Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Public Health 1/2012

Open Access 01.12.2012 | Study protocol

ORCHIDS: an Observational Randomized Controlled Trial on Childhood Differential Susceptibility

verfasst von: Rabia R Chhangur, Joyce Weeland, Geertjan Overbeek, WalterCHJ Matthys, Bram Orobio de Castro

Erschienen in: BMC Public Health | Ausgabe 1/2012

Abstract

Background

A central tenet in developmental psychopathology is that childhood rearing experiences have a major impact on children’s development. Recently, candidate genes have been identified that may cause children to be differentially susceptible to these experiences (i.e., susceptibility genes). However, our understanding of the differential impact of parenting is limited at best. Specifically, more experimental research is needed. The ORCHIDS study will investigate gene-(gene-)environment interactions to obtain more insight into a) moderating effects of polymorphisms on the link between parenting and child behavior, and b) behavioral mechanisms that underlie these gene-(gene-)environment interactions in an experimental design.

Methods/Design

The ORCHIDS study is a randomized controlled trial, in which the environment will be manipulated with an intervention (i.e., Incredible Years parent training). In a screening, families with children aged 4–8 who show mild to (sub)clinical behavior problems will be targeted through community records via two Dutch regional healthcare organizations. Assessments in both the intervention and control condition will be conducted at baseline (i.e., pretest), after 6 months (i.e., posttest), and after 10 months (i.e., follow-up).

Discussion

This study protocol describes the design of a randomized controlled trial that investigates gene-(gene-)environment interactions in the development of child behavior. Two hypotheses will be tested. First, we expect that children in the intervention condition who carry one or more susceptibility genes will show significantly lower levels of problem behavior and higher levels of prosocial behavior after their parent(s) received the Incredible Years training, compared to children without these genes, or children in the control group. Second, we expect that children carrying one or more susceptibility genes will show a heightened sensitivity to changes in parenting behaviors, and will manifest higher emotional synchronization in dyadic interchanges with their parents. This may lead to either more prosocial behavior or antisocial behavior depending on their parents’ behavior.

Trial registration

Dutch Trial Register (NTR3594)
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1471-2458-12-917) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Rabia R Chhangur, Joyce Weeland contributed equally to this work.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

GO designed this study and reviewed and revised the manuscript. RC and JW reviewed the design and drafted the manuscript. WM and BOC reviewed and revised the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript to be published.
Abkürzungen
5-HTTLPR
Serotonin-transporter gene
DAT1
Dopamine transporter gene
DPICS-R
Dyadic Parent–child Interactive Coding System-Revised
DRD2
Dopamine receptor D2
DRD4
Dopamine receptor D4 gene
COMT
Catechol-o-methyltransferase gene
ECBI
Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory
G × E
Gene by environment interaction
G × G × E
Gene by gene by environment interaction
IY
Incredible Years
MAOA
Monoamine oxidase A gene
MESSY
The Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with Youngsters
RCT
Randomized Controlled Trial.

Background

A central tenet in developmental psychopathology is that childhood rearing experiences have a major impact on children’s development across life [1]. At the same time, we know that not all children are equally susceptible to these experiences [2]. Grounded in a diathesis-stress model, there has been growing attention for research on individuals’ genetic susceptibility to parenting. The diathesis-stress model holds that some children, due to a specific vulnerability, are more likely to be negatively affected by environmental risk, such as with parental harshness, than others [35].
A typical characteristic of these studies is that they only examined environmental adversity and negative child outcomes. It may therefore be that we, for a long time, only studied so-called dandelions; the resilient children that do well even in the face of severe adversity [6]. In doing so, we may have overlooked the orchids; children who will suffer severely if ignored or maltreated, but flourish spectacularly when receiving adequate care. This metaphor forms the basis of an intriguing alternative hypothesis, namely the differential susceptibility hypothesis which holds that some children, due to a specific susceptibility factor, are more likely to be affected by environmental factors, for better and for worse[7, 8].
Preliminary evidence for this differential susceptibility hypothesis has accumulated over the past years. Previous studies demonstrated, for example, that children with the dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) 7-repeat allele showed significantly more externalizing problem behavior when mothers were insensitive, but also showed less problem behavior when mothers were highly sensitive, compared to those without the DRD4 7-repeat allele [9]. Studies have identified several candidate genes underlying children’s differential susceptibility (e.g., monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) gene; dopamine transporter (DAT1) gene, dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) gene, dopamine receptor D2 (DRD2) gene, serotonin-transporter (5-HTTLP-R) gene, and the catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) gene [1015].
However, the tenability of the genetic differential susceptibility hypothesis is still unclear, for several reasons. First, most previous studies only measured the presence or absence of environmental adversity and developmental problems, but not environmental enrichment and children’s competence. The absence of parental maltreatment, however, is not the same as parental warmth or sensitivity [16]. Only environmental conditions and outcomes ranging from dysfunction to competence make it possible to avoid ceiling effects in testing differential susceptibility [7, 17, 18]. Most importantly, most previous studies used correlational designs [19] and therefore alternative explanations for gene-environment (G × E) interactions cannot be ruled out. For example, children with oppositional behavior may be, genetically, more likely to evoke harsh parental discipline and to actively select environments that support their problem behavior.
Trials in which families are randomly distributed across different environmental conditions offer a solution to this problem [20, 21], because they permit a manipulation of the environment that is independent of children’s genetic makeup and developmental histories. To our knowledge four randomized controlled trials on G × E interactions in children’s social emotional development have been conducted so far [17, 2224].
These pioneering studies delivered important new insights, however their impact suffered from limitations as well. First, although the trials measured environmental enrichment the outcome was usually measured as a decrease in adolescent and child problem behavior, overlooking a possible increase in competent behavior. However, as argued above, to adequately examine differential susceptibility measurements of both environment and child behavior should range from dysfunction to competence. Second, the trials did not examine the possible underlying behavioral mechanism through which a G × E interaction may lead to different behavioral outcomes. It may be that carriers of candidate susceptibility genes show heightened behavioral reactivity. For example, children with low levels of dopaminergic functioning, associated with low reward sensitivity [25], may improve more during and after parent management training than those with high levels of dopaminergic functioning associated with high reward sensitivity, because they can benefit more from the individualization of use of rewards and extensive praising by parents. Likewise, children with decreased serotonergic functioning [25], associated with negative affect/mood, may improve strongly during and after parenting training due to the effect of an increase in positive parental emotions on this affect/mood. A highly reactive child will likely show an intense, mirroring emotional response to both negative and positive discipline [27, 28], which, in turn may lead to emotional synchronization in parent–child interactions. This congruency in affect may then lead to the development of either problem or prosocial behavior, depending on either positive or negative interactions with parents. Therefore, research should also investigate genetic expression “outside the skin”: the mechanisms through which genetic variation moderates the impact of environmental influences on individuals’ development. A randomized controlled trial can test hypotheses about underlying behavioral processes by examining whether certain mechanisms change in the experimental condition, mediating the intervention effect [29].

Aim and hypotheses

The ORCHIDS study is a genetically informed randomized controlled trial to examine possible G × E and G × G × E (i.e., polygenetic) interactions in the development of child behavior. The study examines parenting in its full scope, from both harsh and inconsistent to positive, sensitive, and appropriate parenting behavior as well as from children’s problem behavior and difficulties to their skills, competencies, and strengths. The primary aim is to investigate whether enrichment of the environment, based on the Incredible Years (IY) parent training, has more effect on a genetically susceptible subgroup of children, and to investigate why this may be the case. We expect that the parent training will bring about an environmental enrichment, leading to behavior changes in the participating parents. Two hypotheses will be tested. First, we expect differential susceptibility, which means that children in the intervention condition who carry one or more susceptibility genes (i.e., carrying a MAOA low activity (short) allele; DAT 10-repeat allele; DRD4 7-repeat allele; DRD2 A1 allele; 5-HTTLPR short allele; and or a COMT val allele) will show a significantly higher decrease of problem behavior and increase of prosocial behavior after their parent(s) received the parent training, compared to children without such susceptibility genes and children in the control group. In the control group, we expect this same genetic subgroup to show most behavior problems and least prosocial behavior. Second, we expect that emotional synchronization in parent–child interactions will mediate the intervention effect. Specifically, we expect that children who carry one or more susceptibility genes show a higher synchronization to their parents’ affect than children without these susceptibility genes. Therefore, we expect these children to benefit most from the increase in parental positive affect and sensitivity induced by the Incredible Years intervention.

Methods/Design

Design

The ORCHIDS study is a randomized controlled trial with an intervention (i.e., the Incredible Years parent training) and a control condition that tests gene-based differential susceptibility to changes in parenting. Participants will be 480 families, with children aged 4–8 who show mild to (sub)clinical externalizing behavior problems. Of those families, 160 will be randomly assigned to the intervention condition and 320 families to the control condition. After enrollment in the trial and randomization, the baseline assessment (pretest) will be carried out. The Incredible Years (IY) program will be implemented after these baseline assessments. Participants in the control condition will receive no intervention, but are allowed –and, in case needed, are assisted– to seek mental health care and parenting support through regular services. Posttest and follow-up assessments will be conducted after 6 months and after 10 months, respectively. Approval for data collection was obtained from the central committee on research involving human subjects in The Netherlands (METC UMC Utrecht, protocol number 11-320/K).

Recruitment

In a first screening (see Figure 1), roughly 17.000 families will be targeted through community records via two Dutch regional health care organizations (estimated response rate is 52%, see [30]). All families will receive a personalized information letter, including the Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory (i.e., ECBI) to screen for children’s problem behavior [30]. The criterion for inclusion will be a score at or above the 75th percentile. This cut-off is chosen so that at-risk families will be selected, without excluding children and parents with subclinical or even normal-range functioning. Based on a conservative estimation, 889 families (10%) are expected to be eligible for inclusion. Additionally, a second screening will take place to check for exclusion criteria: mental retardation of the parent and/or child (IQ ≤ 70) and not mastering the Dutch language. Based on estimates from similar procedures followed in previous research [31], 480 eligible families are expected to eventually participate in the ORCHIDS study. These families will receive a second invitation letter and will be contacted for trial participation.

Randomization

Participants will be informed of the design of the study and will give consent prior to randomization. Randomization will occur through random selection of a participant number that is linked to either the intervention or control condition.

Sample size calculation

Power analyses are essential to maximize chances to find significant G × E interactions [32]. For this calculation, we used a Fixed Effects ANOVA Power Analysis in PASS11 [33]. Based on a meta-analysis that demonstrated a small effect size (d) of .20 of the IY intervention [34], and assuming a small G x E interaction effect and no main effects of genes [3, 35, 36], a sample size of N = 480 families will be required for investigating our hypotheses in a two-sided test at α = .05 and power (1-β) = .80.

Intervention

The IY training is aimed at improving parenting skills in order to reduce child behavioral problems, such as aggressive behavior, and enhance competent behavior. The IY training includes 14 to 16 weekly two hour sessions. During these sessions parents watch video-vignettes, discuss parenting with each other and practice new techniques in role-plays. Each group will consist of approximately 10 to 12 parents. IY parent training is different from most other parent (management) training programs, in that trainers use a collaborative leading style: They do not instruct, but are part of the group and lead discussions [3, 37, 38]. Many previous randomized trials have shown the program to be effective [39], also in The Netherlands [40]. Hence, IY parent training is an evidence-based parent training.

Data collection

An overview of all measurement occasions is given in Table 1. Both the recruitment, as well as the waves of data collection will be conducted in two separate cohorts in 2012/2013 and 2013/2014. The recruitment (i.e., screening) will take place in September-October 2012 and 2013. The pretests will take place in November-January 2012 and 2013. Families will be asked to fill in questionnaires in the presence of a researcher or research assistant during home visits. Furthermore, parent–child interactions will be videotaped during structured play situations. This procedure will be repeated twice after the pretest, namely at posttest and follow-up. Saliva samples for genotyping will be collected once during pretest.
Table 1
Overview of measurements
Measurement
Screening
Pre test
Post test (6 months after baseline)
Follow up (10 months after baseline)
Demographic characteristics
 
*
  
Child behavior:
    
 External behavior (ECBI)
*
*
*
*
 Prosocial behavior (MESSY)
 
*
*
*
 Temperament: (CBQ)
 
*
*
*
 Strengths and Difficulties (SDQ)
 
*
*
*
Genotyping (DRD4, DRD2, DAT1, COMT, 5-HTTLPR, MAOA)
 
*
  
Parenting behavior:
    
 PPI
 
*
*
*
Parent-child interaction:
    
 DPICS-R
 
*
*
*
Note. ECBI Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory, MESSY The Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with Youngsters, CBQ Children’s Behavior Questionnaire, SDQ Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire, DRD4 dopamine receptor D4 gene, DRD2 dopamine receptor D2, DAT1 dopamine transporter gene, COMT catechol-o-methyltransferase gene = 5-HTTLPR = serotonin-transporter gene, MAOA monoamine oxidase A gene, PPI Parenting Practices Interview, DPICS-R Dyadic Parent-Child Interactive Coding System-Revised.

Outcomes

Primary outcomes are the possible moderating effects of child genotype on the IY intervention effect (i.e., on the decrease in externalizing problems behavior and/or increase in prosocial behavior of the child). The intervention effect will be assessed with the ECBI, the Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with Youngsters (MESSY [41]), and the Dyadic Parent–child Interaction Coding System-Revised (DPICS-R [42, 43]), see measures in Table 1. Secondary outcomes will be the observed (changes in) emotional synchronization in parent–child interactions as possible underlying behavioral mechanism to the G × (G) × E interactions.

Analyses

First, using independent t-tests, we will examine whether randomization was successful, comparing baseline levels of externalizing and prosocial behavior across the intervention and control condition. Possible significant differences at baseline will be used as covariates in analyses [44]. Second, as the longitudinal data of individuals will be nested in families, multilevel latent growth curve analyses in Mplus [45] will be performed. In this analysis, we will test the interaction between the variables ‘group’ (i.e., intervention vs. control) and ‘genotype’ (i.e., susceptible genotype vs. non-susceptible genotype) to examine possible G × E interactions. In addition, we will take into account families’ ethnic background (i.e., Caucasian vs. non-Caucasian) and additional parental support or (mental) health care families received during the study (i.e., additional self sought care vs. no additional self sought care). Ethnic background and additional care will both be used as covariates in the analyses.

Discussion

The ORCHIDS study described in this protocol is a genetically informed randomized controlled trial targeting families with children aged 4–8 who show mild to (sub)clinical behavior problems. The primary aim of ORCHIDS is to assess possible G × (G×)E interactions in the development of child behavior in its full scope – that is, from children’s problem behavior to their competencies. Our large scale randomized controlled trial is one of the first experimental studies of G × E interactions in social development. Experimental manipulation of the environment is crucial in understanding G × E interactions, because it is the only way to prevent confounding gene-environment covariation.
Additional to a single gene approach, we will investigate possible cumulative effects of multiple candidate genes (i.e., G × G × E or polygenetic interactions [46]). Also, we will make a first attempt to obtain more insight into the behavioral mechanism underlying these G × E interactions by examining (changes in) emotional synchronization in observed parent–child interactions.
Better insight into individual differences in, for example, reactivity to positive parenting behavior like praise, associated with dopaminergic and serotonergic functioning, may help improve the tailoring of behavioral parent training. This seems necessary as the mean effect size of these interventions is modest (Cohen’s d = 0.47 [47]). Individualization of use of rewards and praise may help increase the efficiency of these parenting skills. For example, if the emotional significance of the positive message of praise is less well processed, associated with altered dopaminergic functioning, both verbal and nonverbal enthusiasm may be particularly relevant for this specific subgroup of children [26]. Thus, instead of delivering interventions in a standardized way, parenting programs may benefit from an individualized approach based on insights from results of studies like the present one.
Despite the strengths and innovative aspects of ORCHIDS, there are some issues that our study is unable to take into account. Differential susceptibility to parenting may also be caused by environmental influences that alter the effects of genes (i.e., epigenetics), rather than by specific DNA sequences or a certain number of repeats alone [48]. Human development is an active process powered by a continuous interaction between the genome and the environment [49]. DNA methylation (i.e., the biochemical process that involves the addition of a methyl group onto cytosine in the DNA, regulating the operation of the human genome), for example, has been shown to mediate the relation between genotype and developmental outcomes [50, 51].
Once differential susceptibility to the environmental manipulations has been demonstrated, a next step will be to further investigate the behavioral as well as neurobiological underlying mechanisms of genetic differences in sensitivity to change. Interpreting the intervention effect in this study will be like looking at an “omnibus effect” that covers a variety of possible environmental effects or change mechanisms; the environmental change induced by the intervention consists of changes in many different parent behaviors and child responses. Which of these changes are driving the omnibus effect cannot be elucidated in a RCT. In order to create a more complete picture of gene-environment interplay, multiple genetically informed experimental designs should be used additionally to large scale RCT’s such as microtrials: small-scale, randomized experiments using a brief and focused environmental manipulation, designed to suppress specific risk mechanisms or enhance specific protective mechanisms but not to bring about full treatment or prevention effects in outcome [52].

Conclusion

The ORCHIDS study will investigate possible G × (G×)E interactions in the development of both positive and negative child behavior by assessing whether an experimental manipulation of the environment with the Incredible Years intervention is more effective for a particular genetic subgroup of children than for others. With this study we will contribute to a further understanding of moderating effects of specific alleles (i.e., polymorphisms) on the malleability of child behavior, and the behavioral mechanisms that may underlie gene-environment interactions. By doing so we gain more insight into what works for whom and how it works when it comes to interventions targeting child problem behavior.

Trial status

The trial is ongoing, still recruiting participants.

Acknowledgements

We thank the participating Dutch healthcare institutions for inviting their population to participate in this study. This study is funded by a VIDI grant (no. 452-10-005) and additional open access funding from Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research.
Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

GO designed this study and reviewed and revised the manuscript. RC and JW reviewed the design and drafted the manuscript. WM and BOC reviewed and revised the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript to be published.
Anhänge

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Schaffer HR: The early experience assumption: past, present, and future. Int J Behav Dev. 2000, 24: 5-14. 10.1080/016502500383412.CrossRef Schaffer HR: The early experience assumption: past, present, and future. Int J Behav Dev. 2000, 24: 5-14. 10.1080/016502500383412.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Klein Velderman M, Bakermans-Kranenburg M, Juffer F, Van IJzendoorn M: Effects of attachment-based Interventions on maternal sensitivity and infant attachment: Differential susceptibility of highly reactive infants. J Fam Psychol. 2006, 20: 266-274.CrossRef Klein Velderman M, Bakermans-Kranenburg M, Juffer F, Van IJzendoorn M: Effects of attachment-based Interventions on maternal sensitivity and infant attachment: Differential susceptibility of highly reactive infants. J Fam Psychol. 2006, 20: 266-274.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Caspi A, McClay J, Moffitt TE, Mill J, Martin J, Craig IW, Taylor A, Poulton R: Role of genotype in the cycle of violence of maltreated children. Science. 2002, 297: 851-854. 10.1126/science.1072290.CrossRefPubMed Caspi A, McClay J, Moffitt TE, Mill J, Martin J, Craig IW, Taylor A, Poulton R: Role of genotype in the cycle of violence of maltreated children. Science. 2002, 297: 851-854. 10.1126/science.1072290.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Dick DM, Meyers JL, Latendresse SJ, Creemers HE, Lansford JE, Pettit GS, Bates JE, Dodge KA, Budde J, Goate A, Buitelaar JK, Ormel J, Verhulst FC, Huizink AC: CHRM2, parental monitoring, and adolescent externalizing behavior: evidence for gene-environment interaction. Psychol Sci. 2011, 22: 481-489. 10.1177/0956797611403318.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Dick DM, Meyers JL, Latendresse SJ, Creemers HE, Lansford JE, Pettit GS, Bates JE, Dodge KA, Budde J, Goate A, Buitelaar JK, Ormel J, Verhulst FC, Huizink AC: CHRM2, parental monitoring, and adolescent externalizing behavior: evidence for gene-environment interaction. Psychol Sci. 2011, 22: 481-489. 10.1177/0956797611403318.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Kochanska G, Philibert RA, Barry RA: Interplay of genes and early mother-child relationship in the development of self-regulation from toddler to preschool age. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2009, 50: 1331-1338. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02050.x.CrossRef Kochanska G, Philibert RA, Barry RA: Interplay of genes and early mother-child relationship in the development of self-regulation from toddler to preschool age. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2009, 50: 1331-1338. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02050.x.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Boyce WT, Ellis BJ: Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary- developmental theory of the origins and function of stress reactivity. Dev Psychopathol. 2005, 17: 271-301.CrossRefPubMed Boyce WT, Ellis BJ: Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary- developmental theory of the origins and function of stress reactivity. Dev Psychopathol. 2005, 17: 271-301.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Belsky J, Pluess M: Beyond diathesis-stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influence. Psychol Bull. 2009, 135: 885-908.CrossRefPubMed Belsky J, Pluess M: Beyond diathesis-stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influence. Psychol Bull. 2009, 135: 885-908.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH: For better and for worse. Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2007, 16: 300-304. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00525.x.CrossRef Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH: For better and for worse. Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2007, 16: 300-304. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00525.x.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Pijlman FTA, Mesman J, Juffer F: Experimental evidence for differential susceptibility: Dopamine D4 receptor polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates intervention effects on toddler's externalizing behavior in a randomized controlled trial. Dev Psychol. 2008, 44: 293-300.CrossRefPubMed Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Pijlman FTA, Mesman J, Juffer F: Experimental evidence for differential susceptibility: Dopamine D4 receptor polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates intervention effects on toddler's externalizing behavior in a randomized controlled trial. Dev Psychol. 2008, 44: 293-300.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Sturge-Apple ML: Interactions of child maltreatment and serotonin transporter and monoamine oxidase A polymorphisms: depressive symptomatology among adolescents from low socioeconomic status backgrounds. Dev Psychopathol. 2007, 19: 161-1180. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Sturge-Apple ML: Interactions of child maltreatment and serotonin transporter and monoamine oxidase A polymorphisms: depressive symptomatology among adolescents from low socioeconomic status backgrounds. Dev Psychopathol. 2007, 19: 161-1180.
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Kahn RS, Khoury J, Nichols WC, Lanphea BP: Role of dopamine transporter genotype and maternal prenatal smoking in childhood hyperactive–impulsive, inattentive, and oppositional behaviors. J Pediatr. 2003, 143: 104-110. 10.1016/S0022-3476(03)00208-7.CrossRefPubMed Kahn RS, Khoury J, Nichols WC, Lanphea BP: Role of dopamine transporter genotype and maternal prenatal smoking in childhood hyperactive–impulsive, inattentive, and oppositional behaviors. J Pediatr. 2003, 143: 104-110. 10.1016/S0022-3476(03)00208-7.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Karayiorgou M, Sobin C, Blundel ML, Galke BL, Malinova L, Goldberg P, Ott J, Gogos JA: Family-based association studies support a sexually dimorphic effect of COMT and MAOA on genetic susceptibility to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiat. 1998, 45: 1178-1189.CrossRef Karayiorgou M, Sobin C, Blundel ML, Galke BL, Malinova L, Goldberg P, Ott J, Gogos JA: Family-based association studies support a sexually dimorphic effect of COMT and MAOA on genetic susceptibility to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiat. 1998, 45: 1178-1189.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Pluess M, Velders FP, Belsky J, Van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans- Kranenburg MJ, Jaddoe VWV, Hofman A, Arp PP, Verhulst FC, Tiemeier H: Serotonin transporter polymorphism moderates effects of prenatal maternal anxiety on infant negative emotionality. Biol Psychiat. 2011, 69: 520-525. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.10.006.CrossRefPubMed Pluess M, Velders FP, Belsky J, Van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans- Kranenburg MJ, Jaddoe VWV, Hofman A, Arp PP, Verhulst FC, Tiemeier H: Serotonin transporter polymorphism moderates effects of prenatal maternal anxiety on infant negative emotionality. Biol Psychiat. 2011, 69: 520-525. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.10.006.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Mesman J: Dopamine system genes associated with parenting in the context of daily hassles. Genes Brain Behav. 2008, 7: 403-410. 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00362.x.CrossRefPubMed Van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Mesman J: Dopamine system genes associated with parenting in the context of daily hassles. Genes Brain Behav. 2008, 7: 403-410. 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00362.x.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Waldman I: Gene–environment interactions reexamined: does mother's marital stability interact with the dopamine receptor D2 gene in the etiology of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder?. Dev Psychopathol. 2007, 19: 1117-1128.CrossRefPubMed Waldman I: Gene–environment interactions reexamined: does mother's marital stability interact with the dopamine receptor D2 gene in the etiology of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder?. Dev Psychopathol. 2007, 19: 1117-1128.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Pluess M, Belsky J: Children's differential susceptibility to effects of parenting. Family Science. 2010, 1: 14-25. 10.1080/19424620903388554.CrossRef Pluess M, Belsky J: Children's differential susceptibility to effects of parenting. Family Science. 2010, 1: 14-25. 10.1080/19424620903388554.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Mesman J, Alink LRA, Juffer F: Effects of an attachment-based intervention on daily cortisol moderated by DRD4: A randomized control trial on 1-3-year-olds screened for externalizing behavior. Dev Psychopathol. 2008, 20: 805-820.CrossRefPubMed Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Mesman J, Alink LRA, Juffer F: Effects of an attachment-based intervention on daily cortisol moderated by DRD4: A randomized control trial on 1-3-year-olds screened for externalizing behavior. Dev Psychopathol. 2008, 20: 805-820.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor SE, Way BM, Welch WT, Hilmert CJ, Lehman BJ, Eisenberger NI: Early family environment, current adversity, the serotonin transporter promotor polymorphism, and depressive symptomatology. Biol Psychiat. 2006, 60: 671-676. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.019.CrossRefPubMed Taylor SE, Way BM, Welch WT, Hilmert CJ, Lehman BJ, Eisenberger NI: Early family environment, current adversity, the serotonin transporter promotor polymorphism, and depressive symptomatology. Biol Psychiat. 2006, 60: 671-676. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.019.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Riley BP: Commentary on “The case for gene-environment interactions in psychiatry”. Curr Opin Psychiatr. 2008, 21: 324-325. 10.1097/01.yco.0000320757.22733.30.CrossRef Riley BP: Commentary on “The case for gene-environment interactions in psychiatry”. Curr Opin Psychiatr. 2008, 21: 324-325. 10.1097/01.yco.0000320757.22733.30.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Van IJzendoorn MH, Caspers K, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Beach SRH, Philibert R: Methylation matters: interaction between methylation density and serotonin transporter genotype predicts unresolved loss or trauma. Biol Psychiat. 2011, 68: 405-407.CrossRef Van IJzendoorn MH, Caspers K, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Beach SRH, Philibert R: Methylation matters: interaction between methylation density and serotonin transporter genotype predicts unresolved loss or trauma. Biol Psychiat. 2011, 68: 405-407.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Overbeek G, Weeland J, Chhangur R: Research on gene-environment interdependence: honing the tools and examining the angles. Eur J Dev Psychol. 2012, 9: 426-431. 10.1080/17405629.2012.698942.CrossRef Overbeek G, Weeland J, Chhangur R: Research on gene-environment interdependence: honing the tools and examining the angles. Eur J Dev Psychol. 2012, 9: 426-431. 10.1080/17405629.2012.698942.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Brody GH, Beach SRH, Philibert RA, Chen Y, McBride Murry V: Prevention effects moderate the association of 5-HTTLPR and youth risk behavior initiation: gene x environment hypotheses tested via a randomized prevention design. Child Dev. 2009, 80: 645-661. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01288.x.CrossRefPubMed Brody GH, Beach SRH, Philibert RA, Chen Y, McBride Murry V: Prevention effects moderate the association of 5-HTTLPR and youth risk behavior initiation: gene x environment hypotheses tested via a randomized prevention design. Child Dev. 2009, 80: 645-661. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01288.x.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Cicchetti D, Rogosch F, Toth S: The effects of child maltreatment and polymorphisms of the serotonin transporter and dopamine D4 receptor genes on infant attachment and intervention efficacy. Dev Psychopathol. 2011, 23: 357-372. 10.1017/S0954579411000113.CrossRefPubMed Cicchetti D, Rogosch F, Toth S: The effects of child maltreatment and polymorphisms of the serotonin transporter and dopamine D4 receptor genes on infant attachment and intervention efficacy. Dev Psychopathol. 2011, 23: 357-372. 10.1017/S0954579411000113.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Van den Hoofdakker BJ, Nauta MH, Emmelkamp PMG, Dijck-Brouwer DAJ, Van der Veen-Mulders L, Sytema S, Minderaa RB, Hoekstra PJ: Dopamine transporter gene moderates response to behavioral parent training in children with ADHD: a pilot study. Dev Psychol. 2012, 48: 567-574.CrossRefPubMed Van den Hoofdakker BJ, Nauta MH, Emmelkamp PMG, Dijck-Brouwer DAJ, Van der Veen-Mulders L, Sytema S, Minderaa RB, Hoekstra PJ: Dopamine transporter gene moderates response to behavioral parent training in children with ADHD: a pilot study. Dev Psychol. 2012, 48: 567-574.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Matthys W, Vanderschuren LJMJ, Schutter DJLG: The neurobiology of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: altered functioning in three mental domains. Dev Psychopathol. 2012, 10.1017/S0954579412000272. Matthys W, Vanderschuren LJMJ, Schutter DJLG: The neurobiology of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: altered functioning in three mental domains. Dev Psychopathol. 2012, 10.1017/S0954579412000272.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Matthys W, Vanderschuren LJMJ, Schutte DJLG, Lochman JE: Impaired neurocognitive functions affect social learning processes in oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: implications for interventions. Clin Child Fam Psych. 2012, 15: 234-246. 10.1007/s10567-012-0118-7.CrossRef Matthys W, Vanderschuren LJMJ, Schutte DJLG, Lochman JE: Impaired neurocognitive functions affect social learning processes in oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: implications for interventions. Clin Child Fam Psych. 2012, 15: 234-246. 10.1007/s10567-012-0118-7.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Kohn PM: Reactivity and anxiety in the laboratory and beyond. Explorations in temperament: International perspectives on theory and measurement. Edited by: Strelau J, Angleitner A. 1991, Plenum Press, London Kohn PM: Reactivity and anxiety in the laboratory and beyond. Explorations in temperament: International perspectives on theory and measurement. Edited by: Strelau J, Angleitner A. 1991, Plenum Press, London
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Strelau J: Temperament personality activity. 1983, Academic Press, New York Strelau J: Temperament personality activity. 1983, Academic Press, New York
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Reiss D, Leve LD: Genetic expression outside the skin: clues to mechanisms of genotype × environment interaction. Dev Psychopathol. 2007, 191: 005-1027. Reiss D, Leve LD: Genetic expression outside the skin: clues to mechanisms of genotype × environment interaction. Dev Psychopathol. 2007, 191: 005-1027.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Zeijl J, Mesman J, Stolk MN, Alink LRA, Van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Juffer M, Koot HM: Differential susceptibility to discipline: The moderating effect of child temperament on the association between maternal discipline and early childhood externalizing problems. J Fam Am Psychol. 2007, 21: 626-636.CrossRef Van Zeijl J, Mesman J, Stolk MN, Alink LRA, Van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Juffer M, Koot HM: Differential susceptibility to discipline: The moderating effect of child temperament on the association between maternal discipline and early childhood externalizing problems. J Fam Am Psychol. 2007, 21: 626-636.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Eyberg SM, Robinson EA: Conduct problem behavior: standardization of a behavioral rating scale with adolescents. J Clin Child Psychol. 1983, 12: 347-354. Eyberg SM, Robinson EA: Conduct problem behavior: standardization of a behavioral rating scale with adolescents. J Clin Child Psychol. 1983, 12: 347-354.
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Van der Zwaluw CS, Engels CME: Gene–environment interactions and alcohol use and dependence: current status and future challenges. Addiction. 2009, 104: 907-914. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02563.x.CrossRefPubMed Van der Zwaluw CS, Engels CME: Gene–environment interactions and alcohol use and dependence: current status and future challenges. Addiction. 2009, 104: 907-914. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02563.x.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Menting ATA, Orobio de Castro B, Matthys W: The effectiveness of the Incredible Years Parent Training to modify disruptive and prosocial child behavior: a meta-analytic review. Manuscript in preparation Menting ATA, Orobio de Castro B, Matthys W: The effectiveness of the Incredible Years Parent Training to modify disruptive and prosocial child behavior: a meta-analytic review. Manuscript in preparation
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Rutter M, Dunn J, Plomin R, Simonoff E, Pickles A, Maughan B, Ormel J, Meyer J, Eaves L: Integrating nature and nurture: implications of person-environment correlations and interactions for developmental psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. 1997, 9: 335-364.CrossRefPubMed Rutter M, Dunn J, Plomin R, Simonoff E, Pickles A, Maughan B, Ormel J, Meyer J, Eaves L: Integrating nature and nurture: implications of person-environment correlations and interactions for developmental psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. 1997, 9: 335-364.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Rutter M, Moffitt TE, Caspi A: Gene-environment interplay and psychopathology: multiple varieties but real effects. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2006, 47: 226-261. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01557.x.CrossRef Rutter M, Moffitt TE, Caspi A: Gene-environment interplay and psychopathology: multiple varieties but real effects. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2006, 47: 226-261. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01557.x.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Webster-Stratton C, Hammond M: Treating children with early-onset conduct problems: a comparison of child and parent training interventions. J Consult Clin Psych. 1997, 65: 93-109.CrossRef Webster-Stratton C, Hammond M: Treating children with early-onset conduct problems: a comparison of child and parent training interventions. J Consult Clin Psych. 1997, 65: 93-109.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Webster-Stratton C: Advancing videotape parent training: a comparison study. J Consult Clin Psych. 1994, 62: 583-593.CrossRef Webster-Stratton C: Advancing videotape parent training: a comparison study. J Consult Clin Psych. 1994, 62: 583-593.CrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Hartman RR, Stage SA, Webster-Stratton C: A growth curve analysis of parent training outcomes: examining the influence of child risk factors (inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity problems), parental and family risk factors. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2002, 44: 388-398.CrossRef Hartman RR, Stage SA, Webster-Stratton C: A growth curve analysis of parent training outcomes: examining the influence of child risk factors (inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity problems), parental and family risk factors. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2002, 44: 388-398.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Posthumus JA, Raaijmakers MAJ, Maassen GH, Van Engeland H, Matthys W: Sustained effects of incredible years as a preventive intervention in preschool children with conduct problems. J Abnorm Child Psych. 2012, 40: 487-500. 10.1007/s10802-011-9580-9.CrossRef Posthumus JA, Raaijmakers MAJ, Maassen GH, Van Engeland H, Matthys W: Sustained effects of incredible years as a preventive intervention in preschool children with conduct problems. J Abnorm Child Psych. 2012, 40: 487-500. 10.1007/s10802-011-9580-9.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Matson JL, Rotatori AF, Helsel WJ: Development of a rating scale to social skills in children. The Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with Youngsters (MESSY). Behav Res Ther. 1983, 21: 335-340. 10.1016/0005-7967(83)90001-3.CrossRefPubMed Matson JL, Rotatori AF, Helsel WJ: Development of a rating scale to social skills in children. The Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with Youngsters (MESSY). Behav Res Ther. 1983, 21: 335-340. 10.1016/0005-7967(83)90001-3.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Eyberg SM, Nelson MM, Duke M, Boggs SR: Manual for the dyadic parent–child interaction coding system. www.PCIT.org, 3, Eyberg SM, Nelson MM, Duke M, Boggs SR: Manual for the dyadic parent–child interaction coding system. www.​PCIT.​org, 3,
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Robinson EA, Eyberg SM: The dyadic parent–child interaction coding system: standardization and validation. J Consult Clin Psych. 1981, 49: 245-250.CrossRef Robinson EA, Eyberg SM: The dyadic parent–child interaction coding system: standardization and validation. J Consult Clin Psych. 1981, 49: 245-250.CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Tabachnik BG, Fidell LS: Using multivariate statistics. 2007, Pearson International Edition, Boston Tabachnik BG, Fidell LS: Using multivariate statistics. 2007, Pearson International Edition, Boston
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Muthén LK, Muthén BO: Mplus user's guide. 2010, Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, 6 Muthén LK, Muthén BO: Mplus user's guide. 2010, Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, 6
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Belsky J, Beaver KM: Cumulative-genetic plasticity, parenting and adolescent self- regulation. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2011, 52: 619-625. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02327.x.CrossRef Belsky J, Beaver KM: Cumulative-genetic plasticity, parenting and adolescent self- regulation. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2011, 52: 619-625. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02327.x.CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat McCart MR, Priester PE, Davies WH, Azen R: Differential effectiveness of behavioral parent-training and cognitive-behavioral therapy for antisocial youth: a meta-analysis. J Abnorm Child Psych. 2006, 34: 527-543.CrossRef McCart MR, Priester PE, Davies WH, Azen R: Differential effectiveness of behavioral parent-training and cognitive-behavioral therapy for antisocial youth: a meta-analysis. J Abnorm Child Psych. 2006, 34: 527-543.CrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Szyf M, Weaver I, Meaney M: Maternal care, the epigenome and phenotypic differences in behavior. Reprod Toxicol. 2007, 24: 9-19. 10.1016/j.reprotox.2007.05.001.CrossRefPubMed Szyf M, Weaver I, Meaney M: Maternal care, the epigenome and phenotypic differences in behavior. Reprod Toxicol. 2007, 24: 9-19. 10.1016/j.reprotox.2007.05.001.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Meaney MJ: Epigenetics and the biological definition of gene x environment interactions. Child Dev. 2010, 81: 41-79. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01381.x.CrossRefPubMed Meaney MJ: Epigenetics and the biological definition of gene x environment interactions. Child Dev. 2010, 81: 41-79. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01381.x.CrossRefPubMed
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Beach SRH, Brody GH, Todorov AA, Gunter TD, Philibert RA: Methylation at SLC6A4 is linked to family history of child abuse: an examination of the Iowa adoptee sample. Am J Med Genet B. 2010, 153: 710-713. Beach SRH, Brody GH, Todorov AA, Gunter TD, Philibert RA: Methylation at SLC6A4 is linked to family history of child abuse: an examination of the Iowa adoptee sample. Am J Med Genet B. 2010, 153: 710-713.
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Belsky J, Beach S, Brody G, Dodge KA, Greenberg M, Posner M, Scott S: Gene-by-environment experiments: a new approach to finding the missing heritability. Nat Rev Genet. 2011, 12: 881-882.CrossRefPubMed Van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Belsky J, Beach S, Brody G, Dodge KA, Greenberg M, Posner M, Scott S: Gene-by-environment experiments: a new approach to finding the missing heritability. Nat Rev Genet. 2011, 12: 881-882.CrossRefPubMed
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Howe GH, Beach SRH, Brody GH: Microtrial Methods for Translating Gene- Environment Dynamics into Preventive Interventions. Prev Sci. 2010, 11: 343-354. 10.1007/s11121-010-0177-2.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Howe GH, Beach SRH, Brody GH: Microtrial Methods for Translating Gene- Environment Dynamics into Preventive Interventions. Prev Sci. 2010, 11: 343-354. 10.1007/s11121-010-0177-2.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
ORCHIDS: an Observational Randomized Controlled Trial on Childhood Differential Susceptibility
verfasst von
Rabia R Chhangur
Joyce Weeland
Geertjan Overbeek
WalterCHJ Matthys
Bram Orobio de Castro
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2012
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Public Health / Ausgabe 1/2012
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2458
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-12-917

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2012

BMC Public Health 1/2012 Zur Ausgabe