Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation 1/2019

Open Access 01.12.2019 | Research

Psychometric characterization of incidental feedback sources during grasping with a hand prosthesis

Erschienen in: Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation | Ausgabe 1/2019

Abstract

Background

A prosthetic system should ideally reinstate the bidirectional communication between the user’s brain and its end effector by restoring both motor and sensory functions lost after an amputation. However, current commercial prostheses generally do not incorporate somatosensory feedback. Even without explicit feedback, grasping using a prosthesis partly relies on sensory information. Indeed, the prosthesis operation is characterized by visual and sound cues that could be exploited by the user to estimate the prosthesis state. However, the quality of this incidental feedback has not been objectively evaluated.

Methods

In this study, the psychometric properties of the auditory and visual feedback of prosthesis motion were assessed and compared to that of a vibro-tactile interface. Twelve able-bodied subjects passively observed prosthesis closing and grasping an object, and they were asked to discriminate (experiment I) or estimate (experiment II) the closing velocity of the prosthesis using visual (VIS), acoustic (SND), or combined (VIS + SND) feedback. In experiment II, the subjects performed the task also with a vibrotactile stimulus (VIB) delivered using a single tactor. The outcome measures for the discrimination and estimation experiments were just noticeable difference (JND) and median absolute estimation error (MAE), respectively.

Results

The results demonstrated that the incidental sources provided a remarkably good discrimination and estimation of the closing velocity, significantly outperforming the vibrotactile feedback. Using incidental sources, the subjects could discriminate almost the minimum possible increment/decrement in velocity that could be commanded to the prosthesis (median JND < 2% for SND and VIS + SND). Similarly, the median MAE in estimating the prosthesis velocity randomly commanded from the full working range was also low, i.e., approximately 5% in SND and VIS + SND.

Conclusions

Since the closing velocity is proportional to grasping force in state-of-the-art myoelectric prostheses, the results of the present study imply that the incidental feedback, when available, could be usefully exploited for grasping force control. Therefore, the impact of incidental feedback needs to be considered when designing a feedback interface in prosthetics, especially since the quality of estimation using supplemental sources (e.g., vibration) can be worse compared to that of the intrinsic cues.
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
CV
Comparison value
Exp I
Experiment I
Exp II
Experiment II
JND
Just noticeable difference
MAE
Median absolute error
SND
Acoustic feedback
SV
Standard value
VIB
Vibrotactile feedback
VIS + SND
Combined feedback
VIS
Visual feedback

Background

Humans can effortlessly grasp and manipulate objects of very different properties, from heavy and robust to delicate and fragile. This is possible thanks to a sophisticated musculoskeletal structure innervated by a network of sensorimotor nerves, providing advanced motor commands and a comprehensive multimodal feedback (e.g., touch, proprioception, force). The studies in human motor control demonstrate that the somatosensory feedback is indeed instrumental for the planning and execution of grasping [13].
After an amputation of the hand, the motor and sensory functions are lost. The lost motor functions can be restored to a certain degree using myoelectric prostheses. These systems are controlled by recording the electrical activity of the user’s muscles to estimate the motion intention, which is then translated into prosthesis commands [4]. Typically, the wrist and hand flexor and extensor muscles are used to command prosthesis closing and opening proportionally, thereby resulting in an intuitive connection between the user’s brain and the artificial device [5]. However, current prosthetic systems are controlled in open loop, without explicitly providing any somatosensory feedback on the prosthesis state (e.g., hand aperture or grasping force) to the user. In order to truly compensate for the missing biological limb, it is commonly assumed that a prosthetic system needs to establish a bilateral communication to the user’s brain, by restoring both motor and sensory functions [6, 7].
Intuitive methods to provide somatosensory feedback have been a research topic for several decades [8], and substantial progress was made in recent years [912]. However, a commercially available solution capable of improving the prosthesis performance in the activities of daily living is still unavailable [13, 14]. There is a single commercial prosthesis (VINCENTevolution 2, Vincent Systems, DE) equipped with a simple vibratory feedback on the grasping force, but its clinical and functional utility has not been yet demonstrated. From the technical viewpoint, the non-invasive artificial feedback can be implemented using relatively simple solutions. A common approach is to use sensory substitution, where the lost sensory information is transmitted using alternative sensory modalities that are still spared following the amputation [15]. To this aim, the prosthesis is equipped with force and position sensors, and the sensor data are transmitted to the user by stimulating the skin of the residual limb to activate the tactile sense. The stimulation can be delivered using low-intensity electrical pulses [16] or vibration motors [17], and the information is conveyed by modulating the stimulation parameters. For example, vibration intensity and/or frequency can be proportionally associated to hand aperture [18] or grasping force [1821]. In addition, the feedback can be provided using direct mechanical stimulation through skin stretch [22], movement on the skin [23], pressure cuffs/braces [24], or linear pushers [2527]. More recently, several invasive solutions that directly stimulate the peripheral nerves [2833] or somatosensory areas of the brain [34] have been presented.
Despite several methods have been successfully implemented and tested for restoring sensory feedback, the actual benefits of the artificial feedback are still elusive. Some studies showed significant improvements in performance with artificial somatosensory feedback [3537]. However, these studies were conducted while controlling a virtual setup [3537] and/or while blocking the incidental (visual and auditory) feedback sources [18, 31, 36, 38]. Some recent studies showed benefits of feedback in realistic, clinical settings [32, 3943]. However, other experiments in realistic settings failed to show functional improvements in performance [13, 19, 20, 44, 45] or demonstrated some benefits of feedback only in specific conditions [13, 14, 40].
A major confounding issue when studying the effect of artificial feedback is the existence of incidental sensory information that is already present in prosthesis control. The user can observe the prosthesis motion (visual feedback) and hear the motor sound, which together with proprioception has recently been shown to already allow for a rather good control of grasping force [46]. In addition, motor vibrations as well as mechanical interaction with the object can propagate through the socket to be felt by the user. The user can exploit these incidental information sources to close the control loop, even in the absence of an explicit somatosensory feedback. For example, in a typical myoelectric prosthesis, the velocity of closing is related to the resulting grasping force, i.e., the faster the hand closes the stronger the force produced after contact (see Fig. 2 in [47]). Therefore, the information on the closing velocity can be employed by the prosthesis user to improve the control of grasping force, as demonstrated in [48]. Importantly, the closing speed can be estimated through visual observation or by listening to the motor sound.
Although it has been demonstrated that human subjects can indeed use such information for prosthesis control, the psychometric properties of the incidental sources of feedback have not been investigated yet, in contrast to psychometric tests of the artificial feedback methods (electro- and vibrotactile stimulation) that are thoroughly documented in the literature [16, 17].
In the present study, we systematically investigated the quality of the incidental prosthetic feedback both objectively and subjectively. To this aim, the psychometric properties of the incidental auditory and visual feedback produced by prosthesis motion were quantified in a discrimination task (Experiment I) and then compared to that of a simple vibration interface in an estimation task (Experiment II). The task for the subjects was to estimate the velocity of prosthesis closing using incidental feedback. Vibratory feedback was selected for comparison as a common method to provide explicit somatosensory feedback in prosthetics. The results demonstrated that the subjects could interpret the incidental information sources rather well, and even better than the supplemental vibrotactile feedback. These observations provide insights on possible reasons for the inconsistent results related to the benefits of sensory feedback in prosthetics and thereby guide the design of novel, more effective feedback solutions.

Methods

Participants

Twelve able-bodied volunteers (24 ± 3 years old, between 18 and 27 years, 6 women, 6 men) participated in the two experiments. The subjects were informed about the experiments and confirmed that they understood the experimental procedure. They gave informed consent to take part in the study and were reimbursed with 10 Euro per hour of participation. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Universitätsmedizin Göttingen. Ten of the participants were right handed, two left handed. Four had corrected vision. All subjects had taken part in one previous study on prosthetics [46] but were otherwise naïve to prosthesis control.

Experimental setup

The current study was divided into two experiments, which were performed on different days, with Experiment I (Exp I) being at least 2 weeks before Experiment II (Exp II). The setup was similar in both experiments, as shown in Fig. 1a. It comprised 1) a prosthetic hand (Michelangelo prosthetic hand, Otto Bock Healthcare GmbH, Vienna, AT), controlled via Bluetooth, 2) a standard desktop PC with a 22″ computer screen (hidden from the subject’s view) controlling the prosthesis, 3) headphones playing white noise, and 4) a wooden block. In Exp II, the setup was extended by 5) two C2 tactors with a control unit (Engineering Acoustics, US) connected to the PC via a USB port to deliver vibrations, and 6) a laptop with a mouse used by the subject to indicate answers with the right hand (as explained below). The subjects were seated in a chair with the prosthetic hand in a table-top position in front of them at a distance of approx. 0.5 m, allowing a lateral view on the prosthesis (thumb-side, see Fig. 1). When closed using pinch grip, the prosthesis grasped a wooden block, which was fixed to the table. In Exp II, one C2 tactor was placed on the ventral aspect of the forearm of the subject’s left arm. The subject placed the arm comfortably on a sideboard. The second vibrator was hanging freely in the air and was activated at the maximum intensity in each trial with vibration feedback, effectively preventing the subject to use the sound produced by the first vibrator as potential source of feedback. In addition, the subject wore headphones playing white noise.
The prosthesis and the tactors were controlled by a control-loop implemented in Matlab Simulink 2015b (Mathworks, US) using the prosthetic closed-loop test bench [49] that operated in real-time at 100 Hz. In this study, the prosthesis was controlled by a computer, i.e. the subjects did not control the prosthesis themselves, making them passive observers. During a single trial, the prosthesis closed with constant velocity, simulating a routine-grasping protocol [46]. Across trials, the closing velocity was changed according to the psychometric test procedure (explained below). In Exp I, the subjects verbally reported their discrete answers to the experimenter, whereas in Exp II they used the mouse to indicate the answer via a Matlab GUI.

Experiment I: discrimination task

The aim of the first experiment was to assess the just noticeable difference (JND) in the incidental feedback using a staircase procedure. The JND is the minimal change in the amplitude of the feedback variable that can be perceived by the subject [50], therefore determining the effective resolution of the feedback channel. In the context of the present study, the JND corresponds to the minimal change in the prosthesis closing velocity that can be perceived by looking at the prosthesis (visual JND) and/or by listening to the prosthesis sound (auditory JND).
The experiment started with a familiarization phase in which the subjects watched as the prosthesis closed 10 times. The closing velocities changed from 20 to 100% of the maximum velocity (in steps of 20%), which corresponds to absolute average closing speeds between 38 mm/s and 324 mm/s (see Fig. 8 in the Appendix). This sequence was repeated twice. The subjects could see and hear the prosthesis while it closed.
The JND was assessed using a transformed 1 up / 2 down staircase procedure in a two-alternative forced-choice (= 2AFC) task, which targets 70.71% correct performance [50]. Each step of the staircase procedure comprised two trials of prosthesis closing. The prosthesis closed once at a fixed predefined velocity (standard value) and once at a velocity (comparison value) that was changed adaptively across trials based on the participant’s previous answers. In each trial, the standard and comparison value were provided in random order. The prosthesis stayed for 1 s in the opened or closed position, before closing or opening again. The comparison value was always higher than the standard value. The subjects observed the prosthesis using the provided auditory information (SND), visual information (VIS), and visual and auditory information (VIS + SND), depending on the condition, and they were then asked to report in which trial the prosthesis closed faster. If subjects answered correctly two times in a row, the comparison value was reduced by 1%. If they made a mistake, it was increased by 1%. The first five steps of the staircase were used for reinforced learning, where the experimenter revealed the correct response, to make sure that the subjects understood the task. The initial comparison velocity was set to be 15% higher than the standard one, which was a large difference and hence simple to discriminate. After the reinforcement learning, the normal staircase procedure resumed, i.e., the subjects received no more feedback on the correct answer. The staircase procedure was terminated if 13 reversals occurred, with a reversal being the change of the comparison value from increasing to decreasing or vice versa (see Fig. 2, VIS for an example). The JND (staircase outcome) was then calculated as the difference between the mean of the last 10 reversals and the standard value. The procedure was also terminated if the comparison value was only 1% higher (i.e., as low as possible) than the standard value 7 times in a row (see Fig. 2, SND and VIS + SND for an example). In this case, the JND was set to 1%. To assess the JND at both low and high velocities, the staircase was performed two times, at the standard values of 35 and 80%, respectively (order pseudorandomized across subjects), that correspond to absolute average closing speeds of 72 mm/s and 231 mm/s (see Fig. 8 in the Appendix).
The JND was measured in several conditions with different implicit feedback. In SND the vision was blocked by a screen in front of the prosthesis, which made only the closing sound available. In VIS, the subjects could see but not hear the prosthesis since the sound was blocked by noise-cancelling earphones playing white noise. In the final condition (VIS + SND), the subjects could normally hear and see the prosthesis. The order of the feedback conditions was pseudorandomized across subjects. Generally, the subjects were encouraged to focus on every detail of the closing and grasping process that they deemed could be useful to evaluate the presented closing velocity. This included the interaction between the wooden block and the prosthesis, the duration of the closing process, the frequency and intensity of the sound, etc. All these possibilities were explained to the subject beforehand.
The JND was computed for each subject, feedback condition, and standard-value velocity. As Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests showed that the data were not normally distributed, non-parametric tests were used and medians and interquartile ranges are reported in the manuscript. To compare the three feedback conditions, the subjects’ JNDs were averaged across the two standard stimuli and a Friedman test was applied. Upon significance, the conditions were compared pairwise using three Bonferroni-corrected Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. For comparison between low and high velocities separately for each feedback condition, three Bonferroni-corrected Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were performed. The threshold for statistical significance was set to p < 0.05.

Experiment II: estimation task

The aim of the second experiment was to assess how well the subjects could estimate the prosthesis closing velocity using different sources of incidental feedback. In this case, the prosthesis closed at a constant but arbitrary velocity and the subjects were asked to provide an estimate of the closing speed (absolute estimation versus relative discrimination in Exp I). In addition, the subjects’ ability of estimating the intensity/frequency of vibrotactile stimulation was assessed, a method which is commonly used in prosthetics to implement sensory feedback [10].
The experiment comprised four feedback conditions, three with implicit (SND, VIS, and VIS + SND), and one with vibrotactile feedback (VIB). Each condition comprised a familiarization phase, a reinforcement-learning phase, and a testing phase. The only exception was VIS + SND, where only the test phase was implemented to assess the potential fusion of information from the incidental feedback sources.
During the familiarization phase in SND and VIS, the prosthetic hand was closed at five velocities (20, 40, 60, 80, and 100%) first in increasing, then in decreasing, and finally in random order. In the reinforcement-learning phase, the prosthesis closed randomly five times at each of these velocities (25 trials). After each trial, the subjects were asked to estimate the closing speed and the experimenter then informed them about the correct response. In the testing phase (correct answer not provided), the prosthesis closed at 17 velocities (20–100% with 5% steps), five times per velocity in random order. Importantly, the subjects were not informed about the discrete step-sizes (5%) but were told that any velocity between 20 and 100% might occur, in order to eliminate a bias towards the 5% steps. After the prosthetic hand closed, the subjects reported their closing-speed estimate by a mouse click to an analog scale (20–100%) shown at the laptop.
For the vibrotactile condition, the protocol was similar except that the task was to estimate the intensity/frequency of a vibrotactile stimulus (the prosthesis was not used). In each trial, a 2.5 s vibration was delivered using a C2 tactor fixed on the subjects’ left arm. The same levels and number of trials were used as in VIS and SND. The levels were implemented by simultaneously changing the frequency and intensity of stimulation. The dual-parameter modulation was used to facilitate the discrimination. The frequency (1–100%) ranged from 30 Hz to 270 Hz, and the intensity (1–100%) was chosen between 8 and 71% of the maximum intensity. Therefore, this condition was equivalent to using vibration feedback to transmit the prosthesis closing velocity via linear mapping.
The order of the four feedback conditions was randomized between subjects. However, the VIS + SND was always placed after VIS and SND such that the subjects received the same amount of learning in each of the two modalities before the fusion of modalities (VIS + SND) was tested. To investigate to which extent specific features were important when interpreting the feedback in each condition, the subjects were asked to indicate how much they relied on each feature using a visual-analog scale (0–10) in a questionnaire. They indicated how much they had focused on the speed of closing, duration of closing, and mechanical interaction between the prosthesis and the object for VIS, duration, frequency, and loudness of sound for SND, and frequency and intensity of vibration for VIB. In addition, in each condition they were asked to estimate their average absolute deviation from the correct value, to assess how confident they were in their estimation.
For evaluation, the success rate for the reinforcement-learning phase and the estimation errors in the testing phase were calculated for each trial, separately for each subject, level, and feedback condition. Again, non-parametric statistics were used. For the analysis of the reinforcement learning, the subjects’ success rate, averaged across the five tested levels, was compared across the three trained feedback conditions using a Friedman test and, upon significance, three Bonferroni-corrected Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were applied. In the testing phase, the median of the estimated value was computed for each level and feedback condition, reflecting whether and how much the subject over- or underestimated the correct level. For each level (except for the lowest and highest where over- and undershooting were not possible, respectively) and feedback condition, Bonferroni corrected Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were used to assess whether the subjects consistently over- or underestimated the level, by subtracting the estimated from the correct value and testing whether that value differed significantly from zero. Then, by pooling the absolute errors of all respective trials, the median absolute error (MAE) was determined for each subject and feedback condition, independently of the level. To test for differences between feedback conditions, a Friedman test and, upon significance, six Bonferroni-corrected Wilcoxon signed-rank tests on the MAE were used.
For analysis of the subjective results from the questionnaire, for both SND and VIS as well as separately for the acoustic and the visual features in VIS + SND, a Friedman test was applied and, upon significance, three Bonferroni-corrected Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were used for pairwise comparison. For VIB, a single Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used (only two features available). Six Bonferroni-corrected Wilcoxon signed-rank tests compared whether the feature usage differed when using one-feedback modality (SND or VIS) instead of full feedback (VIS + SND), e.g., the subjective reliance on the auditory duration of the prosthesis closing was compared between SND and VIS + SND condition. In addition, a dominance towards visual versus auditory features was explored by comparing the amount of reliance on the visual versus auditory features in VIS + SND, indicating which modality captured more attention when the full implicit feedback was available. To that end, in the VIS + SND condition for each subject the most strongly used auditory and visual features were selected across the three possible features per modality and these two were compared via a Wilcoxon signed-rank test.

Results

Experiment I: discrimination task

A representative example of a staircase trial is shown in Fig. 2. In general, at the high level of the standard stimulus, the subjects were able to discriminate the smallest possible change in prosthesis closing velocity (1%) in SND and VIS + SND conditions. The staircase sequence “saturated” at the difference of 1% in 9 and 10 out of the 12 subjects, respectively. However, this did not happen for any of the subjects in the VIS condition, and it was also uncommon at the low level of the standard stimulus regardless of the feedback condition (i.e., only a single subject per condition succeeded in saturating the staircase). Nevertheless, all feedback conditions resulted in a small JND with a median value of less than 4%. The summary results are shown in Fig. 3. The visual feedback was characterized with slightly higher JNDs. With median JND of 3.7 and 3.5% for the low and high level, respectively, VIS led to low but still significantly higher JNDs compared to SND (1.5 and 1%, respectively; p < 0.001) and VIS + SND (1.5 and 1%, respectively; p = 0.007). The median JND in the latter two conditions was very close or even equal to the smallest possible change in velocity (1%). The JND was slightly but significantly smaller at the high compared to the low standard-stimulus level for SND (p = 0.006) and VIS + SND (p = 0.004), while no significant difference was observed for VIS. Generally, these data show a surprisingly good discrimination no matter which type of implicit feedback was provided to the subjects.

Experiment II: estimation task

During reinforcement learning, the success rates (median [interquartile range]) were 92% [8%] in SND, 88% [82%] in VIS, and 74% [18%] in VIB. Hence, the performance in VIB was worse compared to the two other conditions.
The data from the testing phase of one representative subject are shown in Fig. 4, where the estimated versus correct closing velocities (SND, VIS, VIS + SND) and vibration levels (VIB) are plotted against each other for all feedback conditions and tested levels. The points are scattered around the optimal-performance line (black line), with a slightly worse quality of estimation in VIB.
The overall results are shown in Figs. 5 and 6. The medians of the estimated values followed consistenly the optimal-performance line (in black) across levels (Fig. 5). Statistically significant deviations from the line were not observed at any level for SND and VIS, at only two levels for VIS + SND, and at 5 out of 15 levels for VIB, indicating a general trend to underestimate the levels between 40 and 70% in this condition. Figure 6 shows the boxplots of the median absolute errors (MAE) across subjects (obtained by pooling trials across tested levels) for all feedback conditions. The performance was similar in SND (5.2% MAE) and VIS + SND (5.1% MAE). The estimation error in VIS + SND was significantly smaller than in VIS (6.7%; p = 0.005) and VIB (8.4%; p < 0.001) and the error in SND was significantly smaller than in VIB (p = 0.002). No significant difference in the quality of estimation was found between VIS and VIB.
Figure 7 summarizes the results of the questionnaire in which the subjects reported on the relevance assigned to different features of the feedback sources. Regarding the auditory features, most subjects seemed to focus on closing duration (median 80% for SND and 70% for VIS + SND) and frequency (80% for SND and 80% for VIS + SND) instead of intensity (45% for SND and 35% for VIS + SND). However, this trend was not significant neither in SND nor in VIS + SND likely due to high variability across subjects. Regarding visual features in VIS (Friedman test p < 0.001) and VIS + SND (Friedman test p < 0.001), the subjects were significantly more attentive to the duration (90% for VIS, p < 0.001, and 70% for VIS + SND, p = 0.001) and closing speed (60% for VIS, p = 0.001, and 70% for VIS + SND, p < 0.001) than to the interaction between prosthesis and wooden block (10% for VIS and 0% for VIS + SND), which was hardly exploited at all. No significant differences in used features between full (VIS + SND) and partial (SND or VIS) feedback were observed, except for a higher focus on the closing duration in VIS compared to VIS + SND (shown by a black asterisk in Fig. 7; p = 0.002). In VIS + SND, the subjects concentrated similarly on visual and auditory features, with none of the two modalities being rated significantly higher than the other. For the vibratory task, the subjects clearly focused significantly more on the intensity (median 100%) than on the frequency information (10%; p < 0.001). Concerning the subjective estimation of performance, they reported a subjective MAE of 10% (IQR 6%) for SND, 17% (IQR 8%) for VIS, 9% (IQR 7%) for VIS + SND, and 15% (IQR 12%) for VIB.

Discussion

The present study investigated the psychometric properties of the incidental feedback in prosthetic grasping. The quality of feedback sources inherent to a prosthetic device (vision and sound) was assessed by conducting psychometric tests and it was compared to the quality of feedback provided by a vibration motor, which is a common solution to transmit somatosensory feedback in prosthetics. The experimental results demonstrated that the visual and auditory feedback of the prosthesis motion could be used to estimate the closing speed with high reliability. In addition, the estimation using sound and vision alone or in combination was significantly better compared to the vibration feedback.
With sound and vision combined or only sound alone, the obtained JND was below 2% at both high and low closing speeds. With vision alone, the JND was somewhat higher (below 4%), which is in agreement with who reported that auditory feedback is more useful than visual feedback for temporal discrimination. The present study therefore demonstrates that the inherent resolution of the incidental feedback sources is truly remarkable. Only by relying on the incidental feedback, the subjects were capable of discriminating changes in the prosthesis closing velocity that are close to the minimum increment/decrement in velocity that can be commanded to the prosthesis (1%). In addition to detecting a change in velocity, the subjects could estimate the absolute value of the closing speed with high precision. When estimating a closing speed randomly selected from the full working range, the average error was around 5% for SND and VIS + SND and around 7% for VIS. As explained in Introduction, the estimation of prosthesis closing velocity can be used to control grasping force [48]. The present study shows that the incidental feedback can provide such an estimate rather precisely, and this explains why the incidental sources can be so useful for prosthesis control [46].
The combined visual and auditory feedback resulted in the best performance, as indicated by both lowest MAE and low variability across subjects. This demonstrates that the subjects could fuse the incidental sources of feedback to improve the estimation of the prosthesis state. The studies of human motor control have shown that human subjects can integrate sensory information from multiple sources based on their relative uncertainty [5153].
The present study demonstrated that the inherent prosthesis feedback could provide reliable closing-speed information to the prosthesis user after only a brief training (< 5 min). When the same information was transmitted using only vibration feedback, the quality of estimation was significantly worse (MAE ~ 8%). Importantly, the results for VIB condition might be different if another coding scheme would be used. We have used a linear mapping since this is a simple and therefore a common choice in the literature [36, 54, 55]. In reality, the mapping from the intensity of stimulation to the perceived intensity follows a power function [17, 56]. And indeed, such a trend can be seen in Fig. 5 (VIB), where the subjects underestimated consistently in the middle range of intensities. This effect, although significant, was not substantial, possibly because we have modulated the frequency in addition to the intensity to further facilitate the discrimination. Nevertheless, better estimation (e.g., less undershooting) in the VIB condition might be obtained if a different transfer function would be used; this remains to be tested.
The above results might indicate that adding vibration as a supplemental feedback to communicate the variables that can be already estimated from the intrinsic sources directly (closing velocity) or indirectly (grasping force) could have limited impact on performance of prosthesis control. The intrinsic feedback seems to be sufficient so that there is not much margin for improvement. Of course, this assumes that the subject can indeed observe the prosthesis while closing. Also, the performance of VIB was worse already in the learning phase, which was rather short in the present study. It would be interesting to investigate the trends as well as the final performance in each feedback modality after a prolonged learning.
There was a substantial variability in the subjective reports about the relevance of different features of the feedback sources. Nevertheless, in all the incidental feedback conditions, the subjects focused on the temporal aspects of the feedback stimulus, namely, the stimulus duration. On the contrary, the cues from the mechanical interaction between the prosthesis and the object seem not to be exploited. Finally, although the sound frequency was appreciated by the subjects in the acoustic feedback condition, when using vibration feedback, they focused substantially more on the intensity. This could be due to the frequency range of the C2 tactor, which does not include low frequencies (< 30 Hz), whereas the intensity was modulated between 8 and 71%. Interestingly, when estimating their own performance, the subjects ranked the conditions according to the objectively measured results (i.e., SND and VIS + SND performed better than VIS and VIB). However, they reported a higher subjective uncertainty, i.e., the perceived estimation error was greater than experimentally observed.
The present study is an important initial step towards understanding the interaction between incidental and explicit feedback in closed-loop prosthetics. In another prosthesis, the vision and sound feedback would be different in quality and/or quantity (e.g., sound pitch and loudness), but the main principle would still hold (e.g., the movement speed and therefore sound associated to force).

Limitations

In real life scenarios, the usefulness of incidental (visual and auditory) feedback can decrease in the presence of overlaying and distracting sensory inputs, e.g. in noisy environments or when the visual attention is diverted from the prosthesis. Also, wearing a cosmetic glove over the prosthesis, which is usual in clinical applications, might decrease the loudness and discriminability of the auditory information. In this case, supplemental vibro- or electrotactile feedback might be beneficial since it is transmitted through a different sensory channel (sense of touch).
The experimental task in the present study resembled routine grasping, in which the prosthesis closes at a constant velocity to produce proportional force upon contact. However, modulating the force after the prosthesis has closed around an object is a different task, in which supplemental force feedback, delivered using vibration or other methods, is likely to be useful. In this case, different sources of feedback might be relevant. For example, if the prosthesis holds a compliant object while closing, the subjects could estimate the grasping force from the amount of object deformation.
In the present experiment, the computer sent a command to the prosthesis to close at a constant velocity. In real-life application, the prosthesis will be controlled by a user generating myoelectric commands. The myoelectric signals are known to be variable and this is likely to cause variability in the prosthesis closing speed (even if the user would like to produce a constant velocity). This can challenge the estimation of prosthesis variables from the incidental sources. Nevertheless, recent studies show that incidental feedback can be useful despite this uncertainty [46, 48]. However, it would be useful to measure the psychometric parameters in this context, namely, during realistic proportional control and feedback. In this case, a proprioceptive information from the sense of muscle contraction could be used additionally to vision and audition [46]. Finally, the results reported here were observed in naïve able-bodied subjects; the implications obtained, however, should be even more relevant for experienced prosthesis users, who had enough time and practice to learn to interpret the prosthesis sound and movement.

Conclusion

The present study shows that the implicit feedback coming from a hand prosthesis can be an important source of information when estimating the prosthesis state. The subjects were able to accurately discriminate (experiment I) and estimate (experiment II) the velocity of prosthesis closing using vision and sound, and they could even fuse the two incidental feedback sources to improve the estimation quality. This is a strong indication that the feedback, which is intrinsically available from the prosthesis, can be usefully exploited for the control of prosthesis grasping. This point should be considered when designing supplemental feedback provided through sensory substitution (e.g., electro and vibrotactile stimulation). If supplemental feedback is to be efficient, it needs to be of a higher quality or should transmit information that is not already available implicitly (e.g., EMG feedback [41, 57, 58]).

Acknowledgements

None.
The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Universitätsmedizin Göttingen. The subjects provided written informed consent before participation.
Subjects provided written informed consent for publication before participation.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Anhänge

Appendix

To measure the prosthesis closing speed, the hand was mounted on a vice and fully opened in palmar grasp. A Matlab program was implemented that repeatedly closed and opened the prosthesis through a full range of speeds. The normalized prosthesis commands in the range between 20 and 100%, in steps of 5%, were repeated three times and the time needed for closure from the fully open prosthesis (12 cm aperture according to the manufacturer) to the fully closed prosthesis was measured. Then, the average closing speed was computed by dividing the aperture (12 cm) by the mean closing time per prosthesis command. The profile of obtained closing speeds is depicted in Fig. 8. It can be seen that the two standard speeds used for JND measurements correspond to the two segments of the profile with different slopes.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Johansson RS. How Is Grasping Modified by Somatosensory Input? In: Motor Control: Concepts and Issues; 1991. p. 331–55. Johansson RS. How Is Grasping Modified by Somatosensory Input? In: Motor Control: Concepts and Issues; 1991. p. 331–55.
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Nowak DA, Glasauer S, Hermsdörfer J. How predictive is grip force control in the complete absence of somatosensory feedback? Brain. 2004;127(1):182–92.PubMedCrossRef Nowak DA, Glasauer S, Hermsdörfer J. How predictive is grip force control in the complete absence of somatosensory feedback? Brain. 2004;127(1):182–92.PubMedCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Rothwell JC, Traub MM, Day BL, Obeso JA, Thomas PK, Marsden CD. Manual motor performance in a deafferented man. Brain. 1982;105(3):515–42.PubMedCrossRef Rothwell JC, Traub MM, Day BL, Obeso JA, Thomas PK, Marsden CD. Manual motor performance in a deafferented man. Brain. 1982;105(3):515–42.PubMedCrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Parker P, Englehart K, Hudgins B. Myoelectric signal processing for control of powered limb prostheses. J Electromyogr Kinesiol. 2006;16(6):541–8.PubMedCrossRef Parker P, Englehart K, Hudgins B. Myoelectric signal processing for control of powered limb prostheses. J Electromyogr Kinesiol. 2006;16(6):541–8.PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Vujaklija I, Farina D, Aszmann OC. New developments in prosthetic arm systems. Orthop Res Rev. 2016;8(July):31–9.PubMedPubMedCentral Vujaklija I, Farina D, Aszmann OC. New developments in prosthetic arm systems. Orthop Res Rev. 2016;8(July):31–9.PubMedPubMedCentral
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Scott RN, Parker PA. Myoelectric prostheses: state of the art. J Med Eng Technol. 1988;12(4):143–51.PubMedCrossRef Scott RN, Parker PA. Myoelectric prostheses: state of the art. J Med Eng Technol. 1988;12(4):143–51.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Peerdeman B, Boere D, Witteveen H, Hermens H, Stramigioli S, et al. Myoelectric forearm prostheses: state of the art from a user-centered perspective. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2011;48(6):719–37.PubMedCrossRef Peerdeman B, Boere D, Witteveen H, Hermens H, Stramigioli S, et al. Myoelectric forearm prostheses: state of the art from a user-centered perspective. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2011;48(6):719–37.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Childress DS. Closed-loop control in prosthetic systems: historical perspective. Ann Biomed Eng. 1980;8(4–6):293–303.PubMedCrossRef Childress DS. Closed-loop control in prosthetic systems: historical perspective. Ann Biomed Eng. 1980;8(4–6):293–303.PubMedCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Hebert JS, Olson JL, Morhart MJ, Dawson MR, Marasco PD, Kuiken TA, et al. Novel targeted sensory Reinnervation technique to restore functional hand sensation after Transhumeral amputation. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2014;22(4):765–73.PubMed Hebert JS, Olson JL, Morhart MJ, Dawson MR, Marasco PD, Kuiken TA, et al. Novel targeted sensory Reinnervation technique to restore functional hand sensation after Transhumeral amputation. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2014;22(4):765–73.PubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Schofield JS, Evans KR, Carey JP, Hebert JS. Applications of sensory feedback in motorized upper extremity prosthesis: a review. Expert Rev Med Dev. 2014;13(5):1–13. Schofield JS, Evans KR, Carey JP, Hebert JS. Applications of sensory feedback in motorized upper extremity prosthesis: a review. Expert Rev Med Dev. 2014;13(5):1–13.
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Svensson P, Wijk U, Björkman A, Antfolk C. A review of invasive and non-invasive sensory feedback in upper limb prostheses. Expert Rev Med Dev. 2017;14(6):439–47.CrossRef Svensson P, Wijk U, Björkman A, Antfolk C. A review of invasive and non-invasive sensory feedback in upper limb prostheses. Expert Rev Med Dev. 2017;14(6):439–47.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Stephens-Fripp B, Alici G, Mutlu R. A review of non-invasive sensory feedback methods for transradial prosthetic hands. IEEE Access. 2018;6:6878–99.CrossRef Stephens-Fripp B, Alici G, Mutlu R. A review of non-invasive sensory feedback methods for transradial prosthetic hands. IEEE Access. 2018;6:6878–99.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Chatterjee A, Chaubey P, Martin J, Thakor N. Testing a prosthetic haptic feedback simulator with an interactive force matching task. JPO J Prosthetics Orthot. 2008;20(2):27–34.CrossRef Chatterjee A, Chaubey P, Martin J, Thakor N. Testing a prosthetic haptic feedback simulator with an interactive force matching task. JPO J Prosthetics Orthot. 2008;20(2):27–34.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Brown JD, Paek A, Syed M, O’Malley MK, Shewokis PA, Contreras-Vidal JL, et al. An exploration of grip force regulation with a low-impedance myoelectric prosthesis featuring referred haptic feedback. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2015;12:104.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Brown JD, Paek A, Syed M, O’Malley MK, Shewokis PA, Contreras-Vidal JL, et al. An exploration of grip force regulation with a low-impedance myoelectric prosthesis featuring referred haptic feedback. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2015;12:104.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Bach-y-Rita P, Kercel SW. Sensory substitution and the human-machine interface. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7:541–6.PubMedCrossRef Bach-y-Rita P, Kercel SW. Sensory substitution and the human-machine interface. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7:541–6.PubMedCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Szeto AYJ, Saunders FA. Electrocutaneous Stimulation for Sensory Communication in Rehabilitation Engineering. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 1982;4:300–8.CrossRef Szeto AYJ, Saunders FA. Electrocutaneous Stimulation for Sensory Communication in Rehabilitation Engineering. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 1982;4:300–8.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaczmarek KA, Webster JG, Bach-y-Rita P, Tompkins WJ. Electrotactile and vibrotactile displays for sensory substitution systems. Biomed Eng IEEE Trans. 1991;38(1):1–16.CrossRef Kaczmarek KA, Webster JG, Bach-y-Rita P, Tompkins WJ. Electrotactile and vibrotactile displays for sensory substitution systems. Biomed Eng IEEE Trans. 1991;38(1):1–16.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Witteveen HJB, Droog EA, Rietman JS, Veltink PH. Vibro- and electrotactile user feedback on hand opening for myoelectric forearm prostheses. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2012;59(8):2219–26.PubMedCrossRef Witteveen HJB, Droog EA, Rietman JS, Veltink PH. Vibro- and electrotactile user feedback on hand opening for myoelectric forearm prostheses. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2012;59(8):2219–26.PubMedCrossRef
19.
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Cipriani C, Zaccone F, Micera S, Carrozza MC. On the shared control of an EMG-controlled prosthetic hand: analysis of user-prosthesis interaction. IEEE Trans Robot. 2008;24(1):170–84.CrossRef Cipriani C, Zaccone F, Micera S, Carrozza MC. On the shared control of an EMG-controlled prosthetic hand: analysis of user-prosthesis interaction. IEEE Trans Robot. 2008;24(1):170–84.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Antfolk C, D’Alonzo M, Rosén B, Lundborg G, Sebelius F, Cipriani C. Sensory feedback in upper limb prosthetics. Expert Rev Med Dev. 2013;10(1):45–54.CrossRef Antfolk C, D’Alonzo M, Rosén B, Lundborg G, Sebelius F, Cipriani C. Sensory feedback in upper limb prosthetics. Expert Rev Med Dev. 2013;10(1):45–54.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Battaglia E, Clark JP, Bianchi M, Catalano MG, Bicchi A, O’Malley MK. The Rice Haptic Rocker: skin stretch haptic feedback with the Pisa/IIT SoftHand. In: 2017 IEEE World Haptics Conference (WHC). Munich: IEEE; 2017. p. 7–12. Battaglia E, Clark JP, Bianchi M, Catalano MG, Bicchi A, O’Malley MK. The Rice Haptic Rocker: skin stretch haptic feedback with the Pisa/IIT SoftHand. In: 2017 IEEE World Haptics Conference (WHC). Munich: IEEE; 2017. p. 7–12.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Rossi M, Bianchi M, Battaglia E, Catalano MG, Bicchi A. HapPro: a wearable haptic device for proprioceptive feedback. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2019;66(1):138–49.PubMedCrossRef Rossi M, Bianchi M, Battaglia E, Catalano MG, Bicchi A. HapPro: a wearable haptic device for proprioceptive feedback. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2019;66(1):138–49.PubMedCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Patterson PE, Katz JA. Design and evaluation of a sensory feedback system that provides grasping pressure in a myoelectric hand. J Rehabil Res Dev. 1992;29(1):1–8.PubMedCrossRef Patterson PE, Katz JA. Design and evaluation of a sensory feedback system that provides grasping pressure in a myoelectric hand. J Rehabil Res Dev. 1992;29(1):1–8.PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Meek SG, Jacobsen SC, Goulding PP. Extended physiologic taction: design and evaluation of a proportional force feedback system. J Rehabil Res Dev. 1989;26(3):53–62.PubMed Meek SG, Jacobsen SC, Goulding PP. Extended physiologic taction: design and evaluation of a proportional force feedback system. J Rehabil Res Dev. 1989;26(3):53–62.PubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Antfolk C, Balkenius C, Lundborg G, Rosén B, Sebelius F. A tactile display system for hand prostheses to discriminate pressure and individual finger localization. J Med Biol Eng. 2010;30(6):355–60.CrossRef Antfolk C, Balkenius C, Lundborg G, Rosén B, Sebelius F. A tactile display system for hand prostheses to discriminate pressure and individual finger localization. J Med Biol Eng. 2010;30(6):355–60.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Schoepp KR, Dawson MR, Schofield JS, Carey JP, Hebert JS. Design and integration of an inexpensive wearable mechanotactile feedback system for myoelectric prostheses. IEEE J Transl Eng Heal Med. 2018;6:1–11.CrossRef Schoepp KR, Dawson MR, Schofield JS, Carey JP, Hebert JS. Design and integration of an inexpensive wearable mechanotactile feedback system for myoelectric prostheses. IEEE J Transl Eng Heal Med. 2018;6:1–11.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Raspopovic S, Capogrosso M, Petrini FM, Bonizzato M, Rigosa J, Di Pino G, et al. Restoring Natural Sensory Feedback in Real-Time Bidirectional Hand Prostheses. Sci Transl Med. 2014;6(222):222ra19.PubMedCrossRef Raspopovic S, Capogrosso M, Petrini FM, Bonizzato M, Rigosa J, Di Pino G, et al. Restoring Natural Sensory Feedback in Real-Time Bidirectional Hand Prostheses. Sci Transl Med. 2014;6(222):222ra19.PubMedCrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Tan DW, Schiefer MA, Keith MW, Anderson JR, Tyler J, Tyler DJ. A neural interface provides long-term stable natural touch perception. Sci Transl Med. 2014;6(257):257ra138.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Tan DW, Schiefer MA, Keith MW, Anderson JR, Tyler J, Tyler DJ. A neural interface provides long-term stable natural touch perception. Sci Transl Med. 2014;6(257):257ra138.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Ortiz-Catalan M, Håkansson B, Brånemark R. An osseointegrated human-machine gateway for long-term sensory feedback and motor control of artificial limbs. Sci Transl Med. 2014;6(257):257re6.PubMedCrossRef Ortiz-Catalan M, Håkansson B, Brånemark R. An osseointegrated human-machine gateway for long-term sensory feedback and motor control of artificial limbs. Sci Transl Med. 2014;6(257):257re6.PubMedCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Schiefer MA, Graczyk EL, Sidik SM, Tan DW, Tyler DJ. Artificial tactile and proprioceptive feedback improves performance and confidence on object identification tasks. PLoS One. 2018;13(12):e0207659.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Schiefer MA, Graczyk EL, Sidik SM, Tan DW, Tyler DJ. Artificial tactile and proprioceptive feedback improves performance and confidence on object identification tasks. PLoS One. 2018;13(12):e0207659.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Clemente F, Valle G, Controzzi M, Strauss I, Iberite F, Stieglitz T, et al. Intraneural sensory feedback restores grip force control and motor coordination while using a prosthetic hand. J Neural Eng. 2019;16(2):026034.PubMedCrossRef Clemente F, Valle G, Controzzi M, Strauss I, Iberite F, Stieglitz T, et al. Intraneural sensory feedback restores grip force control and motor coordination while using a prosthetic hand. J Neural Eng. 2019;16(2):026034.PubMedCrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Page DM, George JA, Kluger DT, Duncan C, Wendelken S, Davis T, et al. Motor control and sensory feedback enhance prosthesis embodiment and reduce phantom pain after long-term hand amputation. Front Hum Neurosci. 2018;12:1-16. Page DM, George JA, Kluger DT, Duncan C, Wendelken S, Davis T, et al. Motor control and sensory feedback enhance prosthesis embodiment and reduce phantom pain after long-term hand amputation. Front Hum Neurosci. 2018;12:1-16.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Tabot GA, Dammann JF, Berg JA, Tenore FV, Boback JL, Vogelstein RJ, et al. Restoring the sense of touch with a prosthetic hand through a brain interface. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2013;110(45):18279–84.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral Tabot GA, Dammann JF, Berg JA, Tenore FV, Boback JL, Vogelstein RJ, et al. Restoring the sense of touch with a prosthetic hand through a brain interface. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2013;110(45):18279–84.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Stepp CE, An Q, Matsuoka Y. Repeated training with augmentative vibrotactile feedback increases object manipulation performance. PLoS One. 2012;7(2):e32743.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Stepp CE, An Q, Matsuoka Y. Repeated training with augmentative vibrotactile feedback increases object manipulation performance. PLoS One. 2012;7(2):e32743.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Witteveen HJB, Rietman HS, Veltink PH. Vibrotactile grasping force and hand aperture feedback for myoelectric forearm prosthesis users. Prosthetics Orthot Int. 2015;39(3):204–12.CrossRef Witteveen HJB, Rietman HS, Veltink PH. Vibrotactile grasping force and hand aperture feedback for myoelectric forearm prosthesis users. Prosthetics Orthot Int. 2015;39(3):204–12.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Walker J, Blank A, Shewokis P, O’Malley M. Tactile Feedback of Object Slip Facilitates Virtual Object Manipulation. IEEE Trans Haptics. 2015;VV(c):1. Walker J, Blank A, Shewokis P, O’Malley M. Tactile Feedback of Object Slip Facilitates Virtual Object Manipulation. IEEE Trans Haptics. 2015;VV(c):1.
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Valle G, Petrini FM, Strauss I, Iberite F, D’Anna E, Granata G, et al. Comparison of linear frequency and amplitude modulation for intraneural sensory feedback in bidirectional hand prostheses. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):16666.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Valle G, Petrini FM, Strauss I, Iberite F, D’Anna E, Granata G, et al. Comparison of linear frequency and amplitude modulation for intraneural sensory feedback in bidirectional hand prostheses. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):16666.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Clemente F, D’Alonzo M, Controzzi M, Edin BB, Cipriani C. Non-invasive, temporally discrete feedback of object contact and release improves grasp control of closed-loop myoelectric transradial prostheses. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2016;24(12):1314–22.PubMedCrossRef Clemente F, D’Alonzo M, Controzzi M, Edin BB, Cipriani C. Non-invasive, temporally discrete feedback of object contact and release improves grasp control of closed-loop myoelectric transradial prostheses. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2016;24(12):1314–22.PubMedCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Markovic M, Schweisfurth MA, Engels LF, Bentz T, Wüstefeld D, Farina D, et al. The clinical relevance of advanced artificial feedback in the control of a multi-functional myoelectric prosthesis. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2018;15(1):28.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Markovic M, Schweisfurth MA, Engels LF, Bentz T, Wüstefeld D, Farina D, et al. The clinical relevance of advanced artificial feedback in the control of a multi-functional myoelectric prosthesis. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2018;15(1):28.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Shehata AW, Engels LF, Controzzi M, Cipriani C, Scheme EJ, Sensinger JW. Improving internal model strength and performance of prosthetic hands using augmented feedback. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2018;15(1):70.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Shehata AW, Engels LF, Controzzi M, Cipriani C, Scheme EJ, Sensinger JW. Improving internal model strength and performance of prosthetic hands using augmented feedback. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2018;15(1):70.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Valle G, Mazzoni A, Iberite F, D’Anna E, Strauss I, Granata G, et al. Biomimetic Intraneural Sensory Feedback Enhances Sensation Naturalness, Tactile Sensitivity, and Manual Dexterity in a Bidirectional Prosthesis. Neuron. 2018;100(1):37–45.e7.PubMedCrossRef Valle G, Mazzoni A, Iberite F, D’Anna E, Strauss I, Granata G, et al. Biomimetic Intraneural Sensory Feedback Enhances Sensation Naturalness, Tactile Sensitivity, and Manual Dexterity in a Bidirectional Prosthesis. Neuron. 2018;100(1):37–45.e7.PubMedCrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Aboseria M, Clemente F, Engels LF, Cipriani C. Discrete Vibro-tactile feedback prevents object slippage in hand prostheses more intuitively than other modalities. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2018;26(8):1577–84.PubMedCrossRef Aboseria M, Clemente F, Engels LF, Cipriani C. Discrete Vibro-tactile feedback prevents object slippage in hand prostheses more intuitively than other modalities. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2018;26(8):1577–84.PubMedCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Schiefer M, Tan D, Sidek SM, Tyler DJ. Sensory feedback by peripheral nerve stimulation improves task performance in individuals with upper limb loss using a myoelectric prosthesis. J Neural Eng. 2015;13(1):16001.CrossRef Schiefer M, Tan D, Sidek SM, Tyler DJ. Sensory feedback by peripheral nerve stimulation improves task performance in individuals with upper limb loss using a myoelectric prosthesis. J Neural Eng. 2015;13(1):16001.CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Raveh E, Friedman J, Portnoy S. Visuomotor behaviors and performance in a dual-task paradigm with and without vibrotactile feedback when using a myoelectric controlled hand. Assist Technol. 2018;30(5):274–80.PubMedCrossRef Raveh E, Friedman J, Portnoy S. Visuomotor behaviors and performance in a dual-task paradigm with and without vibrotactile feedback when using a myoelectric controlled hand. Assist Technol. 2018;30(5):274–80.PubMedCrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Markovic M, Schweisfurth MA, Engels LF, Farina D, Dosen S. Myocontrol is closed-loop control: incidental feedback is sufficient for scaling the prosthesis force in routine grasping. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2018;15(1):81.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Markovic M, Schweisfurth MA, Engels LF, Farina D, Dosen S. Myocontrol is closed-loop control: incidental feedback is sufficient for scaling the prosthesis force in routine grasping. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2018;15(1):81.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Schweisfurth MA, Markovic M, Dosen S, Teich F, Graimann B, Farina D. Electrotactile EMG feedback improves the control of prosthesis grasping force. J Neural Eng. 2016 Oct;13(5):056010.PubMedCrossRef Schweisfurth MA, Markovic M, Dosen S, Teich F, Graimann B, Farina D. Electrotactile EMG feedback improves the control of prosthesis grasping force. J Neural Eng. 2016 Oct;13(5):056010.PubMedCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Ninu A, Dosen S, Muceli S, Rattay F, Dietl H, Farina D. Closed-loop control of grasping with a myoelectric hand prosthesis: which are the relevant feedback variables for force control? IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2014;22(5):1041–52.PubMedCrossRef Ninu A, Dosen S, Muceli S, Rattay F, Dietl H, Farina D. Closed-loop control of grasping with a myoelectric hand prosthesis: which are the relevant feedback variables for force control? IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2014;22(5):1041–52.PubMedCrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Dosen S, Markovic M, Hartmann C, Farina D. Sensory feedback in prosthetics: a standardized test bench for closed-loop control. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2015;23(2):267–76.PubMedCrossRef Dosen S, Markovic M, Hartmann C, Farina D. Sensory feedback in prosthetics: a standardized test bench for closed-loop control. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2015;23(2):267–76.PubMedCrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Kingdom FAA, Prins N. Psychophysics: a practical introduction. San Diego: Academic; 2010. Kingdom FAA, Prins N. Psychophysics: a practical introduction. San Diego: Academic; 2010.
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Berniker M, Kording K. Estimating the sources of motor errors for adaptation and generalization. Nat Neurosci. 2011;11:1454–61.CrossRef Berniker M, Kording K. Estimating the sources of motor errors for adaptation and generalization. Nat Neurosci. 2011;11:1454–61.CrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Wolpert DM, UKPMC Funders Group. Probabilistic models in human sensorimotor control. Hum Mov Sci. 2009;26(4):511–24.CrossRef Wolpert DM, UKPMC Funders Group. Probabilistic models in human sensorimotor control. Hum Mov Sci. 2009;26(4):511–24.CrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Ernst MO, Banks MS. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature. 2002;415(6870):429–33.PubMedCrossRef Ernst MO, Banks MS. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature. 2002;415(6870):429–33.PubMedCrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Pylatiuk C, Kargov A, Schulz S. Design and evaluation of a low-cost force feedback system for myoelectric prosthetic hands. J Prosthet Orthot. 2006;18(2):57–61.CrossRef Pylatiuk C, Kargov A, Schulz S. Design and evaluation of a low-cost force feedback system for myoelectric prosthetic hands. J Prosthet Orthot. 2006;18(2):57–61.CrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Witteveen HJB, Luft F, Rietman JS, Veltink PH. Stiffness feedback for myoelectric forearm prostheses using vibrotactile stimulation. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2014;22(1):53–61.PubMedCrossRef Witteveen HJB, Luft F, Rietman JS, Veltink PH. Stiffness feedback for myoelectric forearm prostheses using vibrotactile stimulation. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2014;22(1):53–61.PubMedCrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Marcus PL, Fuglevand AJ. Perception of electrical and mechanical stimulation of the skin: implications for electrotactile feedback. J Neural Eng. 2009;6(6):066008.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral Marcus PL, Fuglevand AJ. Perception of electrical and mechanical stimulation of the skin: implications for electrotactile feedback. J Neural Eng. 2009;6(6):066008.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Schweisfurth MA, Markovic M, Dosen S, Teich F, Graimann B, Farina D. Electrotactile EMG feedback improves the control of prosthesis grasping force. J Neural Eng. 2016;13(5):1–16.CrossRef Schweisfurth MA, Markovic M, Dosen S, Teich F, Graimann B, Farina D. Electrotactile EMG feedback improves the control of prosthesis grasping force. J Neural Eng. 2016;13(5):1–16.CrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Dosen S, Markovic M, Somer K, Graimann B, Farina D. EMG biofeedback for online predictive control of grasping force in a myoelectric prosthesis. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2015;12(1):55.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Dosen S, Markovic M, Somer K, Graimann B, Farina D. EMG biofeedback for online predictive control of grasping force in a myoelectric prosthesis. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2015;12(1):55.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Psychometric characterization of incidental feedback sources during grasping with a hand prosthesis
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2019
Erschienen in
Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation / Ausgabe 1/2019
Elektronische ISSN: 1743-0003
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-019-0622-9

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2019

Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation 1/2019 Zur Ausgabe

Neu in den Fachgebieten Neurologie und Psychiatrie

Akuter Schwindel: Wann lohnt sich eine MRT?

28.04.2024 Schwindel Nachrichten

Akuter Schwindel stellt oft eine diagnostische Herausforderung dar. Wie nützlich dabei eine MRT ist, hat eine Studie aus Finnland untersucht. Immerhin einer von sechs Patienten wurde mit akutem ischämischem Schlaganfall diagnostiziert.

Niedriger diastolischer Blutdruck erhöht Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Komplikationen

25.04.2024 Hypotonie Nachrichten

Wenn unter einer medikamentösen Hochdrucktherapie der diastolische Blutdruck in den Keller geht, steigt das Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Ereignisse: Darauf deutet eine Sekundäranalyse der SPRINT-Studie hin.

Frühe Alzheimertherapie lohnt sich

25.04.2024 AAN-Jahrestagung 2024 Nachrichten

Ist die Tau-Last noch gering, scheint der Vorteil von Lecanemab besonders groß zu sein. Und beginnen Erkrankte verzögert mit der Behandlung, erreichen sie nicht mehr die kognitive Leistung wie bei einem früheren Start. Darauf deuten neue Analysen der Phase-3-Studie Clarity AD.

Viel Bewegung in der Parkinsonforschung

25.04.2024 Parkinson-Krankheit Nachrichten

Neue arznei- und zellbasierte Ansätze, Frühdiagnose mit Bewegungssensoren, Rückenmarkstimulation gegen Gehblockaden – in der Parkinsonforschung tut sich einiges. Auf dem Deutschen Parkinsonkongress ging es auch viel um technische Innovationen.