat Barsky AJ, Brener J, Coeytaux RR, Cleary PD. Accurate awareness of heartbeat in hypochondriacal and non-hypochondriacal patients. J Psychosom Res. 1995;39:489–97.PubMedCrossRef Barsky AJ, Brener J, Coeytaux RR, Cleary PD. Accurate awareness of heartbeat in hypochondriacal and non-hypochondriacal patients. J Psychosom Res. 1995;39:489–97.PubMedCrossRef
118.
Zurück zum Zitat Mailloux J, Brener J. Somatosensory amplification and its relationship to heartbeat detection ability. Psychosom Med. 2002;64:353–7.PubMedCrossRef Mailloux J, Brener J. Somatosensory amplification and its relationship to heartbeat detection ability. Psychosom Med. 2002;64:353–7.PubMedCrossRef
119.
120.
Zurück zum Zitat Hariri AR, Bookheimer SY, Mazziotta JC. Modulating emotional responses: effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system. Neuroreport. 2000;11:43–8.PubMedCrossRef Hariri AR, Bookheimer SY, Mazziotta JC. Modulating emotional responses: effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system. Neuroreport. 2000;11:43–8.PubMedCrossRef
121.
Zurück zum Zitat Schaefer M, Egloff B, Witthöft M. Is interoceptive awareness really altered in somatoform disorders? Testing competing theories with two paradigms of heartbeat perception. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012;121:719–24.PubMedCrossRef Schaefer M, Egloff B, Witthöft M. Is interoceptive awareness really altered in somatoform disorders? Testing competing theories with two paradigms of heartbeat perception. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012;121:719–24.PubMedCrossRef
122.
Zurück zum Zitat Schaefer M, Egloff B, Gerlach AL, Witthöft M. Improving heartbeat perception in patients with medically unexplained symptoms reduces symptom distress. Biol Psychol. 2014;101:69–76.PubMedCrossRef Schaefer M, Egloff B, Gerlach AL, Witthöft M. Improving heartbeat perception in patients with medically unexplained symptoms reduces symptom distress. Biol Psychol. 2014;101:69–76.PubMedCrossRef
124.
Zurück zum Zitat Mauss IB, Levenson RW, McCarter L, Wilhelm FH, Gross JJ. The tie that binds? Coherence among emotion experience, behavior, and physiology. Emotion. 2005;5:175–90.PubMedCrossRef Mauss IB, Levenson RW, McCarter L, Wilhelm FH, Gross JJ. The tie that binds? Coherence among emotion experience, behavior, and physiology. Emotion. 2005;5:175–90.PubMedCrossRef
125.
Zurück zum Zitat Lazarus RS. Emotion and Adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press; 1991. Lazarus RS. Emotion and Adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press; 1991.
126.
Zurück zum Zitat Sze JA, Gyurak A, Yuan JW, Levenson RW. Coherence between emotional experience and physiology: Does body awareness training have an impact? Emotion. 2010;10:803–14.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Sze JA, Gyurak A, Yuan JW, Levenson RW. Coherence between emotional experience and physiology: Does body awareness training have an impact? Emotion. 2010;10:803–14.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
127.
Zurück zum Zitat Ikemi Y, Ikemi A. Psychosomatic medicine: a meeting ground of eastern and western medicine. J Am Soc Psychosom Dent Med. 1983;30:3.PubMed Ikemi Y, Ikemi A. Psychosomatic medicine: a meeting ground of eastern and western medicine. J Am Soc Psychosom Dent Med. 1983;30:3.PubMed
128.
Zurück zum Zitat Damasio AR, Everitt BJ, Bishop D. The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1996;351:1413–20.PubMedCrossRef Damasio AR, Everitt BJ, Bishop D. The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1996;351:1413–20.PubMedCrossRef
129.
Zurück zum Zitat Damasio AR. The body-minded brain. In: Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Penguin; 1994. p. 223–44. Damasio AR. The body-minded brain. In: Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Penguin; 1994. p. 223–44.
Metadaten
Titel
Links among emotional awareness, somatic awareness and autonomic homeostatic processing
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2016
Erschienen in
BioPsychoSocial Medicine / Ausgabe 1/2016
Elektronische ISSN: 1751-0759
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13030-016-0059-3

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2016

BioPsychoSocial Medicine 1/2016 Zur Ausgabe