Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BioPsychoSocial Medicine 1/2019

Open Access 01.12.2019 | Review

Special series on “effects of board games on health education and promotion” board games as a promising tool for health promotion: a review of recent literature

verfasst von: Mutsuhiro Nakao

Erschienen in: BioPsychoSocial Medicine | Ausgabe 1/2019

Abstract

Board games are played by moving game pieces in particular ways on special boards marked with patterns. To clarify the possible roles of board game use in psychosomatic medicine, the present review evaluated studies that investigated the effects of this activity on health education and treatment. A literature search conducted between January 2012 and August 2018 identified 83 relevant articles; 56 (67%) targeted education or training for health-related problems, six (7%) examined basic brain mechanisms, five (6%) evaluated preventative measures for dementia or contributions to healthy aging, and three (4%) assessed social communication or public health policies. The results of several randomized controlled trials indicated that the playing of traditional board games (e.g., chess, Go, and Shogi) helps to improve cognitive impairment and depression, and that the playing of newly developed board games is beneficial for behavioral modifications, such as the promotion of healthy eating, smoking cessation, and safe sex. Although the number of studies that have evaluated board game use in terms of mental health remains limited, many studies have provided interesting findings regarding brain function, cognitive effects, and the modification of health-related lifestyle factors.
Abkürzungen
(f)MRI
(functional) Magnetic resonance imaging
AD
Anno Domini
ADHD
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
AI
Artificial intelligence
BDNF
Brain-derived neurotrophic factor
EEG
Electroencephalographic
HIV
Human immunodeficiency virus
ICT
Information and communication technology
RCT
Randomized controlled trial

Introduction

Board games are played by moving game pieces in particular ways on special boards marked with patterns [1]. For example, one game originated in northern India in the sixth century AD and spread to Eastern as well as Western countries. In the West, it spread to Persia and then to Spain via the Moorish conquest, and then throughout Europe, where it ultimately became “chess.” In the East, this game became “Xiangqi” in China, “Shogi” in Japan, and a variety of similar games in other countries. Other popular board games that use two patterns for the game pieces include “Go” and “Othello,” also known as “Reversi.”
In the field of psychosomatic medicine, board game playing is sometimes regarded as a leisure activity, and engagement in this type of activity has been shown to protect against dementia and cognitive decline in elderly individuals [2]. For example, a 20-year prospective population-based study conducted in southwestern France investigated the relationship between the playing of board games and the risk of subsequent dementia [3]. Of the 3675 participants without dementia in that study, 1176 (32%) reported regular board game playing and 840 (23%) developed dementia during the follow-up period. The risk of dementia was 15% lower in board game players than in non-players, and board game players exhibited lesser declines in Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores and less incident depression than did non-players. Although the mechanisms underlying the reduced risk of dementia in board game players have yet to be elucidated fully, these games require players to be proactive and to anticipate, thinking several steps ahead, during play. These processes may enhance logical thinking and prevent declines in cognitive function. Individuals may also engage in non-verbal communication while playing board games, and players are more likely to have the opportunity to gather and participate in a fun activity with others. These factors could enhance individuals’ social networks, which also protects against cognitive decline. Furthermore, in terms of leisure activities, board game playing may also be a form of stress management [4], as the fight-or-flight response is regulated safely within the sophisticated structures of match-type games. Board game playing could also be a form of art therapy, similar to miniature garden therapy [5], facilitating infinite internal manifestations within a narrow space.
In terms of education, the playing of board games may help children learn to follow rules and stay seated for a certain amount of time, and it may increase children’s concentration levels [6]. For students and trainees, board game use can enhance health education by stimulating players’ interests and motivation. A search of the Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews [7] identified a total of 2079 unique citations related to educational games, such as board games and games based on television shows. Of these citations, 84 were potentially eligible for review based on methodological quality, number of participants, interventions, and outcomes of interest, and two randomized controlled trials (RCTs) were chosen. The first study [8] was based on the television game show “Family Feud” and focused on infection control; the group that was randomized to play the game had significantly higher scores on a knowledge test. The second study [9] compared game-based learning (using “Snakes and Ladders”) with traditional case-based learning of stroke prevention and management information. Although the two study groups did not have significantly different knowledge test scores immediately or 3 months after the intervention, the reported level of enjoyment was higher in the game-based learning group. The findings of an original review of articles published through January 2012 [7] neither confirmed nor refuted the utility of game playing as a teaching strategy for health professionals. Thus, the present study aimed to clarify the possible roles of board game use in psychosomatic medicine through a literature search for articles published after 2012 that focused on the effects of board game playing on health-related issues.

Mind/body changes due to board game use

Using “board game” as a PubMed search term, 83 studies published between January 2012 and August 2018 were identified; 56 (67%) articles targeted education or training for health-related problems, six (7%) examined basic brain mechanisms, five (6%) evaluated preventative measures for dementia or contributions to healthy aging, and three (4%) assessed social communication or public health policies. The major studies that investigated the effects of traditional board game use are shown in Table 1 [1033]; some of the articles listed in the table were identified in the reference sections of the original 56 articles or other databases.
Table 1
Examples of recent studies using traditional board games
Authors (years)
Countries
Study design
Subjects or materials
Outcomes or variables
Impact
Chess:
Fuentes JP et al. (2018) [10]
Spain
Experimental, single case
Expert chess player, male, 33 years old
EEG changes, decreased heart rate variability
Increased cortical arousal by critical flicker fusion threshold, decreased heart rate variability during chess play
Barzegar K & Barzegar S (2017) [11]
Iran
Clinical case
Middle-aged man with panic attack after post-traumatic stress
Clinical course, including subjective physical symptoms
No symptom of nausea, vomiting, or panic attack after cell-phone chess play
Schaigorodsky AL et al. (2016) [12]
Argentina
Database
1.4 million chess games played by humans
Long-range correlations, inter-event time distributions
Cattuto’s model well described long-range memory used in opening chess lines
Chassy P & Gobet F (2015) [13]
UK
Database
667,599 chess games played by experts from 11 civilizations
Conflict avoidance, risk-taking behaviors during open aggression
Buddhist experts used riskiest strategy nearly 35% more vs. Jewish experts
Sheridan H & Reingold EM (2014) [14]
Canada
Experimental
41 chess players (17 experts, 24 novices)
Eye movements in 8 chess problems
Only experts distinguished relevant and irrelevant information during early trial
Moxley JH & Charness N (2013) [15]
USA
Meta-analysis
4 studies of age and skill effects in chess
Age, chess skill, move selection, chess recall
Best-move, recall tasks associated negatively with aging, positively with skill
Leone MJ et al. (2012) [16]
Argentina
Experimental
25 chess games played by 9 subjects
Heart rate variation
Heart rate signals relevant cognitive episodes, e.g., objective choice correctness events
Go:
Barradas-Bautista D et al. (2018) [17]
Mexico
Computer simulation
Ising Hamiltonian model of black, white Go stones fighting
Two-player scenarios, cancer vs. immune system
Go, Ising model provided elements for characterization of cancer invasion, reduction, metastasis
Bae J et al. (2017) [18]
Republic of Korea
Questionnaire survey
63 subjects predicting outcome of AlpaGo vs. Sedol Lee match
Network density, game predictions
Game predictions more accurate in low-density vs. high-density group
Silver D et al. (2016) [19]
UK
AI Go program
Search algorithm of Monte Carlo simulation and networks
Go win rate
AlphaGo had 99.8% win rate against other Go programs, defeated human Go champion
Lin Q et al. (2015) [20]
China
RCT
147 patients with Alzheimer’s disease
Cognitive impairment, depression, anxiety, serum BDNF level
Go ameliorated Alzheimer’s disease symptoms, with BDNF up-regulation
Kim SH et al. (2014) [21]
Republic of Korea
Case-control study
17 children with ADHD, 17 age-, sex-matched controls
Cognitive function, brain EEG changes during Go play–based education
Right theta/beta change in prefrontal cortex during study period greater in ADHD group
Jung WH et al. (2013) [22]
Republic of Korea
Experimental
17 Go experts
Structural, functional MRI during working memory tasks
Experts had increased gray-matter volume, functional connectivity around amygdala
Lee MK et al. (2012) [23]
Republic of Korea
Clinical case
11 patients with reflex epilepsy, including 6 male Go players
MRI, EEG with clinical course
Individualized strategies like game avoidance most effectively prevented seizures
Shogi:
Tanaka K (2018) [24]
Japan
Review
Summary of data from [26, 27, 30]
fMRI changes in game situations
Cingulate cortex essential for intuitive, strategic decision making for any given Shogi board position
Nakao M et al. (2017) [25]
Japan
Protocol, RCT
65 men aged ≥65 years
Cognitive-behavioral attitudes, depression, anxiety, well-being
Depression, anxiety levels lower during 6-week Shogi stress management program
Wan X et al. (2016) [26]
China
Experimental
17 professional, 17 amateur Shogi players, 19 novices
fMRI signals during problem-solving tasks
In professional group, rostral frontal cortex activated only in post-decision period
Wan X et al. (2015) [27]
China
Experimental
17 amateur Shogi players
fMRI signals during quick offense-vs.-defense strategy decisions
Rostral anterior, posterior cingulate cortices encoded defense, attack strategy values
Nakanishi H & Yamaguchi Y (2014) [28]
Japan
Experimental
12 professional, 12 amateur Shogi players, 12 novices
EEG responses in quick understanding of Shogi game patterns
Frontal area responded only to meaningful game positions, in contrast to temporal area
Aoyagi M & Ogawa T (2013) [29]
Japan
Clinical case
Man with Alzheimer’s disease aged 75–79 years
Frequent chewing for aspiration pneumonia prevention
Shogi play encouragement useful for education about frequent, smooth chewing during eating
Wan X et al. (2012) [30]
Japan
Experimental
20 men aged 20–22 years with little Shogi knowledge
fMRI changes during Shogi training period
Activation in caudate nucleus head developed over training course
Others:
Duan X et al. (2014) [31]
China
Experimental
20 expert Chinese-chess players, 20 novices
Functional connectivity networks assessed by fMRI
Increased connectivity between basal ganglia, thalamus, hippocampus and parietal, temporal areas in experts
Panphunpho S et al. (2013) [32]
Thailand
RCT
20 elderly Ska players, 20 elderly controls
Cognitive function (memory, attention, executive function)
16-week Ska group joiners had significantly better cognitive function scores
van den Dries S & Wiering MA (2012) [33]
The Netherlands
Computer algorithms of learning to play Othello
Combination of three structured neural network techniques
Evaluation functions (simple linear networks, multilayered perceptions)
Method outperforms linear networks, fully connected neural networks or evaluation functions evolved with algorithms
Experimental studies investigating brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) or electroencephalographic (EEG) signals in professional board game players [10, 22, 2628, 31] demonstrated that the basal ganglia play an important role in the ability to rapidly determine, or intuit, the best subsequent move in a game situation [24]. Additionally, variations in heart rate and eye movements were examined as physiological parameters during chess play [10, 14, 16]. In case studies and case-control studies, board games were shown to effectively improve symptoms in individuals who experience panic attacks [11], as well as those with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [21] and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [29]. On the other hand, one study revealed possible hazardous effects associated with the playing of “Go” in individuals with seizure disorders [23]. The amounts of real and virtual playing of board games have increased recently and, as a result, the number of published studies assessing the effects of board game use has also increased [12, 13, 17, 19]. The increase in game play is likely due to the prevalence of computer systems in the current age of information and communication technology (ICT) and artificial intelligence (AI).

Recent RCTs evaluating board game use

According to a recent meta-analysis of four studies that investigated chess play [14], age and skill have differential effects on two tasks during game play: selecting the best move for chess positions and recalling chess game positions. The authors found that age was associated negatively, whereas skill was associated positively, with performance in both tasks. Another RCT showed that an intervention using Go improved depression and increased serum levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in patients with AD [20]. Similarly, players’ depression and anxiety levels were shown to decrease significantly during a 6-week stress management intervention that utilized Shogi games [25]. Although these data have been presented only at a scientific conference, they will soon be published in this special series. The authors also reported that several patterns of negative cognitive distortion (e.g., lower levels of activity) significantly improved following completion of the Shogi program compared with those in a wait-list control group. An RCT showed that the playing of “Ska,” a traditional board game in Thailand [32], enhanced cognitive function in terms of memory and attention in elderly subjects.
Of the 83 articles identified in the present PubMed literature search, 12 articles [3445] report on RCTs that assessed non-traditional board games (Table 2). A variety of board games has been developed to aid in the health education of patients, children, and medical trainees; most of these games are focused on behavioral modifications, such as the promotion of healthy eating [34, 38], smoking cessation [43], and safe sex [45]. For example, in a Swiss study [43], 240 current smokers were assigned randomly to a group participating in smoking cessation program employing an educational board game (“Pick-Klop”) and a wait-list control group. Compared with those in the wait-list group, individuals in the board game group were less likely to remain smokers at the end of the program and at the 3-month follow-up assessment. The authors suggested that use of the board game would be an interesting alternative for the education of smokers in the precontemplation stage.
Table 2
Examples of recent RCTs using board games
Authors (years)
Countries
Subjects
Board games
Control setting
Outcomes or variables
Impact
Nederkoorn C et al. (2018) [34]
The Netherlands
66 children aged 3–10 years
Age-appropriate memory-related board game
Play with large bowl filled with colorless, odorless jelly (Jelly group)
Acceptance of a food with a specific texture
Jelly group ate significantly more jelly dessert
Fancourt D et al. (2016) [35]
UK
352 subjects aged > 16 years without surgical training
Board game requiring removal of 3 organs from Cavity Sam (experimental tool)
Operating theater sound, classical music, or rock as background music
Surgical speed, accuracy, and perceived distraction
Rock music impaired men’s performance of complex surgical procedures in board game
Karbownik MS et al. (2016) [36]
Poland
124 medical students
AntimicroGAME to learn bacteriology, antimicrobial drug actions
Lecture-based seminar
Short-term knowledge retention about pharmacology of antimicrobial drugs
Long-term knowledge retention greater in board game participants vs. controls
Sharps M & Robinson E (2016) [37]
UK
143 children aged 6–11 years
Board game with descriptive social norm–based or health message
Board game with animal images
Children’s fruit and vegetable intake
Health and social norm–based messages increased fruit and vegetable intake vs. controls
Viggiano A et al. (2015) [38]
Italy
3110 subjects aged 9–19 years
Kaledo board game to promote nutrition education, improve dietary behavior
No board game during study period
Adolescent food habits and body mass index
Treatment group showed improved nutrition knowledge, healthy diet, food habits, physical activity
Fernandes SC et al. (2014) [39]
Sweden
125 children aged 8–12 years
Educational board game, video, or booklet with surgery and hospitalization information
Entertaining tools with same formats (comparison group), no tool (control group)
Children’s preoperative worries and parental anxiety
Educational group less worried about surgery, hospital procedures vs. other two groups
Laski EV & Siegler RS (2014) [40]
USA
42 kindergartners, mean age 5.8 years
Numerical board game, counting on from current number on board
Same game, standard count-from-1 procedure
Children’s knowledge of numbers in the 0–100 range
Number line estimates, numeral identification, count-on skill improved more in count-on group
Charlier N & De Fraine B (2013) [41]
Belgium
120 students
Board game to obtain first-aid knowledge
Traditional lecture
Students’ Knowledge of first aids
Game condition was preferred, but lecture more effectively increased knowledge
Swiderska N et al. (2013) [42]
UK
67 medical students
Educational board game in neonatology
Normally provided teaching
Students’ test scores in neonatology
Neonatology test scores higher in game vs. control group (p = 0.09)
Khazaal Y et al. (2013) [43]
Switzerland
240 current smokers aged 18–65 years
Pick-Klop game, cards with smoking-related questions, response options
Psychoeducation to stop smoking, wait-list control
Smoking-related attitudes and behaviors
Game group less likely to remain smokers vs. wait-list group
Cho KH et al. (2012) [44]
Republic of Korea
24 stroke patients
Virtual reality training with balance-board game system
Standard rehabilitation program only
Statics balance of chronic stroke patients
Significant improvement in dynamic balance in chronic stroke patients with virtual-reality balance training
Wanyama JN et al. (2012) [45]
Uganda
180 HIV-positive participants
Educational board game to impart health knowledge
Standardized health talk
Uptake of knowledge to HIV and sexually transmitted infections
Educational game improved uptake of HIV, sexually transmitted infection knowledge

Clinical applications of board games

Based on the results of studies investigating traditional and non-traditional board games, it was hypothesized that board game use would prevent cognitive impairment in elderly individuals and illness-prone behaviors in children and adults. Board game playing also seems to be an effective, fun means of delivering medical and safety education to students and trainees. Currently, many people spend large portions of their time playing games online and offline on television monitors, personal computers, tablets, and/or smart-phones. For example, more than half of Japanese elementary and junior-high school students play video games for more than 1 h on weekdays [46]. Thus, video game–based training will become more popular in the future.
On the other hand, a series of meta-analyses [47] found only small or null effect sizes in three models examining correlations between video game skills and cognitive ability, differences in cognitive ability between game players and non-players, and the effects of video game–based training on cognitive ability, respectively. Thus, examination of the clinical effects of real or virtual training using board games may provide more appropriate information for discussion of the advantages and disadvantages of each style of board game for future applications in clinical settings. A recent assessment of cognitive science research on board game playing [48] highlighted six suggestions for future studies: 1) do not forget about chess (i.e., a traditional board game for which large amounts of data have been collected), 2) look beyond action games and chess, 3) use optimal play to understand human play and players, 4) investigate social phenomena, 5) raise the standards for studies investigating game play as treatment, and 6) talk to real experts.

Conclusions

Although the number of studies investigating board game use remains limited, interesting findings have recently been obtained in terms of brain function, cognitive effects, and health-related lifestyle modification. Board games may also be applicable as educational tools for health professionals. Although a systematic review [7] neither confirmed nor refuted the utility of game playing as a teaching strategy for health professionals, these findings were published in 2013 and additional high-quality studies have been reported since then. Thus, it is time to re-evaluate the usefulness of games and gamifications following technological advances made in modern society. Clinical medicine is closely linked to a public health approach, and medical practices should be undertaken within the limited human, time, and financial resources available [49]. In this sense, appropriate health education programs with a board game component would be useful for both preventive and therapeutic intervention for cognitive-behavioral functioning (e.g., ADHD and dementia), psychological conditions (e.g., depression and anxiety disorders), and life-style diseases (e.g., metabolic syndromes and smoking-related diseases).

Acknowledgements

The author (M.N.) appreciates the support of the members of the Japan Shogi Association and officials in Kakogawa City for conceptualizing health promotion models using board games, such as Shogi and other traditional games.

Funding

Not applicable.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.
Not applicable.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The author (M.N.) declares no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Kyppo J. Board games: throughout the history and multidimensional spaces. NJ, U.S: World Scientific Publishing Co Inc; 2018. Kyppo J. Board games: throughout the history and multidimensional spaces. NJ, U.S: World Scientific Publishing Co Inc; 2018.
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Akbaraly TN, Portet F, Fustinoni S, Dartigues JF, Artero S, Rouaud O, Touchon J, Ritchie K, Berr C. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly: results from the three-city study. Neurology. 2009;73:854–61.CrossRef Akbaraly TN, Portet F, Fustinoni S, Dartigues JF, Artero S, Rouaud O, Touchon J, Ritchie K, Berr C. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly: results from the three-city study. Neurology. 2009;73:854–61.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Dartigues JF, Foubert-Samier A, Le Goff M, Viltard M, Amieva H, Orgogozo JM, Barberger-Gateau P, Helmer C. Playing board games, cognitive decline and dementia: a French population-based cohort study. BMJ Open. 2013 Aug 29;3(8):e002998.CrossRef Dartigues JF, Foubert-Samier A, Le Goff M, Viltard M, Amieva H, Orgogozo JM, Barberger-Gateau P, Helmer C. Playing board games, cognitive decline and dementia: a French population-based cohort study. BMJ Open. 2013 Aug 29;3(8):e002998.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Hoffmann A, Christmann CA, Bleser G. Gamification in stress management apps: a critical app review. JMIR Serious Games. 2017 Jun 7;5(2):e13.CrossRef Hoffmann A, Christmann CA, Bleser G. Gamification in stress management apps: a critical app review. JMIR Serious Games. 2017 Jun 7;5(2):e13.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Ueda T. The availability of shogi for art therapy. Jpn Bull Arts Ther. 2002;33:38–45 [in Japanese]. Ueda T. The availability of shogi for art therapy. Jpn Bull Arts Ther. 2002;33:38–45 [in Japanese].
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Riggs AE, Young AG. Developmental changes in children's normative reasoning across learning contexts and collaborative roles. Dev Psychol. 2016;52:1236–46.CrossRef Riggs AE, Young AG. Developmental changes in children's normative reasoning across learning contexts and collaborative roles. Dev Psychol. 2016;52:1236–46.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Akl EA, Kairouz VF, Sackett KM, Erdley WS, Mustafa RA, Fiander M, Gabriel C, Schünemann H. Educational games for health professionals. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2013 Mar 28;3:CD006411. Akl EA, Kairouz VF, Sackett KM, Erdley WS, Mustafa RA, Fiander M, Gabriel C, Schünemann H. Educational games for health professionals. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2013 Mar 28;3:CD006411.
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Burke CT. The influences of teaching strategies and reinforcement techniques on health care workers’ learning and retention [PhD thesis]. Hattiesburg: University of Southern Mississippi; 2001. [UM I Microform 3038675] Burke CT. The influences of teaching strategies and reinforcement techniques on health care workers’ learning and retention [PhD thesis]. Hattiesburg: University of Southern Mississippi; 2001. [UM I Microform 3038675]
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Telner D, Bujas-Bobanovic M, Chan D, Chester B, Marlow B, Meuser J, Rothman A, Harvey B. Game-based versus traditional case-based learning: comparing effectiveness in stroke continuing medical education. Can Fam Physician. 2010;56(9):e345–51.PubMedPubMedCentral Telner D, Bujas-Bobanovic M, Chan D, Chester B, Marlow B, Meuser J, Rothman A, Harvey B. Game-based versus traditional case-based learning: comparing effectiveness in stroke continuing medical education. Can Fam Physician. 2010;56(9):e345–51.PubMedPubMedCentral
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Fuentes JP, Villafaina S, Collado-Mateo D, de la Vega R, Gusi N, Clemente-Suárez VJ. Use of biotechnological devices in the quantification of psychophysiological workload of professional chess players. J Med Syst. 2018;42(3):40.CrossRef Fuentes JP, Villafaina S, Collado-Mateo D, de la Vega R, Gusi N, Clemente-Suárez VJ. Use of biotechnological devices in the quantification of psychophysiological workload of professional chess players. J Med Syst. 2018;42(3):40.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Barzegar K, Barzegar S. Chess therapy: a new approach to curing panic attack. Asian J Psychiatr. 2017;30:118–9.CrossRef Barzegar K, Barzegar S. Chess therapy: a new approach to curing panic attack. Asian J Psychiatr. 2017;30:118–9.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Schaigorodsky AL, Perotti JI, Billoni OV. A study of memory effects in a chess database. PLoS One. 2016;11(12):e0168213.CrossRef Schaigorodsky AL, Perotti JI, Billoni OV. A study of memory effects in a chess database. PLoS One. 2016;11(12):e0168213.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Chassy P, Gobet F. Risk taking in adversarial situations: civilization differences in chess experts. Cognition. 2015 Aug;141:36–40.CrossRef Chassy P, Gobet F. Risk taking in adversarial situations: civilization differences in chess experts. Cognition. 2015 Aug;141:36–40.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Sheridan H, Reingold EM. Expert vs novice differences in the detection of relevant information during a chess game: Evidence from eye movements. Front Psychol. 2014;5:941.CrossRef Sheridan H, Reingold EM. Expert vs novice differences in the detection of relevant information during a chess game: Evidence from eye movements. Front Psychol. 2014;5:941.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Moxley JH, Charness N. Meta-analysis of age and skill effects on recalling chess positions and selecting the best move. Psychon Bull Rev. 2013;20(5):1017–22.CrossRef Moxley JH, Charness N. Meta-analysis of age and skill effects on recalling chess positions and selecting the best move. Psychon Bull Rev. 2013;20(5):1017–22.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Leone MJ, Petroni A, Fernandez Slezak D, Sigman M. The tell-tale heart: heart rate fluctuations index objective and subjective events during a game of chess. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012;6:273.CrossRef Leone MJ, Petroni A, Fernandez Slezak D, Sigman M. The tell-tale heart: heart rate fluctuations index objective and subjective events during a game of chess. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012;6:273.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Barradas-Bautista D, Alvarado-Mentado M, Agostino M, Cocho G. Cancer growth and metastasis as a metaphor of go gaming: an Ising model approach. PLoS One. 2018;13(5):e0195654.CrossRef Barradas-Bautista D, Alvarado-Mentado M, Agostino M, Cocho G. Cancer growth and metastasis as a metaphor of go gaming: an Ising model approach. PLoS One. 2018;13(5):e0195654.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Bae J, Cha YJ, Lee H, Lee B, Baek S, Choi S, Jang D. Social networks and inference about unknown events: a case of the match between Google's AlphaGo and Sedol Lee. PLoS One. 2017;12(2):e0171472.CrossRef Bae J, Cha YJ, Lee H, Lee B, Baek S, Choi S, Jang D. Social networks and inference about unknown events: a case of the match between Google's AlphaGo and Sedol Lee. PLoS One. 2017;12(2):e0171472.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Silver D, Huang A, Maddison CJ, Guez A, Sifre L, van den Driessche G, Schrittwieser J, Antonoglou I, Panneershelvam V, Lanctot M, Dieleman S, Grewe D, Nham J, Kalchbrenner N, Sutskever I, Lillicrap T, Leach M, Kavukcuoglu K, Graepel T, Hassabis D. Mastering the game of go with deep neural networks and tree search. Nature. 2016;529(7587):484–9.CrossRef Silver D, Huang A, Maddison CJ, Guez A, Sifre L, van den Driessche G, Schrittwieser J, Antonoglou I, Panneershelvam V, Lanctot M, Dieleman S, Grewe D, Nham J, Kalchbrenner N, Sutskever I, Lillicrap T, Leach M, Kavukcuoglu K, Graepel T, Hassabis D. Mastering the game of go with deep neural networks and tree search. Nature. 2016;529(7587):484–9.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Lin Q, Cao Y, Gao J. The impacts of a GO-game (Chinese chess) intervention on Alzheimer disease in a northeast Chinese population. Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;7:163.CrossRef Lin Q, Cao Y, Gao J. The impacts of a GO-game (Chinese chess) intervention on Alzheimer disease in a northeast Chinese population. Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;7:163.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim SH, Han DH, Lee YS, Kim BN, Cheong JH, Han SH. Baduk (the game of go) improved cognitive function and brain activity in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Investig. 2014;11(2):143–51.CrossRef Kim SH, Han DH, Lee YS, Kim BN, Cheong JH, Han SH. Baduk (the game of go) improved cognitive function and brain activity in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Investig. 2014;11(2):143–51.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Jung WH, Kim SN, Lee TY, Jang JH, Choi CH, Kang DH, Kwon JS. Exploring the brains of Baduk (go) experts: gray matter morphometry, resting-state functional connectivity, and graph theoretical analysis. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:633.PubMedPubMedCentral Jung WH, Kim SN, Lee TY, Jang JH, Choi CH, Kang DH, Kwon JS. Exploring the brains of Baduk (go) experts: gray matter morphometry, resting-state functional connectivity, and graph theoretical analysis. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:633.PubMedPubMedCentral
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Lee MK, Yoo J, Cho YJ, Lee BI, Heo K. Reflex epilepsy induced by playing go-stop or Baduk games. Seizure. 2012;21(10):770–4.CrossRef Lee MK, Yoo J, Cho YJ, Lee BI, Heo K. Reflex epilepsy induced by playing go-stop or Baduk games. Seizure. 2012;21(10):770–4.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Tanaka K. Brain mechanisms of intuition in shogi experts. Brain Nerve. 2018;70:607–15 [in Japanese].PubMed Tanaka K. Brain mechanisms of intuition in shogi experts. Brain Nerve. 2018;70:607–15 [in Japanese].PubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Nakao M, Furukawa H, Oomine A, Fukumoto T, Ono H, Obara A, Noda S, Kitashima C. Introduction of “Shogi” health promotion project in Kakogawa City. Tokyo: Abstract of the 24th Annual Scientific Conference of the Japanese Society of Behavioral Medicine; 2017. [in Japanese] Nakao M, Furukawa H, Oomine A, Fukumoto T, Ono H, Obara A, Noda S, Kitashima C. Introduction of “Shogi” health promotion project in Kakogawa City. Tokyo: Abstract of the 24th Annual Scientific Conference of the Japanese Society of Behavioral Medicine; 2017. [in Japanese]
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Wan X, Cheng K, Tanaka K. The neural system of post-decision evaluation in rostral frontal cortex during problem-solving tasks. eNeuro. 2016;29:3(4). Wan X, Cheng K, Tanaka K. The neural system of post-decision evaluation in rostral frontal cortex during problem-solving tasks. eNeuro. 2016;29:3(4).
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Wan X, Cheng K, Tanaka K. Neural encoding of opposing strategy values in anterior and posterior cingulate cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2015 May;18(5):752–9.CrossRef Wan X, Cheng K, Tanaka K. Neural encoding of opposing strategy values in anterior and posterior cingulate cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2015 May;18(5):752–9.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Nakatani H, Yamaguchi Y. Quick concurrent responses to global and local cognitive information underlie intuitive understanding in board-game experts. Sci Rep. 2014;4:5894. Nakatani H, Yamaguchi Y. Quick concurrent responses to global and local cognitive information underlie intuitive understanding in board-game experts. Sci Rep. 2014;4:5894.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Aoyagi M, Ogawa T. Effects of denture wearing and chewing in elderly with dementia repeating aspiration pneumonia. J Jpn Psychiatr Nurs Soc. 2013;56:118–9 [Japanese]. Aoyagi M, Ogawa T. Effects of denture wearing and chewing in elderly with dementia repeating aspiration pneumonia. J Jpn Psychiatr Nurs Soc. 2013;56:118–9 [Japanese].
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Wan X, Takano D, Asamizuya T, Suzuki C, Ueno K, Cheng K, Ito T, Tanaka K. Developing intuition: neural correlates of cognitive-skill learning in caudate nucleus. J Neurosci. 2012;32:17492–501.CrossRef Wan X, Takano D, Asamizuya T, Suzuki C, Ueno K, Cheng K, Ito T, Tanaka K. Developing intuition: neural correlates of cognitive-skill learning in caudate nucleus. J Neurosci. 2012;32:17492–501.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Duan X, Long Z, Chen H, Liang D, Qiu L, Huang X, Liu TC, Gong Q. Functional organization of intrinsic connectivity networks in Chinese-chess experts. Brain Res. 2014;1558:33–43.CrossRef Duan X, Long Z, Chen H, Liang D, Qiu L, Huang X, Liu TC, Gong Q. Functional organization of intrinsic connectivity networks in Chinese-chess experts. Brain Res. 2014;1558:33–43.CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Panphunpho S, Thavichachart N, Kritpet T. Positive effects of Ska game practice on cognitive function among older adults. J Med Assoc Thail. 2013;96:358–64. Panphunpho S, Thavichachart N, Kritpet T. Positive effects of Ska game practice on cognitive function among older adults. J Med Assoc Thail. 2013;96:358–64.
33.
Zurück zum Zitat van den Dries S, Wiering MA. Neural-fitted TD-leaf learning for playing Othello with structured neural networks. IEEE Trans Neural Netw Learn Syst. 2012;23:1701–13.CrossRef van den Dries S, Wiering MA. Neural-fitted TD-leaf learning for playing Othello with structured neural networks. IEEE Trans Neural Netw Learn Syst. 2012;23:1701–13.CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Nederkoorn C, Theiβen J, Tummers M, Roefs A. Taste the feeling or feel the tasting: tactile exposure to food texture promotes food acceptance. Appetite. 2018;120:297–301.CrossRef Nederkoorn C, Theiβen J, Tummers M, Roefs A. Taste the feeling or feel the tasting: tactile exposure to food texture promotes food acceptance. Appetite. 2018;120:297–301.CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Fancourt D, Burton TM, Williamon A. The razor's edge: Australian rock music impairs men's performance when pretending to be a surgeon. Med J Aust. 2016;205:515–8.CrossRef Fancourt D, Burton TM, Williamon A. The razor's edge: Australian rock music impairs men's performance when pretending to be a surgeon. Med J Aust. 2016;205:515–8.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Karbownik MS, Wiktorowska-Owczarek A, Kowalczyk E, Kwarta P, Mokros Ł, Pietras T. Board game versus lecture-based seminar in the teaching of pharmacology of antimicrobial drugs--a randomized controlled trial. FEMS Microbiol Lett. 2016;363(7):fnw045. Karbownik MS, Wiktorowska-Owczarek A, Kowalczyk E, Kwarta P, Mokros Ł, Pietras T. Board game versus lecture-based seminar in the teaching of pharmacology of antimicrobial drugs--a randomized controlled trial. FEMS Microbiol Lett. 2016;363(7):fnw045.
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Sharps M, Robinson E. Encouraging children to eat more fruit and vegetables: Health vs descriptive social norm-based messages. Appetite. 2016;100:18–25.CrossRef Sharps M, Robinson E. Encouraging children to eat more fruit and vegetables: Health vs descriptive social norm-based messages. Appetite. 2016;100:18–25.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Viggiano A, Viggiano E, Di Costanzo A, Viggiano A, Andreozzi E, Romano V, Rianna I, Vicidomini C, Gargano G, Incarnato L, Fevola C, Volta P, Tolomeo C, Scianni G, Santangelo C, Battista R, Monda M, Viggiano A, De Luca B, Amaro S. Kaledo, a board game for nutrition education of children and adolescents at school: cluster randomized controlled trial of healthy lifestyle promotion. Eur J Pediatr. 2015;174:217–28.CrossRef Viggiano A, Viggiano E, Di Costanzo A, Viggiano A, Andreozzi E, Romano V, Rianna I, Vicidomini C, Gargano G, Incarnato L, Fevola C, Volta P, Tolomeo C, Scianni G, Santangelo C, Battista R, Monda M, Viggiano A, De Luca B, Amaro S. Kaledo, a board game for nutrition education of children and adolescents at school: cluster randomized controlled trial of healthy lifestyle promotion. Eur J Pediatr. 2015;174:217–28.CrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Fernandes SC, Arriaga P, Esteves F. Providing preoperative information for children undergoing surgery: a randomized study testing different types of educational material to reduce children's preoperative worries. Health Educ Res. 2014;29:1058–76.CrossRef Fernandes SC, Arriaga P, Esteves F. Providing preoperative information for children undergoing surgery: a randomized study testing different types of educational material to reduce children's preoperative worries. Health Educ Res. 2014;29:1058–76.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Laski EV, Siegler RS. Learning from number board games: you learn what you encode. Dev Psychol. 2014;50:853–64.CrossRef Laski EV, Siegler RS. Learning from number board games: you learn what you encode. Dev Psychol. 2014;50:853–64.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Charlier N, De Fraine B. Game-based learning as a vehicle to teach first aid content: a randomized experiment. J Sch Health. 2013;83:493–9.CrossRef Charlier N, De Fraine B. Game-based learning as a vehicle to teach first aid content: a randomized experiment. J Sch Health. 2013;83:493–9.CrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Swiderska N, Thomason E, Hart A, Shaw BN. Randomised controlled trial of the use of an educational board game in neonatology. Med Teach. 2013;35:413–5.CrossRef Swiderska N, Thomason E, Hart A, Shaw BN. Randomised controlled trial of the use of an educational board game in neonatology. Med Teach. 2013;35:413–5.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Khazaal Y, Chatton A, Prezzemolo R, Zebouni F, Edel Y, Jacquet J, Ruggeri O, Burnens E, Monney G, Protti AS, Etter JF, Khan R, Cornuz J, Zullino D. Impact of a board-game approach on current smokers: a randomized controlled trial. Subst Abuse Treat Prev Policy. 2013 Jan 17;8:3.CrossRef Khazaal Y, Chatton A, Prezzemolo R, Zebouni F, Edel Y, Jacquet J, Ruggeri O, Burnens E, Monney G, Protti AS, Etter JF, Khan R, Cornuz J, Zullino D. Impact of a board-game approach on current smokers: a randomized controlled trial. Subst Abuse Treat Prev Policy. 2013 Jan 17;8:3.CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Cho KH, Lee KJ, Song CH. Virtual-reality balance training with a video-game system improves dynamic balance in chronic stroke patients. Tohoku J Exp Med. 2012;228:69–74.CrossRef Cho KH, Lee KJ, Song CH. Virtual-reality balance training with a video-game system improves dynamic balance in chronic stroke patients. Tohoku J Exp Med. 2012;228:69–74.CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Wanyama JN, Castelnuovo B, Robertson G, Newell K, Sempa JB, Kambugu A, Manabe YC, Colebunders R. A randomized controlled trial to evaluate the effectiveness of a board game on patients’ knowledge uptake of HIV and sexually transmitted diseases at the infectious diseases institute, Kampala, Uganda. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2012;59:253–8.CrossRef Wanyama JN, Castelnuovo B, Robertson G, Newell K, Sempa JB, Kambugu A, Manabe YC, Colebunders R. A randomized controlled trial to evaluate the effectiveness of a board game on patients’ knowledge uptake of HIV and sexually transmitted diseases at the infectious diseases institute, Kampala, Uganda. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2012;59:253–8.CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Sala G, Tatlidil KS, Gobet F. Video game training does not enhance cognitive ability: a comprehensive meta-analytic investigation. Psychol Bull. 2018;144:111–39.CrossRef Sala G, Tatlidil KS, Gobet F. Video game training does not enhance cognitive ability: a comprehensive meta-analytic investigation. Psychol Bull. 2018;144:111–39.CrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Chabris CF. Six suggestions for research on games in cognitive science. Topics Cog Sci. 2017;9:497–509.CrossRef Chabris CF. Six suggestions for research on games in cognitive science. Topics Cog Sci. 2017;9:497–509.CrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Nakao M. Bio-psycho-social medicine is a comprehensive form of medicine bridging clinical medicine and public health. BioPsychoSoc Med. 2010;4:19.CrossRef Nakao M. Bio-psycho-social medicine is a comprehensive form of medicine bridging clinical medicine and public health. BioPsychoSoc Med. 2010;4:19.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Special series on “effects of board games on health education and promotion” board games as a promising tool for health promotion: a review of recent literature
verfasst von
Mutsuhiro Nakao
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2019
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BioPsychoSocial Medicine / Ausgabe 1/2019
Elektronische ISSN: 1751-0759
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13030-019-0146-3

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2019

BioPsychoSocial Medicine 1/2019 Zur Ausgabe

Neu in den Fachgebieten Neurologie und Psychiatrie

Niedriger diastolischer Blutdruck erhöht Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Komplikationen

25.04.2024 Hypotonie Nachrichten

Wenn unter einer medikamentösen Hochdrucktherapie der diastolische Blutdruck in den Keller geht, steigt das Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Ereignisse: Darauf deutet eine Sekundäranalyse der SPRINT-Studie hin.

Frühe Alzheimertherapie lohnt sich

25.04.2024 AAN-Jahrestagung 2024 Nachrichten

Ist die Tau-Last noch gering, scheint der Vorteil von Lecanemab besonders groß zu sein. Und beginnen Erkrankte verzögert mit der Behandlung, erreichen sie nicht mehr die kognitive Leistung wie bei einem früheren Start. Darauf deuten neue Analysen der Phase-3-Studie Clarity AD.

Viel Bewegung in der Parkinsonforschung

25.04.2024 Parkinson-Krankheit Nachrichten

Neue arznei- und zellbasierte Ansätze, Frühdiagnose mit Bewegungssensoren, Rückenmarkstimulation gegen Gehblockaden – in der Parkinsonforschung tut sich einiges. Auf dem Deutschen Parkinsonkongress ging es auch viel um technische Innovationen.

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.